Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T13:16:52.583Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

THE TIMING OF ISLAND EFFECTS IN NONNATIVE SENTENCE PROCESSING

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 March 2012

Claudia Felser*
Affiliation:
University of Essex
Ian Cunnings
Affiliation:
University of Essex
Claire Batterham
Affiliation:
University of Essex
Harald Clahsen
Affiliation:
University of Essex
*
*Address correspondence to Claudia Felser, Potsdam Research Institute for Multilingualism, University of Potsdam, Karl-Liebknecht-Strasse 24-25, 14476 Potsdam, Germany; e-mail: [email protected].

Abstract

Using the eye-movement monitoring technique in two reading comprehension experiments, this study investigated the timing of constraints on wh-dependencies (so-called island constraints) in first- and second-language (L1 and L2) sentence processing. The results show that both L1 and L2 speakers of English are sensitive to extraction islands during processing, suggesting that memory storage limitations affect L1 and L2 comprehenders in essentially the same way. Furthermore, these results show that the timing of island effects in L1 compared to L2 sentence comprehension is affected differently by the type of cue (semantic fit versus filled gaps) signaling whether dependency formation is possible at a potential gap site. Even though L1 English speakers showed immediate sensitivity to filled gaps but not to lack of semantic fit, proficient German-speaking learners of English as a L2 showed the opposite sensitivity pattern. This indicates that initial wh-dependency formation in L2 processing is based on semantic feature matching rather than being structurally mediated as in L1 comprehension.

Type
ARTICLES
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Alexopoulou, T., & Keller, F. (2007). Locality, cyclicity and resumption: At the interface between the grammar and the human sentence processor. Language, 83, 110160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, H., Piepenbrock, R., & van Rijn, H. (1993). The CELEX lexical database [CD ROM]. Philadelphia: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Balota, D., Yap, M., Cortese, M., Hutchison, K., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., et al. . (2007). The English lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 445459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, N. (1973). Conditions on transformations. In Anderson, S. & Kiparsky, P. (Eds.), A festschrift for Morris Halle (pp. 232286). New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Featherston, S. (1999). Antecedent priming at trace positions: Evidence from German scrambling. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 415437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006a). Continuity and shallow structures in language processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 107126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006b). Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clifton, C., Staub, A., & Rayner, K. (2007). Eye movements in reading words and sentences. In van Gompel, R. (Ed.), Eye movements: A window on mind and brain (pp. 341372). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cunnings, I., Batterham, C., Felser, C., & Clahsen, H. (2010). Constraints on L2 learners’ processing of wh-dependencies: Evidence from eye movements. In VanPatten, B. & Jegerski, J. (Eds.), Research in second language processing and parsing: Issues in theory and research (pp. 87110). Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dallas, A., & Kaan, E. (2008). Second language processing of filler-gap dependencies by late learners. Language and Linguistics Compass, 2, 372388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Felser, C., & Roberts, L. (2007). Processing wh-dependencies in a second language: A cross-modal priming study. Second Language Research, 23, 936.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frazier, L., & Clifton, C. (1989). Successive cyclicity in the grammar and the parser. Language and Cognitive Processes, 4, 93126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, C. (2005). Eye-movement recording as a tool for studying syntactic processing in a second language: A review of methodologies and experimental findings. Second Language Research, 21, 175198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garnsey, S., Tanenhaus, M., & Chapman, R. (1989). Evoked potentials and the study of sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18, 5160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, E. (1998). Syntactic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68, 175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, E. (2000). The dependency locality theory: A distance-based theory of linguistic complexity. In Marantz, A., Miyashita, Y., & O’Neil, W. (Eds.), Image, language, brain (pp. 95125). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gibson, E., & Warren, T. (2004). Reading-time evidence for intermediate linguistic structure in long-distance dependencies. Syntax, 7, 5578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grier, J. (1971). Nonparametric indexes for sensitivity and bias: Computing formulas. Psychological Bulletin, 75, 424429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrington, M., & Sawyer, M. (1992). Working memory capacity and L2 reading skill. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 14, 2538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hestvik, A., Maxfield, N., Schwartz, R., & Shafer, V. (2007). Brain responses to filled gaps. Brain and Language, 100, 301316.Google Scholar
Hofmeister, P., and Sag, I. (2010). Cognitive constraints and island effects. Language, 86, 366415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopp, H. (2006). Syntactic features and reanalysis in near-native processing. Second Language Research, 22, 369397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juffs, A. (2005). The influence of first language on the processing of wh-movement in English as a second language. Second Language Research, 21, 121151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juffs, A., & Harrington, M. (1995). Parsing effects in second language sentence processing: Subject and object asymmetries in wh-extraction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 17, 483516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kluender, R. (2004). Are subject islands subject to a processing account? In Rodríguez, A., Chand, V., Kelleher, A., & Scheiser, B. (Eds.), Proceedings of West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (Vol. 23; pp. 475499). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Kluender, R., & Kutas, M. (1993). Subjacency as a processing phenomenon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 573633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, M.-W. (2004). Another look at the role of empty categories in sentence processing (and grammar). Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 33, 5173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2005). A unified model of language acquisition. In Kroll, J. & De Groot, A. (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 4967). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Marinis, T., Roberts, L., Felser, C., & Clahsen, H. (2005). Gaps in second language sentence processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 5378.Google Scholar
Murray, W., & Rowan, S. (1998). Early, mandatory, pragmatic processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 27, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakano, Y., Felser, C., & Clahsen, H. (2002). Antecedent priming at trace positions in Japanese long-distance scrambling. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31, 531571.Google Scholar
Nicol, J. (1993). Reconsidering reactivation. In Shillcock, R. (Ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing: The second Sperlonga meeting (pp. 321347). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Phillips, C. (2006). The real-time status of island phenomena. Language, 82, 795823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M., & Barry, G. (1991). Sentence processing without empty categories. Language and Cognitive Processes, 6, 229259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollard, C., & Sag, I. (1994). Head-driven phrase structure grammar. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Warren, T., Juhasz, B., & Liversedge, S. (2004). The effect of plausibility on eye movements in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 12901301.Google ScholarPubMed
Roberts, L., & Felser, C. (2011). Plausibility and recovery from garden-paths in second-language sentence processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 299331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, L., Marinis, T., Felser, C., & Clahsen, H. (2007). Antecedent priming at gap positions in children’s sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36, 175188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, J. (1967). Constraints on variables in syntax. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Saah, K., & Goodluck, H. (1995). Island effects in parsing and grammar: Evidence from Akan. The Linguistic Review, 12, 381409.Google Scholar
Sato, M. (2007). Sensitivity to syntactic and semantic information in second language sentence processing. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Essex, United Kingdom.Google Scholar
Staub, A., & Rayner, K. (2007). Eye movements and online comprehension processes. In Gaskell, M. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 327342). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stowe, L. (1986). Parsing wh-constructions: Evidence for on-line gap location. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1, 227245.Google Scholar
Traxler, M., & Pickering, M. (1996). Plausibility and the processing of unbounded dependencies: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 542562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trueswell, J., Tanenhaus, M., & Kello, C. (1993). Verb-specific constraints in sentence processing: Separating effects of lexical preference from garden-paths. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 528553.Google ScholarPubMed
Wagers, M., & Phillips, C. (2009). Multiple dependencies and the role of the grammar in real-time comprehension. Journal of Linguistics, 45, 395433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. (2006). Incremental interpretation in second language sentence processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 7188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J., Möbius, P., & Kim, C. (2001). Native and non-native processing of English wh-questions: Parsing strategies and plausibility constraints. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 509540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar