Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T05:08:31.891Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Early Childhood Bilingualism

In Search of Explanation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 November 2008

Linda Schinke-Llano
Affiliation:
Millikin University

Extract

Although a number of researchers over the years have focused on the language development of bilingual children, we currently possess only a fragmentary picture of the total phenomenon. Further, what appears to be known about the processes involved may need reexamination in light of recent theoretical and empirical work. It is the purpose of this article to review recent research (i.e., that done within the last five to six years), as well as to discuss difficulties inherent in research on bilingual children. In addition, the article argues for a multidisciplinary approach to research in the area and outlines suggested avenues of inquiry within such a framework. Throughout, the focus is on children's bilingual acquisition in naturalistic settings (i.e., prior to school attendance).

Type
State of the art
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1989

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Albanèse, J. (1985). Language lateralization in English-French bilinguals. Brain and Language, 24, 284296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alvarez, L. (1986). Home and school contexts for language learning: A case study of two Mexican-American bilingual preschoolers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Amodeo, L., & Arnberg, L. (1983). Issues in early childhood bilingualism: Pros and cons. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 230 306)Google Scholar
Andersen, R. (1983). Transfer to somewhere. In Gass, S. & Selinker, L. (Eds.), Language transfer in language learning (pp. 177201). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Arnberg, L. (1984). Mother tongue playgroups for pre-school bilingual children. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 5, 6584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arsenian, S. (1937). Bilingualism and mental development. New York: Columbia University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Babu, N. (1984). Perception of syntactic ambiguity by bilingual and unilingual tribal children. Psycho-lingua, 14, 4754.Google Scholar
Bai, B. L. (1984). Dative in Dravidian—A perspective from child language. Psycho-lingua, 14, 135144.Google Scholar
Ben Zeev, S. (1977). The influence of bilingualism on cognitive strategy and cognitive development. Child Development, 48, 10091018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergman, C. (1976). Interference vs. independent development in infant bilingualism. In Keller, G., Teschner, R., & Viera, S. (Eds.), Bilingualism in the bicentennial and beyond (pp. 8696). New York: Bilingual Press.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1986a). Children's concept of word. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 15, 1332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1986b). Factors in the growth of metalinguistic awareness. Child Development, 57, 498510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1987). Words as things: Development of word concept by bilingual children. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 9, 133140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bizzarri, H. (1984). Language and thought in bilingual children. Rassegna Italiana di Linguistica Applicata, 16, 223233.Google Scholar
Boeschoten, H., & Verhoeven, L. (1987). Language mixing in children's speech: Dutch language use in Turkish discourse. Language Learning, 37, 191215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. (1983). Child's talk. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Burling, S. (1959). Language development of a Garo and English speaking child. Word, 15, 4568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Celce-Murcia, M. (1978). The simultaneous acquisition of English and French in a two-year-old child. In Hatch, E. (Ed.), Second language acquisition: A book of readings (pp. 3853). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Chengappa, S. (1984). Code-alternation in an early bilingual. Psycho-lingua, 14, 5562.Google Scholar
Díaz, R. (1985). The intellectual power of bilingualism. In California State Department of Education (Ed.), Second language learning by young children. Sacramento, CA: California State Department of Education, Child Development Programs Advisory Committee. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 283 368)Google Scholar
Dodson, C. J. (1985). Second language acquisition and bilingual development: A theoretical framework. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 6, 325346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Döpke, S. (1986). Discourse structures in bilingual families. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 7, 493507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dulay, H., & Burt, M. (1974). Natural sequences in child second language acquisition. Language Learning, 24, 3753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, S., & DeAvila, E. (1979). Bilingualism and cognition: Some recent findings. NABE Journal, 4, 1550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckman, F. (1981). On the naturalness of interlanguage phonological rules. Language Learning, 31, 195216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, M. (1987). Linguistic accommodation in a bilingual family: One perspective on the language acquisition of a bilingual child being raised in a monolingual community. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 8, 231235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fried, L. (1985). On the validity of second language tests. In Hyltenstam, K. & Pienemann, M. (Eds.), Modelling and assessing second language acquisition (pp. 349371). San Diego: College Hill Press.Google Scholar
Galambos, S., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1983). Learning a second language and metalinguistic awareness. In Chicago Linguistic Society Papers from the Nineteenth Regional Meeting, (pp. 117133). Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
García, E. (1983). Becoming bilingual in early childhood. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 6, 375404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gass, S., & Selinker, L. (1983). Language transfer in language learning. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
George, K. (1985, 07). Bringing up a bilingual child. Language Monthly, pp. 2223.Google Scholar
Genesee, F. (in press). Early bilingual development: One language or two? Journal of Child Language.Google Scholar
Giles, H., Taylor, D., & Bourhis, R. (1973). Towards a theory of interpersonal accommodation through language: Some Canadian data. Language in Society, 2, 177192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorrell, J. (1987). Spatial role-taking ability among bilingual and monolingual kindergarten children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 16, 9199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gorrell, J., Bregman, N., McAllister, H., & Lipscomb, T. (1982). A comparison of spatial role-taking in monolingual and bilingual children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 140, 310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. (1986). Control, activation, and resource: A framework and a model for the control of speech in bilinguals. Brain and Language, 27, 210223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grenier, G., & Vaillancourt, F. (1983). An economic perspective on learning a second language. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 4, 471483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F. (1985). The bilingual as a competent but specific speaker-hearer. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 6, 467477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hakuta, K. (1974). A report on the development of the grammatical morphemes in a Japanese girl learning English as a second language. Working Papers on Bilingualism, 4, 1844.Google Scholar
Harding, E. (1986). Mother tongue and other tongue. English Today, 7, 3133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harding, E., & Riley, P. (1986). The bilingual family: A handbook for parents. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Harley, B. (1986). Age in second language acquisition. San Diego: College Hill Press.Google Scholar
Hoffman, C. (1985). Language acquisition in two trilingual children. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 6, 479495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoppe, R., & Kess, J. (1983). The acquisition of metalinguistic abilities. Rassegna Italiana di Linguistica Applicata, 15, 105120.Google Scholar
Hulstijn, J. (1985). Second language proficiency: An interactive approach. In Hyltenstam, K. & Pienemann, M. (Eds.), Modelling and assessing second language acquisition (pp. 373380). San Diego: College Hill Press.Google Scholar
Ianco-Worrall, A. (1972). Bilingualism and cognitive development. Child Development, 43, 13901400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imedadze, N. (1960). K psykhologicheskoy prirode rannego dvuyazychiya. Voprosy Psikhologii, 6, 6068.Google Scholar
Imedadze, N. (1967). On the psychological nature of child speech formation under conditions of exposure to two languages. International Journal of Psychology, 2, 129132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingram, D. (1981). The emerging phonological system of an Italian-English bilingual child. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, New York. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 213 263)Google Scholar
Kessler, C. (1984). Language acquisition processes in bilingual children. Bilingual Education Paper Series, 7. Los Angeles: California State University Evaluation, Dissemination, and Assessment Center. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 255 046)Google Scholar
Klinger, C. (1985). The self-regulatory speech of children in an additive bilingual situation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of New Mexico.Google Scholar
Lado, R. (1957). Linguistics across cultures. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Leopold, W. (1947). Sound-learning in the first two years: Vol. 2. Speech development of a bilingual child: A linguist's record. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Leopold, W. (1949). Grammar and general problems: Vol. 3. Speech development of a bilingual child: A linguist's record. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Levy, Y. (1985). Theoretical gains from the study of bilingualism: A case report. Language Learning, 35, 541554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindholm, K., & Padilla, A. (1975). Language mixing in bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 5, 327335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Littlewood, W. (1984). Foreign and second language learning: Language acquisition research and its implications for the classroom. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lombardi, J. III, (1986). Le abilita metalinguistiche nei soggetti bilingui. Rassegna Italiana di Linguistica Applicata, 18, 103114.Google Scholar
Mackey, W. (1983). Six questions sur la valeur d'une dichotomie (L1/L2). Rassegna Italiana di Linguistica Applicata, 75, 2351.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1978). The acquisition of morphophonology. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 43 (l2).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1983). Miniature linguistic systems as tests of the use of universal operating principles in second-language learning by children and adults. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 5, 467478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcon, R. (1985). Variation in children's nonverbal communication as a function of listener and second-language development. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 146, 459468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcon, R., & Coon, R. (1983). Communication styles of bilingual preschoolers in preferred and nonpreferred languages. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 142, 189202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCreary, D. (1985). A Vygotskyan psycholinguistic perspective on the acquisition and loss of Japanese. Issues in Applied Psycholinguistics, 17, 159171.Google Scholar
McLaughlin, B. (1984). Second language acquisition in childhood: Vol. 1. Preschool children (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Meisel, J. (1985). Les phases initiales du développement de notions temporelles. Lingua, 66, 321374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J. (1986). Word order and case marking in early child language. Evidence from simultaneous acquisition of two first languages: French and German. Linguistics, 24, 123183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merrill, J. (1984). Bringing up baby bilingual. New York: Facts on File Publications.Google Scholar
Mikěs, M. (1967). Acquisition des catégories grammaticales dans le langage de I'enfant. Enfance, 289298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mininni, G. (1983). La metacommunication en L1 et en L2. Rassegna Italiana di Linguistica Applicata, 15, 295309.Google Scholar
Oksaar, E. (1971). Code switching as an interactional strategy for developing bilingual competence. Word, 27, 377385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Malley, J. M., Chamot, A., & Walker, C. (1987). Some applications of cognitive theory to second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 9, 287306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owens, M. (1986). Eithne: A study of second language development. CLCS Occasional Paper, 15, Dublin University, Trinity College. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 268 842)Google Scholar
Padilla, A., & Liebman, E. (1975). Language acquisition in a bilingual child. Bilingual Review, 2, 3455.Google Scholar
Paradis, M., & LeBrun, I. (Eds.). (1984). Early bilingualism and child development. Lisse, the Netherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Pattnaik, K., & Mohanty, A. (1984). Relationship between metalinguistics and cognitive development of bilingual and unilingual tribal children. Psycho-lingua, 14, 6370.Google Scholar
Pavlovitch, M. (1920). Le langage enfantin: Acquisition du serbe et du français par un enfant serbe. Paris: Champion.Google Scholar
Peal, E., & Lambert, W. (1962). The relation of bilingualism to intelligence. Psychological Monographs, 76, 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pedersen, K. (1987). German minority children in the Danish border region: Code-switching and interference. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 8, 111120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petersen, J. (1988). Word-internal code-switching constraints in a bilingual child's grammar. Linguistics, 26, 479493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prakash, P. (1984). Second language acquisition and critical period hypothesis. Psycho-lingua, 14, 1317.Google Scholar
Pye, C. (1986). One lexicon or two? An alternative interpretation of early bilingual speech. Journal of Child Language, 13, 591593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapport, R., Tan, C., & Whitaker, H. (1983). Language function and dysfunction among Chinese- and English-speaking polyglots: Cortical stimulation, Wada testing, and clinical studies. Brain and Language, 18, 324366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ravem, R. (1978). Two Norwegian children's acquisition of English syntax. In Hatch, E. (Ed.), Second language acquisition: A book of readings (pp. 148154). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Redlinger, W., & Park, T. (1980). Language mixing in young bilinguals. Journal of Child Language, 7, 337352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robertson, H. Jr, (1986). The order of acquisition of fourteen grammatical morphemes in English by a native Spanish-speaking four-year-old learning English in a naturalistic setting. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Texas, Austin.Google Scholar
Ronjat, J. (1913). Le développement du langage observé chez un enfant bilingue. Paris: Champion.Google Scholar
Saunders, G. (1982). Bilingual children: Guidance for the family. Clevedon, England: Multilingual matters.Google Scholar
Saville-Troike, M. (1987). Dilingual discourse: The negotiation of meaning without a common code. Linguistics, 25, 81106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schinke-Llano, L. (1986). Language acquisition: Evidence from bilingual children. Paper presented at the Twentieth Annual TESOL Conference, Anaheim, CA.Google Scholar
Scliar-Cabral, L. (1983). Acquisition of narrative skills by children: Possible influences in second language learning. Rassegna Italiana di Linguistica Applicata, 15, 131142.Google Scholar
Scribner, S., & Cole, M. (1981). The psychology of literacy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In Ferguson, C. & Slobin, D. (Eds.), Studies of child language development (pp. 175208). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Smith, M. (1935). A study of the speech of eight bilingual children of the same family. Child Development, 6, 1925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tabouret-Keller, A. (1963). L'acquisition du langage parlé chez un petit enfant en milieu bilingue. Problemes de Psycholinguistique, 8, 205219.Google Scholar
Taeschner, T. (1983). Does the bilingual child possess twice the lexicon of the monolingual child? Rassegna Italiana di Linguistica Applicata, 15, 179188.Google Scholar
Titone, R., & Danesi, M. (1985). Applied psycholinguistics: An introduction to the psychology of language learning and teaching. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. (1982). The acquisition of morphology by a bilingual child: A whole-word approach. Applied Psycholinguistics, 3, 141160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vihman, M. (1985). Language differentiation by the bilingual infant. Journal of Child Language, 12, 297324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vihman, M., & McLaughlin, B. (1982). Bilingualism and second language acquisition in preschool children. In Brainerd, C. & Pressley, M. (Eds.), Verbal processes in children (pp. 3558). New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vilke, M. (1983). Linguistic and conceptual transfer from L1 to L2 in children. Rassegna Italiana di Linguistica Applicata, 15, 285294.Google Scholar
Volterra, V., & Taeschner, T. (1978). The acquisition and development of language by bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 5, 311326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisgerber, L. (1935). Deutsches volk und deutsche sprache. Frankfurt am Main.Google Scholar
Wells, G. (1981). Learning through interaction: The study of language development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whalen, S. (1985). Mother-child interaction: Methodological considerations. Issues in Applied Psycholinguistics, 17, 231242.Google Scholar
Williamson, R., & Williamson, V. (1984). Selected factors in bilingualism: The case of Galicia. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 5, 401413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wode, H. (1978). Developmental sequences in naturalistic L2 acquisition. In Hatch, E. (Ed.), Second language acquisition: A book of readings (pp. 101117). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Wong-Fillmore, L. (1976). The second time around: Cognitive and social strategies in second language acquisition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Zobl, H. (1980). The formal and developmental selectivity of L1 influence on L2 acquisition. Language Learning, 30, 4357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zobl, H. (1983). L1 acquisition, age of L2 acquisition, and the learning of word order. In Gass, S. & Selinker, L. (Eds.), Language transfer in language learning (pp. 205221). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar