Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T20:21:28.361Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

EFFECTS OF MULTITALKER INPUT AND INSTRUCTIONAL METHOD ON THE DIMENSION-BASED STATISTICAL LEARNING OF SYLLABLE-TONE COMBINATIONS

AN EYE-TRACKING STUDY

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 October 2020

Seth Wiener*
Affiliation:
Carnegie Mellon University
Kiwako Ito
Affiliation:
University of Newcastle
Shari R. Speer
Affiliation:
The Ohio State University
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Seth Wiener, Department of Modern Languages, Carnegie Mellon University, 160 Baker Hall, 5000 Forbes Avenue, Pittsburgh, PA 15213. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

To test the effects of talker variability and explicit instruction on the statistical learning of lexical tone, 80 monolingual English listeners were taught an artificial language that mimicked Mandarin’s asymmetric distribution of syllable-tone co-occurrences. Training stimuli consisted of either speech from one talker or speech from four talkers. Participants were either never instructed or explicitly taught associations between phonemes (CVs), tones, and nonce symbols across four consecutive days. Learning was assessed by the accuracy of mouse clicks and eye movements to visual nonce symbols. Critical trials induced competition between the target symbol, which matched the acoustic input, and a competitor symbol that had a statistically more probable tone (but mismatched the acoustic input). Eye fixations indicated that participants were sensitive to syllable-tone co-occurrence probabilities even without explicit instruction of tone. The degree to which statistical knowledge was used to recognize words appeared to increase when participants processed more variable speech.

Type
Research Article
Open Practices
Open materials
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

The experiment in this article earned an Open Materials badge for transparent practices. The materials are available at https://osf.io/pxd5f

This work was funded in part by a Doctoral Dissertation Research Improvement Grant from the National Science Foundation (BCS-1451677). We thank Marjorie Chan, Chao-Yang Lee, Mineharu Nakayama, Jessie Nixon, and the anonymous reviewers for their feedback on earlier versions of this work. We are especially grateful to James Brennan for his help with data collection.

References

REFERENCES

Arnon, I., & Ramscar, M. (2012). Granularity and the acquisition of grammatical gender: How order-of-acquisition affects what gets learned. Cognition, 122, 292305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baese-Berk, M. M., & Samuel, A. G. (2016). Listeners beware: Speech production may be bad for learning speech sounds. Journal of Memory and Language, 89, 2336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barcroft, J., & Sommers, M. S. (2005). Effects of acoustic variability on second language vocabulary learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 387414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barcroft, J., & Sommers, M. S. (2014). Effects of variability in fundamental frequency on L2 vocabulary learning: A comparison between learners who do and do not speak a tone language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 36, 423449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2016). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 6.0.19) [Computer program]. http://www.praat.orgGoogle Scholar
Bowles, A. R., Chang, C. B., & Karuzis, V. P. (2016). Pitch ability as an aptitude for tone learning. Language Learning, 66, 774808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cai, Q., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). SUBTLEX-CH: Chinese word and character frequencies based on film subtitles. PLOS ONE, 5, e10729.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caldwell-Harris, C. L., Lancaster, A., Ladd, D. R., Dediu, D., & Christiansen, M. H. (2015). Factors influencing sensitivity to lexical tone in an artificial language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37, 335357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chandrasekaran, B., Sampath, P. D., & Wong, P. C. M. (2010). Individual variability in cueweighting and lexical tone learning. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 128, 456465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, C. B., & Bowles, A. R. (2015). Context effects on second-language learning of tonal contrasts. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 138, 37033716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, H. C., Vaid, J., & Wu, J. T. (2009). Homophone density and phonological frequency in Chinese word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24, 967982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, J. Y., Chen, T. M., & Dell, G. S. (2002). Word-form encoding in Mandarin Chinese as assessed by the implicit priming task. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 751781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, J. Y., Lin, W. C., & Ferrand, L. (2003). Masked priming of the syllable in Mandarin Chinese speech production. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 45, 107120.Google Scholar
Chun, D. M., Jiang, Y., Meyr, J., & Yang, R. (2015). Acquisition of L2 Mandarin Chinese tones with learner-created tone visualizations. Journal of Second Language Pronunciation, 1, 86114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chung, K. K. (2003). Effects of Pinyin and first language words in learning of Chinese characters as a second language. Journal of Behavioral Education, 12, 207223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cleveland, W. S. (1979). Robust locally weighted regression and smoothing scatterplots. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 74, 829836.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colantoni, L., Steele, J., Escudero, P., & Neyra, P. R. E. (2015). Second language speech. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A. (2012). Native listening. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dahan, D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2001). Time course of frequency effects in spoken-word recognition: Evidence from eye movements. Cognitive Psychology, 42, 317367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeFrancis, J. (1986). The Chinese language: Fact and fantasy. University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Dong, H., Clayards, M., Brown, H., & Wonnacott, E. (2019). The effects of high versus low talker variability and individual aptitude on phonetic training of Mandarin lexical tones. PeerJ, 7, e7191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duanmu, S. (2007). The phonology of standard Chinese (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Duanmu, S. (2009). Syllable structure: The limits of variation. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2002). Frequency effects in language processing: A review with implications for theories of implicit and explicit language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 143188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2011). Frequency-based accounts of SLA. In Gass, S. & Mackey, A. (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 193210). Routledge/Taylor Francis.Google Scholar
Escudero, P., Benders, T., & Wanrooij, K. (2011). Enhanced bimodal distributions facilitate the learning of second language vowels. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130, EL206EL212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Escudero, P., & Boersma, P. (2004). Bridging the gap between L2 speech perception research and phonological theory. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 551585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Escudero, P., Hayes-Harb, R., & Mitterer, H. (2008). Novel second-language words and asymmetric lexical access. Journal of Phonetics, 36, 345360.Google Scholar
Ettlinger, M., Morgan-Short, K., Faretta-Stutenberg, M., & Wong, P. C. (2016). The relationship between artificial and second language learning. Cognitive Science, 40, 822847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, R. A., & Unkefer, J. (1985). The effect of lexical status on the perception of tone. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 13, 6990.Google Scholar
Gandour, J. (1983). Tone perception in far eastern-languages. Journal of Phonetics, 11, 149175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Godfroid, A., Lin, C. H., & Ryu, C. (2017). Hearing and seeing tone through color: An efficacy study of web-based, multimodal Chinese tone perception training. Language Learning, 67, 819857.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gómez, R. L., & Gerken, L. (2000). Infant artificial language learning and language acquisition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 178186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, P., & MacLeod, C. J. (2016). SIMR: An R package for power analysis of generalized linear mixed models by simulation. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 7, 493498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hao, Y. C. (2012). Second language acquisition of Mandarin Chinese tones by tonal and non-tonal language speakers. Journal of Phonetics, 40, 269279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hao, Y. C. (2018). Second language perception of Mandarin vowels and tones. Language and Speech, 61, 135152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardison, D. M. (2003). Acquisition of second-language speech: Effects of visual cues, context, and talker variability. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 495522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes-Harb, R. (2007). Lexical and statistical evidence in the acquisition of second language phonemes. Second Language Research, 23, 6594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes-Harb, R., & Masuda, K. (2008). Development of the ability to lexically encode novel second language phonemic contrasts. Second Language Research, 24, 533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ho, A. T. (1976). The acoustic variation of Mandarin tones. Phonetica, 33, 353367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holt, L. L., & Lotto, A. J. (2006). Cue weighting in auditory categorization: Implications for first and second language acquisition. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 30593071.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howie, J. (1976). Acoustical studies of Mandarin vowels and tones. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Idemaru, K., & Holt, L. L. (2011). Word recognition reflects dimension-based statistical learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 19391956.Google ScholarPubMed
Idemaru, K., & Holt, L. L. (2014). Specificity of dimension-based statistical learning in word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 10091021.Google ScholarPubMed
Idemaru, K., & Holt, L. L. (2020). Generalization of dimension-based statistical learning. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82, 17441762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamin, L. J. (1969). Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning. In Campbell, B. A. & Church, R. M. (Eds.), Punishment aversive behavior (pp. 279296). Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leather, J. (1983). Speaker normalization in perception of lexical tone. Journal of Phonetics, 11, 373382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lecumberri, M. L. G., Cooke, M., & Cutler, A. (2010). Non-native speech perception in adverse conditions: A review. Speech Communication, 52, 864886.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, C.-Y., Tao, L., & Bond, Z. S. (2009). Speaker variability and context in the identification of fragmented Mandarin tones by native and non-native listeners. Journal of Phonetics, 37, 115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, C.-Y., Tao, L., & Bond, Z. S. (2010). Identification of multi-speaker Mandarin tones in noise by native and non-native listeners. Speech Communication, 52, 900910CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, C.-Y., Tao, L., & Bond, Z. S. (2013). Effects of speaker variability and noise on Mandarin tone identification by native and non-native listeners. Speech, Language and Hearing, 16, 19CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, P., & Yip, M. C. (1998). Context effects and the processing of spoken homophones. In Leong, C. K. & Tamaoka, K. (Eds.), Cognitive processing of the Chinese and the Japanese languages (pp. 6989). Kluwer Academic Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Y., Wang, M., Perfetti, C. A., Brubaker, B., Wu, S., & MacWhinney, B. (2011). Learning a tonal language by attending to the tone: An in vivo experiment. Language Learning, 61, 11191141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lively, S. E., Logan, J. S., & Pisoni, D. B. (1993). Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/. II: The role of phonetic environment and talker variability in learning new perceptual categories. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 94, 12421255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: The neighborhood activation model. Ear and Hearing, 19, 136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malins, J. G., & Joanisse, M. F. (2010). The roles of tonal and segmental information in Mandarin spoken word recognition: An eyetracking study. Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 407420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malins, J. G., & Joanisse, M. F. (2012). Setting the tone: An ERP investigation of the influences of phonological similarity on spoken word recognition in Mandarin Chinese. Neuropsychologia, 50, 20322043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mandell, J. (2015). Tonometric [Computer software]. http://jakemandell.com/adaptivepitch/Google Scholar
Matin, E., Shao, K.C., & Boff, K. R. (1993). Saccadic overhead: Information-processing time with and without saccades. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 53, 372380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maye, J., Werker, J. F., & Gerken, L. (2002). Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, 82, B101B111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McQueen, J. M., & Cutler, A. (2010). Cognitive processes in speech perception. In Hardcastle, W. J., Laver, J., & Gibbon, F. E. (Eds.) The handbook of phonetic sciences (Vol. 1) (pp. 489520). Blackwell Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, C. B., & Jongman, A. (1997). Speaker normalization in the perception of Mandarin Chinese tones. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 18641877.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myers, J. (2002). An analogical approach to the Mandarin syllabary. Journal of Chinese Phonology, 11, 163190.Google Scholar
Myers, J. (2010). Chinese as a natural experiment. The Mental Lexicon, 5, 423437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nixon, J. S. (2018). Effective acoustic cue learning is not just statistical, it is discriminative. In Proceedings of INTERSPEECH (pp. 14471451).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nixon, J. S. (2020). Of mice and men: Speech sound acquisition as discriminative learning from prediction error, not just statistical tracking. Cognition, 197, e104081. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104081.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nixon, J. S., & Best, C. T. (2018). Acoustic cue variability affects eye movement behaviour during non-native speech perception. In Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Speech Prosody (pp. 493497).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nixon, J. S., van Rij, J., Mok, P., Baayen, R. H., & Chen, Y. (2016). The temporal dynamics of perceptual uncertainty: Eye movement evidence from Cantonese segment and tone perception. Journal of Memory and Language, 90, 103125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ong, J. H., Burnham, D., & Escudero, P. (2015). Distributional learning of lexical tones: A comparison of attended vs. unattended listening. PLOS ONE, 10, e0133446.Google Scholar
Ong, J. H., Burnham, D., Escudero, P., & Stevens, C. J. (2017). Effect of linguistic and musical experience on distributional learning of nonnative lexical tones. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60, 27692780.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Packard, J. L. (1999). Lexical access in Chinese speech comprehension and production. Brain and Language, 68, 8994.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Packard, J. L. (2000). The morphology of Chinese: A linguistic and cognitive approach. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pajak, B., Fine, A. B., Kleinschmidt, D. F., & Jaeger, T. F. (2016). Learning additional languages as hierarchical probabilistic inference: insights from first language processing. Language Learning, 66, 900944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pelzl, E. (2019). What makes second language perception of Mandarin tones hard? A non-technical review of evidence from psycholinguistic research. Chinese as a Second Language.The Journal of the Chinese Language Teachers Association, USA, 54, 5178.Google Scholar
Pelzl, E., Lau, E. F., Guo, T., & DeKeyser, R. (2019). Advanced second language learners’ perception of lexical tone contrasts. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41, 5986.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perrachione, T. K., Lee, J., Ha, L. Y. Y., & Wong, P. C. M. (2011). Learning a novel phonological contrast depends on interactions between individual differences and training paradigm design. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 130, 461472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potter, C. E., Wang, T., & Saffran, J. R. (2017). Second language experience facilitates statistical learning of novel linguistic materials. Cognitive Science, 41, 913927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qin, Z., Tremblay, A., & Zhang, J. (2019). Influence of within-category tonal information in the recognition of Mandarin-Chinese words by native and non-native listeners: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Phonetics, 73, 144157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qin, Z., & Zhang, C. (2019). The effect of overnight consolidation in the perceptual learning of non-native tonal contrasts. PLOS ONE, 14, e0221498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramscar, M., Dye, M., & McCauley, S. M. (2013). Error and expectation in language learning: The curious absence of “mouses” in adult speech. Language, 760793.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramscar, M., Yarlett, D., Dye, M., Denny, K., & Thorpe, K. (2010). The effects of feature-label-order and their implications for symbolic learning. Cognitive Science, 34, 909957.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Core Team, R. (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer program]. Version 3.6.1. http://www.r-project.orgGoogle Scholar
Sadakata, M., & McQueen, J. M. (2014). Individual aptitude in Mandarin lexical tone perception predicts effectiveness of high-variability training. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saffran, J. R. (2003). Statistical language learning mechanisms and constraints. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 110114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saffran, J. R., Newport, E. L., Aslin, R. N., Tunick, R. A., & Barrueco, S. (1997). Incidental language learning: Listening (and learning) out of the corner of your ear. Psychological Science, 8, 101105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saito, K. (2011). Examining the role of explicit phonetic instruction in native-like and comprehensible pronunciation development: An instructed SLA approach to L2 phonology. Language Awareness, 20, 4559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saito, K. (2015). Communicative focus on second language phonetic form: Teaching Japanese learners to perceive and produce English/ɹ/without explicit instruction. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36, 377409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saito, K., & Wu, X. (2014). Communicative focus on form and second language suprasegmental learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 36, 647680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shen, X. S. (1989). Toward a register approach in teaching Mandarin tones. Journal of the Chinese Language Teachers Association, 24, 2747.Google Scholar
Shen, X. S., & Lin, M. C. (1991). A perceptual study of Mandarin tones 2 and 3. Language and Speech, 34, 145156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Showalter, C. E., & Hayes-Harb, R. (2013). Unfamiliar orthographic information and second language word learning: A novel lexicon study. Second Language Research, 29, 185200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Theodore, R. M., Monto, N. R., & Graham, S. (2019). Individual differences in distributional learning for speech: What’s ideal for ideal observers? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 113.Google ScholarPubMed
Turk-Browne, N. B., Scholl, B. J., Johnson, M. K., & Chun, M. M. (2010). Implicit perceptual anticipation triggered by statistical learning. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 1117711187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vitevitch, M. S., & Luce, P. A. (1998). When words compete: Levels of processing in perception of spoken words. Psychological Science, 9, 325329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vitevitch, M. S., & Luce, P. A. (1999). Probabilistic phonotactics and neighborhood activation in spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 374408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vitevitch, M. S., Luce, P. A., Pisoni, D. B., & Auer, E. T. (1999). Phonotactics, neighborhood activation, and lexical access for spoken words. Brain and Language, 68, 306311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wade, T., Jongman, A., & Sereno, J. (2007). Effects of acoustic variability in the perceptual learning of non-native-accented speech sounds. Phonetica, 64, 122144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, H. S. (1998). An experimental study on the phonotactic constraints of Mandarin Chinese. Studia Linguistica Serica, 259268.Google Scholar
Wang, T., & Saffran, J. R. (2014). Statistical learning of a tonal language: The influence of bilingualism and previous linguistic experience. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 953.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y., Spence, M. M., Jongman, A., & Sereno, J. A. (1999). Training American listeners to perceive Mandarin tones. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106, 36493658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wanrooij, K., Escudero, P., & Raijmakers, M. E. (2013). What do listeners learn from exposure to a vowel distribution? An analysis of listening strategies in distributional learning. Journal of Phonetics, 41, 307319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weber, A., & Cutler, A. (2004). Lexical competition in non-native spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weber, A., & Cutler, A. (2006). First-language phonotactics in second-language listening. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 597607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wewalaarachchi, T. D., Wong, L. H., & Singh, L. (2017). Vowels, consonants, and lexical tones: Sensitivity to phonological variation in monolingual Mandarin and bilingual English–Mandarin toddlers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 1633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiener, S. (2015). The representation, organization and access of lexical tone by native and non-native Mandarin speakers (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). The Ohio State University.Google Scholar
Wiener, S., Chan, M. K. M., & Ito, K. (2020). Do explicit instruction and high variability phonetic training improve non-native speakers’ Mandarin tone productions? The Modern Language Journal, 104, 152168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiener, S., & Ito, K. (2015). Do syllable-specific tonal probabilities guide lexical access? Evidence from Mandarin, Shanghai and Cantonese speakers. Language, Cognition & Neuroscience, 30, 10481060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiener, S., & Ito, K. (2016). Impoverished acoustic input triggers probability-based tone processing in mono-dialectal Mandarin listeners. Journal of Phonetics, 56, 3851.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiener, S., Ito, K., & Speer, S. R. (2018). Early L2 spoken word recognition combines input-based and knowledge-based processing. Language and Speech, 61, 632656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiener, S., & Lee, C. Y. (2020). Multi-talker speech promotes greater knowledge-based spoken Mandarin word recognition in first and second language listeners. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiener, S., Lee, C. Y., & Tao, L. (2019). Statistical regularities affect the perception of second language speech: Evidence from adult classroom learners of Mandarin Chinese. Language Learning, 69, 527558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiener, S., & Turnbull, R. (2016). Constraints of tones, vowels and consonants on lexical selection in Mandarin Chinese. Language and Speech, 59, 5982.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, P. C. M., & Perrachione, T. K. (2007). Learning pitch patterns in lexical identification by native English-speaking adults. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 565585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xing, J. Z. (2006). Teaching and learning Chinese as a foreign language: A pedagogical grammar (Vol. 1). Hong Kong University Press.Google Scholar
Yang, B. (2015). Perception and production of Mandarin tones by native speakers and L2 learners. Springer Publisher.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yin, B. Y. (1984). Hanyu yusu de dingliang yanjiu [A quantitative research of Chinese morphemes]. Zhongguo Yuwen [Chinese Literature and Language], 182, 338347.Google Scholar
Zee, Y.-Y. (1980). Phonetic studies of Chinese tones (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of California.Google Scholar
Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (1994). Words, morphemes and syllables in the Chinese mental lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 9, 393422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (1995). Morphological structure in the Chinese mental lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10, 545600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zipf, G. K. (1935). The psychobiology of language. Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Zipf, G. K. (1949). Human behavior and the principle of least-effort. Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Wiener et al. supplementary material

Appendix

Download Wiener et al. supplementary material(File)
File 24.7 KB