Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T14:05:24.308Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

See red? Turn pale? Unveiling Emotions through Cardiovascular and Hemodynamic Changes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 April 2014

Pedro Montoya*
Affiliation:
University of the Balearic Islands
J. Javier Campos
Affiliation:
Complutense Universidad of Madrid
Rainer Schandry
Affiliation:
University of Munich
*
Address correspondence to: Dr. Pedro Montoya, Department of Psychology, University of the Balearic Islands, 07071 Palma de Mallorca, Spain. E-mail: [email protected]. Phone +34 971 172646. Mobile phone +34 656 913926. Fax: +34 971 172309

Abstract

Cardiodynamic and hemodynamic reactions to emotion-eliciting film sequences were investigated. Thirty-two healthy subjects (12 women, 20 men) were randomly assigned to one of two groups. In the first group, anger was induced using selected scenes of the film “Ragtime.” In the second group, scenes of the film “The Shining” were chosen to elicit fear. A documentary film was used as a baseline stimulus in both groups. EKG, impedance cardiography, and blood pressure were continuously monitored. The two emotional conditions elicited significant differential changes in subjective ratings and cardiovascular indices. Fear was associated with decreased cardiac output, increased total peripheral resistance, and a reduction in stroke volume and myocardial contractility. Anger was associated with an increase of cardiac output and small changes in total peripheral resistance. These results support the hypothesis that discrete emotions such as fear and anger elicit differential patterns of physiological responses.

Se investigaron las reacciones cardiodinámicas y hemodinámicas asociadas a la visión de secuencias de películas con distinto contenido emocional. Treinta y dos sujetos (12 mujeres y 20 hombres) se asignaron al azar a uno de los dos grupos. En un grupo se indujo la ira utilizando una selección de escenas de la película “Ragtime”. En el segundo grupo se escogieron escenas de la película “El Resplandor” para elicitar el estado afectivo relacionado con el miedo. Se utilizó un documental para establecer la línea de base en ambos grupos. Se registró de modo continuo la presión sanguínea, el EKG y la cardiografía de impedancia. Las dos condiciones emocionales relacionadas con el miedo o la ira mostraron cambios diferencialmente significativos en la valoración introspectiva realizada por los sujetos y en los índices cardiovasculares. El miedo se asociaba con un descenso de la salida cardiaca, un incremento de la resistencia periférica total y una reducción del volumen sanguíneo y de la contractilidad miocárdica. La ira estaba asociada con un incremento de la salida cardiaca y cambios reducidos en la resistencia periférica. Estos resultados apoyan la hipótesis de que las emociones discretas de miedo e ira elicitan patrones diferenciados de respuestas psicofisiológicas.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, M.T., Matthews, K.A., & Sherman, F.S. (1997). Cardiovascular reactivity to stress and left ventricular mass in youth. Hypertension, 30, 82787.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Averill, J.R. (1969). Autonomic response patterns during sadness and mirth. Psychophysiology, 5, 399414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ax, A.(1953). The physiological differentiation between fear and anger in humans. Psychosomatic Medicine, 15, 433442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bogaard, H.J., Woltjer, H.H., Dekker, B.M., van Keimpema, A.R.J., Postmus, P.E., & de Vries, P.M.J.M. (1997). Haemodynamic response to exercise in healthy young and elderly subjects. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 75, 435442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boutcher, S.H., & Stocker, D. (1999). Cardiovascular responses to light isometric and aerobic exercise in 21- and 59-year-old males. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 80, 220226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, W., Corriveau, D., & Monti, P. (1977). Anger arousal by a motion picture: A methodological note. American Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 930931.Google ScholarPubMed
Brown, W., & Heninger, G. (1975). Cortisol, growth hormone, free fatty acids and experimentally evoked affective arousal. American Journal of Psychiatry, 132, 11721176.Google ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, J.T., Klein, D.J., Bernston, G.G., & Hatfield, E. (1993). The psychophysiology of emotion. In Lewis, M. & Haviland, J.M. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 119142). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Cannon, W.B. (1927). The James-Lange theory of emotions: A critical examination and a alternative theory. American Journal of Psychology, 39, 106112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, M.H., Hull, J.G., Young, R.D., & Warren, G.G. (1987). Emotional reactions to dramatic film stimuli: The influence of cognitive and emotional empathy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 126133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., Levenson, R.W., & Friesen, W.V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221, 12081210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engel, G., Frader, M., Barry, C., & Morrow, G. (1984). Sadness evoked by a film of grief: An experimental study. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 14, 130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folkow, B. (1982). Physiological aspects of primary hypertension. Physiological Review, 62, 347504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, L.K., Carroll, D., Burns, V.E., Corkill, A.R., Harrison, C.M., Ring, C., & Drayson, M. (2000). Cardiovascular and secretory immunoglobulin A reactions to humorous, exciting, and didactic film presentations. Biological Psychology, 52, 113126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herd, J.A. (1991). Cardiovascular response to stress. Physiological Review, 71, 305330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horowitz, M.W.N. (1976). Stress films, emotion, and cognitive response. Archives of General Psychiatry, 33, 13391344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubert, W., & de Jong-Meyer, R. (1989). Emotional stress and saliva cortisol response. Journal of Clinical Chemistry and Clinical Biochemistry, 27, 235237.Google ScholarPubMed
Hubert, W., & de Jong-Meyer, R. (1991). Psychophysiological response patterns to positive and negative film stimuli. Biological Psychology, 31, 7393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ijzerman, R.G., Stehouwer, C.D., de Geus, E.J., van Weissenbruch, M.M., Delemarre-van de Waal, H.A., & Boomsma, D.I. (2003). Low birth weight is associated with increased sympathetic activity: Dependence on genetic factors. Circulation, 108, 566571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janke, W., & Debus, G. (1978). Die Eigenschaftswörterliste (EWL). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Johnson, A.K., & Anderson, E.A. (1990). Stress and arousal. In Cacioppo, J.T. & Tassinary, L.G. (Eds.), Principles of psychophysiology (pp. 216252). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kubicek, W.G., Patterson, R.P., & Witsoe, D.A. (1970). Impedance cardiography as a noninvasive method of monitoring cardiac function and other parameters of the cardiovascular system. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 170, 724732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lang, P.J. (1979). A bio-informational theory of emotional imagery. Psychophysiology 16, 495512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, P.J., Bradley, M.M., & Cuthbert, B.N. (1998). Emotion and motivation: Measuring affective perception. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 15, 397408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laux, L., Glanzmann, P., Schaffner, P., & Spielberger, C.D. (1981). Das State-Trait-Angstinventar (STAI). Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.Google Scholar
Levenson, R.W. (1992). Autonomic nervous system difference among emotions. Psychological Science, 3, 2327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lovallo, W.R., Wilson, M.F., Pincomb, G.A., Edwards, G.L., Topkinds, P., & Brackett, D.J. (1985). Activation patterns to aversive stimulation in man: Passive exposure versus effort to control. Psychophysiology, 22, 291293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, G.A., & Kozak, M.J. (1993). Three-systems assessment and the construct of emotion. In Birbaumer, N. & Öhman, A. (Eds.), The structure of emotion (pp. 3147). Toronto: Hogrefe & Huber.Google Scholar
Miller, G.A., Levin, D.N., Kozak, M.J., Cook, E.W., McLean, A., & Lang, P.J. (1987). Individual differences in perceptual imagery and psychophysiological response in emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 367390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neumann, S., & Waldstein, S.R. (2001). Similar patterns of cardiovascular response during emotional activation as a function of affective valence and arousal and gender. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 50, 245253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palomba, D., Sarlo, M., Angrilli, A., Mini, A., & Stegagno, L. (2000). Cardiac responses associated with affective processing of unpleasant film stimuli. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 36, 4557.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palomba, D., & Stegagno, L. (1993). Physiology, perceived emotion and memory: Responding to film sequences. In Birbaumer, N. & Öhman, A. (Eds.), The structure of emotion (pp. 156168). Toronto: Hogrefe & Huber.Google Scholar
Papillo, J.F., & Shapiro, D. (1990). The cardiovascular system. In Cacioppo, J.T. & Tassinary, L.G. (Eds.), Principles of psychophysiology (pp. 456512). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Petri, H.L. (1991). Motivation: Theory, research and applications. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Prkachin, K.M., Williams-Avery, R.M., Zwaal, C., & Mills, D.E. (1999). Cardiovascular changes during induced emotion: An application of Lang's theory of emotional imagery. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 47, 255267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richard, R., Lonsdorfer-Wolf, E., Charloux, A., Doutreleau, S., Buchheit, M., Oswald-Mammosser, M., Lampert, E., Mettauer, B., Geny, B., & Lonsdorfer, J. (2001). Noninvasive cardiac output evaluation during a maximal progressive exercise test, using a new impedance cardiograph device. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 85, 202207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, R., & Weerts, T. (1982). Cardiovascular responding during anger and fear imagery. Psychological Report, 50, 219230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, G.E., Weinberger, D.A., & Singer, J.A. (1981). Cardiovascular differentiation of happiness, sadness, anger, and fear following imagery and exercise. Psychosomatic Medicine, 43, 343364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwenkmezger, P., Hodapp, V., & Spielberger, C.D. (1989). Das State-Trait Argerausdrucks-Inventar (STAXI). Bern, Switzerland: Huber.Google Scholar
Sherwood, A., Allen, M.T., Fahrenberg, J., Kelsey, R.M., Lovallo, W.R., & Doornen, U.P. (1990). Methodological guidelines for impedance cardiography. Psychophysiology, 27, 122.Google ScholarPubMed
Sherwood, A., & Turner, J.R. (1992). A conceptual and methodological overview of cardiovascular reactivity research. In Turner, J.R.Sherwood, A., & Light, K.C. (Eds.), Individual differences in cardiovascular response to stress (pp. 332). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinha, R., Lovallo, W.R., & Parsons, O.A. (1992). Cardiovascular differentiation of emotions. Psychosomatic Medicine, 54, 422435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spielberger, C.D., Gorsuch, R.L., & Lushene, R.E. (1970). STAI Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Takahashi, T., Okada, A., Sayito, T., Hayano, J., & Miyamoto, Y. (2000). Difference in human cardiovascular response between upright and supine recovery from upright cycle exercise. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 81, 233239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, J.R. (1994). Cardiovascular reactivity and stress. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wesseling, K.H., Settels, J.J., & deWit, B. (1986). The measurement of continuous finger arterial pressure noninvasively in stationary subjects. In Schmidt, T.H.Dembroski, T.M., & Blümchen, G. (Eds.), Biological and psychological factors in cardiovascular disease (pp. 355375). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittling, W., Block, A., Schweiger, E., & Genzel, S. (1998). Hemisphere asymmetry in sympathetic control of the human myocardium. Brain and Cognition, 38, 1735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed