Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-s2hrs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T15:02:51.433Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

NEW DIRECTIONS IN THE STUDY OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT LEARNING

An Introduction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 May 2015

Sible Andringa*
Affiliation:
University of Amsterdam
Patrick Rebuschat
Affiliation:
Lancaster University
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Sible Andringa, Amsterdam Center for Language and Communication, University of Amsterdam, Spuistraat 134, 1012 VB, Amsterdam, the Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Andringa, S., & Curcic, M. (this issue). How explicit knowledge affects online L2 processing: Evidence from differential object marking acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andringa, S. J., de Glopper, K., & Hacquebord, H. I. (2011). Effect of explicit and implicit instruction on free written response task performance. Language Learning, 61, 868903.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowles, M. A. (2011). Measuring implicit and explicit linguistic knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 247271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. (2008). Usage-based grammar and second language acquisition. In Robinson, P. & Ellis, N. C. (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition (pp. 216236). London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Caldwell-Harris, C. L., Lancaster, A., Ladd, D. R., Dediu, D., & Christiansen, M. H. (this issue). Factors influencing sensitivity to lexical tone in an artificial language: Implications for second language learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, W. W., Guo, X. Y., Tang, J. H., Zhu, L., Yang, Z. L., & Dienes, Z. (2011). Unconscious structural knowledge of form-meaning connections. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 17511760.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cintrón-Valentín, M., & Ellis, N. C. (this issue). Exploring the interface: Explicit focus-on-form instruction and learned attentional biases in L2 Latin. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, C. M., Bauernschmidt, A., Huang, S. S., & Pisoni, D. B. (2010). Implicit statistical learning in language processing: Word predictability is the key. Cognition, 114, 356371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conway, C. M., & Christiansen, M. H. (2006). Statistical learning within and between modalities: Pitting abstract against stimulus-specific representations. Psychological Science, 17, 905912.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Jong, N. (2005). Can second language grammar be learned through listening? An experimental study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 205234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2003). Implicit and explicit learning. In Doughty, C. & Long, M. H. (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 313348). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Doughty, C. (2001). Cognitive underpinnings of focus on form. In Robinson, P. (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 206257). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doughty, C. (2003). Instructed SLA: Constraints, compensation, and enhancement. In Doughty, C. & Long, M. H. (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 256310). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2002). Frequency effects in language processing: A review with implications for theories of implicit and explicit language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 143188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2005). At the interface: Dynamic interactions of explicit and implicit language knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 305352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2008). Usage-based and form-focused language acquisition: The associative learning of constructions, learned-attention, and the limited L2 endstate. In Robinson, P. & Ellis, N. C. (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition (pp. 372405). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (in press). Implicit AND explicit language learning: Their dynamic interface and complexity. In Rebuschat, P. (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C., Hafeez, K., Martin, K. I., Chen, L., Boland, J., & Sagarra, N. (2014). An eye-tracking study of learned attention in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 547579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C., & Larsen-Freeman, D. (2006). Language emergence: Implications for applied linguistics. Applied Linguistics, 27, 558589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C., & O’Donnell, M. B. (2012). Statistical construction learning: Does a zipfian problem space ensure robust language learning? In Rebuschat, P. & Williams, J. N. (Eds.), Statistical learning and language acquisition (pp. 265304). Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Ellis, R. (2002). Does form-focused instruction affect the acquisition of implicit knowledge? A review of the research. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 223236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R. (2004). The definition and measurement of explicit knowledge. Language Learning, 54, 227275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R. (2005). Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: A psychometric study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 141172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Godfroid, A., Loewen, S., Jung, S., Park, J.-H., Gass, S., & Ellis, R. (this issue). Timed and untimed grammaticality judgments measure distinct types of knowledge: Evidence from eye-movement patterns. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Godfroid, A., & Winke, P. (in press). Investigating implicit and explicit processing using L2 learners’ eye-movement data. In Rebuschat, P. (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Grey, S., Williams, J. N., & Rebuschat, P. (in press). Individual differences in incidental language learning: The role of learning styles, personality, and working memory. Learning and Individual Differences.Google Scholar
Grey, S., Williams, J. N., & Rebuschat, P. (2014). Incidental exposure and L3 learning of morphosyntax. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 36, 134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hama, M., & Leow, R. P. (2010). Learning without awareness revisited: Extending Williams (2005). Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32, 465491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamrick, P., & Rebuschat, P. (2012). How implicit is statistical learning? In Rebuschat, P. & Williams, J. N. (Eds.), Statistical learning and language acquisition (pp. 365382). Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Han, Y., & Ellis, R. (1998). Implicit knowledge, explicit knowledge and general proficiency. Language Teaching Research, 2, 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulstijn, J. H. (2005). Theoretical and empirical issues in the study of implicit and explicit second-language learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 129140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulstijn, J. H. (in press). Explaining phenomena of first and second language acquisition with the constructs of implicit and explicit learning: The virtues and pitfalls of a two-system view. In Rebuschat, P. (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hulstijn, J. H., & Ellis, R. (Eds.). (2005). Implicit and explicit second-language learning [Special issue]. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(2).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krashen, S. D. (1977). The Monitor Model for adult second language performance. In Burt, M., Dulay, H., & Finocchiaro, M. (Eds.), Viewpoints on English as a second language: In honor of James E. Alatis (pp. 152161). New York, NY: Regents.Google Scholar
Krashen, S. D. (1979). The Monitor Model for second language acquisition. In Gingras, R. (Ed.), Second language acquisition and foreign language teaching (pp. 126). Arlington, VA: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Krashen, S. D. (1981). Second language acquisition and second language learning. Oxford, UK: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Krashen, S. (1994). The input hypothesis and its rivals. In Ellis, N. C. (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 4577). London, UK: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kuhn, G., & Dienes, Z. (2008). Learning non-local dependencies. Cognition, 106, 184206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leow, R. P. (1997). Attention, awareness, and foreign language behavior. Language Learning, 47, 467505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leow, R. P. (2000). A study of the role of awareness in foreign language behavior: Aware versus unaware learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22, 557584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leow, R. P., Grey, S., Marijuan, S., & Moorman, C. (2014). Concurrent data elicitation procedures, processes, and the early stages of L2 learning: A critical overview. Second Language Research, 30, 111127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leow, R. P., & Hama, M. (2013). Implicit learning in SLA and the issue of internal validity: A response to Leung and Williams (2011). Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35, 545557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leung, J. H. C., & Williams, J. N. (2011). The implicit learning of mappings between forms and contextually-derived meanings. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 3355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leung, J. H. C., & Williams, J. N. (2012). Constraints on implicit learning of grammatical form-meaning connections. Language Learning, 62, 634662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Misyak, J. B., Goldstein, M. H., & Christiansen, M. H. (2012). Statistical-sequential learning in development. In Rebuschat, P. & Williams, J. N. (Eds.), Statistical learning and language acquisition (pp. 1354). Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Morgan-Short, K., Deng, Z., Brill-Schuetz, K. A., Faretta-Stutenberg, M., Wong, P. C. M., & Wong, F. C. K. (this issue). A view of the neural representation of second language syntax through artificial language learning under implicit contexts of exposure. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan-Short, K., Faretta-Stutenberg, M., & Bartlet-Hsu, L. (in press). Contributions of event-related potential research to issues in explicit and implicit second language acquisition. In Rebuschat, P. (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Morgan-Short, K., Faretta-Stutenberg, M., Brill-Schuetz, K. A., Carpenter, H., & Wong, P. C. M. (2014). Declarative and procedural memory as individual differences in second language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 5672.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norris, J. M., & Ortega, L. (2000). Effectiveness of L2 instruction: A research synthesis and quantitative meta-analysis. Language Learning, 50, 417528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Onnis, L., Destrebecqz, A., Christiansen, M. H., Chater, N., & Cleeremans, A. (in press). Implicit learning of non-adjacent dependencies: A graded, associative account. In Rebuschat, P. (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Paciorek, A., & Williams, J. N. (this issue). Implicit learning of semantic preferences of verbs. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perruchet, P., & Pacton, S. (2006). Implicit learning and statistical learning: One phenomenon, two approaches. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 233238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perruchet, P., & Poulin-Charronnat, B. (in press). The learnability of language: Insights from the implicit learning literature. In Rebuschat, P. (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 219235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rebuschat, P. (2013). Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge in second language research. Language Learning, 63, 595626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rebuschat, P. (in press). Introduction: Implicit and explicit learning of languages. In Rebuschat, P. (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Rebuschat, P., Hamrick, P., Sachs, R., Riestenberg, K., & Ziegler, N. (this issue). Triangulating measures of awareness: A contribution to the debate on learning without awareness. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rebuschat, P., & Williams, J. N. (2012a). Introduction: Statistical learning and language acquisition. In Rebuschat, P. & Williams, J. N. (Eds.), Statistical learning and language acquisition (pp. 112). Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Rebuschat, P., & Williams, J. N. (Eds.). (2012b). Statistical learning and language acquisition. Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2003). Attention and memory during SLA. In Doughty, C. & Long, M. H. (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 631678). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, P. (2005). Cognitive abilities, chunk-strength, and frequency effects in implicit artificial grammar and incidental L2 learning: Replications of Reber, Walkenfeld, and Hernstadt (1991) and Knowlton and Squire (1996) and their relevance for SLA. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 235268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, R. (1990). The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 11, 129158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, R. (1995a). Attention and awareness in foreign language learning. Honolulu: University of Hawai'i Second Language Teaching and Curriculum Center.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R. (1995b). Consciousness and foreign language learning: A tutorial on attention and awareness in learning. In Schmidt, R. (Ed.), Attention and awareness in foreign language learning (pp. 163). Honolulu: University of Hawai'i National Foreign Language Resource Center.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R. (2001). Attention. In Robinson, P. (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 332). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shukla, M., Gervain, J., Mehler, J., & Nespor, M. (2012). Linguistic constraints on statistical learning in early language acquisition. In Rebuschat, P. & Williams, J. N. (Eds.), Statistical learning and language acquisition (pp. 171202). Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Spada, N., & Tomita, Y. (2010). Interactions between type of instruction and type of language feature: A meta-analysis. Language Learning, 60, 263308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tagarelli, K. M., Borges Mota, M., & Rebuschat, P. (in press). Working memory, learning context, and the acquisition of L2 syntax. In Zhisheng, W., Borges Mota, M., & McNeill, A. (Eds.), Working memory in second language acquisition and processing: Theory, research and commentary. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Tagarelli, K., Borges Mota, M., & Rebuschat, P. (2011). The role of working memory in implicit and explicit language learning. In Carlson, L., Hölscher, C., & Shipley, T. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. (pp. 20612066). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Tokowicz, N., & MacWhinney, B. (2005). Implicit and explicit measures of sensitivity to violations in second language grammar: An event-related potential investigation. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 173204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B. (2006). Input processing and grammar instruction. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B., & Cadierno, T. (1993). Explicit instruction and input processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 15, 225241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walk, A. M., & Conway, C. M. (in press). Implicit statistical learning and language acquisition: Experience-dependent constraints on learning. In Rebuschat, P. (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Williams, J. N. (2005). Learning without awareness. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 269304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. N. (2009). Implicit learning in second language acquisition. In Ritchie, W. C. & Bhatia, T. K. (Eds.), The new handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 319353). Bingley, UK: Emerald Press.Google Scholar