Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T13:52:06.174Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Noun Phrase Accessibility Hierarchy: Lame Duck or Dead Duck in Theories of SLA?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 April 2007

Roger Hawkins
Affiliation:
University of Essex

Extract

The use of the noun phrase accessibility hierarchy (NPAH) as an explanation for the use, interpretation, or both of relative clauses (RCs) by second language (L2) speakers is one of the earliest examples of an attempt to offer a principled hypothesis about L2 development. Its explicit claims have allowed empirical testing that has produced important counterexamples, including the results of the studies reported in this special issue, which suggest that the hierarchy is not applicable to the SLA of languages like Cantonese, Japanese, and Korean. A weak version of the hypothesis that applies only to languages in which RCs express head-complement dependencies (i.e., postnominal relatives) is just as problematic. I suggest here that the time has come to consign this hierarchy to the history of SLA research and focus on other kinds of question about L2 speakers' knowledge of RCs.

Type
COMMENTARY
Copyright
© 2007 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Clahsen, H. & Felser, C. (2006). Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 342.Google Scholar
Cole, P., Hermon, G., & Sung, L.-M. (1990). Principles and parameters of long-distance reflexives. Linguistic Inquiry, 21, 121.Google Scholar
Comrie, B. (1998). Attributive clauses in Asian languages: Towards an areal typology. In W. Boeder, C. Schroeder, K. H. Wagner, & W. Wildgen (Eds.), Sprache in Raum und Zeit: In Memoriam Johannes Bechert, Band 2 (pp. 5160). Tübingen: Gunter Narr.
Dulay, H., Burt, M., & Krashen, S. (1982). Language two. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hamilton, R. L. (1995). The noun phrase accessibility hierarchy in SLA: Determining the basis for its developmental effects. In F. R. Eckman, D. Highland, P. W. Lee, J. Mileham, & R. R. Weber (Eds.), Second language acquisition theory and pedagogy (pp. 101114). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Manzini, R. M. & Wexler, K. (1987). Parameters, binding theory, and learnability. Linguistic Inquiry, 18, 413444.Google Scholar
O'Grady, W., Lee, M., & Choo, M. (2003). A subject-object asymmetry in the acquisition of relative clauses in Korean as a second language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 433448.Google Scholar
Pica, P. (1984). Subject, tense and truth: Towards a modular approach to binding. In J. Gueron, H.-G. Obenauer, & J.-Y. Pollock (Eds.), Grammatical representation (pp. 259291). Dordrecht: Foris.
Progovac, L. (1992). Relativized SUBJECT: Long-distance reflexives without movement. Linguistic Inquiry, 23, 671680.Google Scholar
White, L. (2003). Second language acquisition and Universal Grammar. New York: Cambridge University Press.