Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T07:11:31.877Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

NATIVE AND NONNATIVE PERCEPTION OF WESTERN ANDALUSIAN SPANISH /S/ ASPIRATION IN QUIET AND NOISE

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 March 2019

María Del Saz*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Santiago de Chile, Facultad de Humanidades, Departamento de Lingüística y Literatura
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to María Del Saz, Universidad de Santiago de Chile, Avenida Libertador Bernardo O'Higgins 3363, Estación Central, Santiago, Chile. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Many college students set out on Study Abroad Programs to other countries in which the variant of the foreign language spoken in the region differs from the variant to which learners have been exposed. This study explores the perception of L2 Western Andalusian Spanish aspiration of word-final /s/ by L1 American English listeners, in relation to their length of exposure to the L2 in quiet conditions and to their competence level in the L2 in noisy conditions. Results indicate that perception drops in adverse conditions, particularly for listeners in their intermediate stages of learning. Proficient listeners’ perception also suffers with respect to native listeners’ performance at lower signal-to-noise ratios. For all listeners, native and nonnative, standard features are best identified, whether in quiet or noise.

Type
Research Article
Open Practices
Open materials
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

The experiment in this article earned an Open Materials badge for transparent practices. The materials are available at https://www.iris-database.org/iris/app/home/detail?id=york%3a936116&ref=search.

References

REFERENCES

Alvar, M. (1996). Manual de Dialectología Hispánica. El Español de España. Barcelona, Spain: Ariel.Google Scholar
Alvar López, M. (2004). Estudios Sobre las Hablas Meridionales. Granada, Spain: Editorial Universidad de Granada.Google Scholar
Barreiro Bilbao, S. C. (1999). Perception of fricative “voicing” and “place of articulation” in English sibilants. In Actes del I Congrés de Fonètica Experimental (pp. 111118). Tarragona, Spain: Universitat Rovira i Virgili–Universitat de Barcelona.Google Scholar
Barreiro Bilbao, S. C. (2002). Speech perception in L2. Odisea, 2, 714.Google Scholar
Best, C. T., & Strange, W. (1992). Effects of phonological and phonetic factors on cross-language perception of approximants. Journal of Phonetics, 20, 305330.Google Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenik, P. (2009). Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer (Version 5.2.46) [Computer software]. Retrieved from http://www.praat.org/.Google Scholar
Bohn, O.-S., & Flege, J. E. (1990). Interlingual identification and the role of foreign language experience in L2 vowel perception. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 303328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boomershine, A., Hall, K. C., Hume, E., & Johnson, K. (2008). The impact of allophony vs. contrast on speech perception. In Avery, P., Dresher, B. E., & Rice, K. (Eds.), Phonological contrast (pp. 146172). New York, NY: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. (2002). Phonological evidence for exemplar storage of multiword sequences. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 215221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cebrian, J., & Chambers, C. G. (2011). Subphonemic detail in lexical perception and production: The case of Canadian Raising. In Lee, W.-S. & Zee, E. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 17th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 424427). Hong Kong, China: City University of Hong Kong.Google Scholar
Celata, C. (2007). Allophonic variation can affect L2 speech perception: Evidence from a Tuscan neutralization process. In Rauber, A. S., Watkins, M. A., & Baptista, B. O. (Eds.), New sounds 2007: Proceedings of the Fifth International Symposium on the Acquisition of Second Language Speech (pp. 114124). Florianópolis, Brazil: Federal University of Santa Catarina.Google Scholar
Cervera, T., & González-Álvarez, J. (2011). Test of Spanish sentences to measure speech intelligibility in noise conditions. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 459467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clopper, C. G., & Bradlow, A. R. (2006). Effects of dialect variation on speech intelligibility in noise. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 119, 3424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clopper, C. G., & Bradlow, A. R. (2008). Perception of dialect variation in noise: Intelligibility and classification. Language and Speech, 51, 175198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., Cooke, M., Garcia-Lecumberri, M. L., & Pasveer, D. (2007). L2 consonant identification in noise: Cross-language comparisons. In van Hamme, H. & van Son, R. (Eds.), Proceedings of Interspeech 2007 (pp. 15851588). Adelaide, Australia: Causal Productions.Google Scholar
Cutler, A., Smits, R., & Cooper, N. (2005). Vowel perception: Effects of nonnative language versus nonnative dialect. Speech Communication, 47, 3242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Del Saz, M. (2015). Effects of place of articulation on Spanish coda /s/ perception. In Cabedo Nebot, A. (Ed.), Perspectivas actuales en el análisis fónico del habla. Tradición y avances en la fonética experimental (pp. 399410). Valencia, Spain: Universitat de València.Google Scholar
Escudero, P., Benders, T., & Lipski, S. C. (2004). Native, non-native and L2 perceptual cue weighting for Dutch vowels: The case of Dutch, German, and Spanish listeners. Journal of Phonetics, 37, 452465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., & Boersma, P. (2004). Bridging the gap between L2 speech perception research and phonological theory. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 551585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E. (1995). Second-language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In Strange, W. (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience, issues in cross-linguistic research (pp. 233277). Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., Bohn, O.-S., & Jang, S. (1997). Effects of experience on non-native speakers' production and perception of English vowels. Journal of Phonetics, 25(4), 437470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García Lecumberri, M. L., Cooke, M., & Cutler, A. (2010). Non-native speech perception in adverse conditions: A review. Speech Communication, 52, 864886.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerfen, C. (2002). Andalusian codas. Probus, 14, 247277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, S. (2011) Does phonetic detail guide situation-specific speech recognition? In Lee, W.-S. & Zee, E. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 17th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 918). Hong Kong, China: City University of Hong Kong.Google Scholar
Hualde, J. I. (2014). Los sonidos del español. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Iverson, P., & Evans, B. G. (2009). Learning English vowels with different first language vowel systems II: Auditory training for native Spanish and German speakers. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 126, 866877.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 434446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, K. (2012). Acoustic and auditory phonetics (3rd ed.). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Kalikow, D. N., Stevens, K. N., & Elliott, L. L. (1977). Development of a test of speech intelligibility in noise using sentence materials with controlled word predictability. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 61, 13371352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kissling, E. M. (2015). Phonetics instruction improves learners’ perception of L2 sounds. Language Teaching Research, 19, 254275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, E. S. (2009). Language experience and consonantal context effects on perceptual assimilation of French vowels by American-English learners of French. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 125, 11381152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, E. S., & Strange, W. (2008). Perception of French vowels by American English adults with and without French language experience. Journal of Phonetics, 36(4), 141157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lipski, J. M. (1999). The many faces of Spanish /s/-weakening: (Re)alignment and ambisyllabicity. In Gutiérrez-Rexach, J. & Martínez-Gil, F. (Eds.), Advances in Hispanic Linguistics (pp. 198213). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
Major, R. C. (2001). Foreign accent: The ontogeny and phylogeny of second language phonology. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitterer, H., & Tuinman, A. (2012). The role of native-language knowledge in the perception of casual speech in a second language. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mora, J. C., & Rochdi, Y. (2016). Phonological awareness in Spanish-English interphonology: The case of spirantization. In Fernández Planas, A. Ma. (Ed.), 53 Reflexiones sobre Aspectos de la Fonética y otros Temas de Lingüística (pp. 395403). Barcelona, Spain: Universitat de Barcelona.Google Scholar
O’Neill, P. (2005). Utterance final /s/ in Andalusian Spanish: The phonetic neutralization of a phonological contrast. Language Design: Journal of Theoretical and Experimental Linguistics, 7, 151166.Google Scholar
Plitcha, B. (2010). Akustyk (Version 1.9.3) [Computer software]. Retrieved from http://bartus.org/akustyk/index.php.Google Scholar
Polka, L., & Strange, W. (1985). Perceptual equivalence of acoustic cues that differentiate /r/ and /l/. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 78, 11871197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romero Gallego, J. (1995). An articulatory study of historical /s/ aspiration in Spanish. Rivista di Linguistica, 7, 191208.Google Scholar
Roseano, P., Fernández Planas, A. M., Elvira-García, W., Cerdà Massó, R., & Martínez Celdrán, E. (2015). Diferencias perceptivas entre los acentos tonales prenucleares en catalán. In Cabedo Nebot, A. (Ed.), Perspectivas actuales en el análisis fónico del habla: tradición y avances en la fonética experimental (pp. 163173). Valencia, Spain: Universitat de València.Google Scholar
Ruch, H., & Harrington, J. (2014). Synchronic and diachronic factors in the change from pre-aspiration to post-aspiration in Andalusian Spanish. Journal of Phonetics, 45, 1225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruch, H., & Peters, S. (2016). On the origin of post-aspirated stops: Production and perception of /s/ + voiceless stop sequences in Andalusian Spanish. Laboratory Phonology. Journal of the Association of Laboratory Phonology, 7, 136.Google Scholar
Schmidt, L. (2018). L2 development of perceptual categorization of dialectal sounds. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40, 857882.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, L. B. (2009). The effect of dialect familiarity via a study abroad experience on L2 comprehension of Spanish. In Collentine, J., García, M., Lafford, B., & Marín, F. M. (Eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 11th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium (pp. 143154). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Schmidtke, J. (2016). The bilingual disadvantage in speech understanding in noise is likely a frequency effect related to reduced language exposure. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strange, W. (2011). Automatic selective perception (ASP) of first and second language speech: A working model. Journal of Phonetics, 39, 456466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strange, W., & Shafer, V. L. (2008). Speech perception in second language learners. In Hansen Edwards, J. G. & Zampini, M. L. (Eds.), Phonology and second language acquisition (pp. 153191). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sumner, M., & Samuel, A. G. (2009). The effect of experience on the perception and representation of dialect variants. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 487501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Syrdal, A. K. (2014). Perception of consonant place of articulation. In Lass, N. J. (Ed.), Speech and language: Advances in basic research and practice (pp. 313349). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tabri, D., Smith Abou Chacra, K. M., & Pring, T. (2010). Speech perception in noise by monolingual, bilingual and trilingual listeners. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 46, 411422.Google Scholar
Torreira, F. (2007a). Pre- and post-aspirated stops in Andalusian Spanish. In Prieto, P., Mascaro, J., & Solé, M.-J. (Eds.), Prosodic and segmental issues in romance (pp. 6782). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torreira, F. (2007b). Coarticulation between aspirated-s and voiceless stops in Spanish: An interdialectal comparison. In Sagarra, N. & Toribio, A. J. (Eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 9th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium (pp. 113120). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Torreira, F. (2012). Investigating the nature of aspirated stops in Western Andalusian Spanish. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 42, 4963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tuinman, A., Mitterer, H., & Cutler, A. (2007). Speakers differentiate English intrusive and onset /r/, but L2 listeners do not. In Trouvain, J. & Barry, W. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 16th International Congress Phonetic Sciences (pp. 19051908). Saarbrücken, Germany: Universität des Saarlandes.Google Scholar
Tuinman, A., Mitterer, H., & Cutler, A. (2011). Perception of intrusive /r/ in English by native, cross-language and cross-dialect listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130, 16431652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, A., Ernestus, M., & Cutler, A. (2006). Formant transitions in fricative identification: The role of native fricative inventory. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 120, 22672277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Widdison, K. A. (1993). Hacia los orígenes de la s aspirada en español. Estudios de Fonética Experimental, V, 3459.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: PDF

Del Saz supplementary material

Del Saz supplementary material 1

Download Del Saz supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 9.8 KB