Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T10:49:52.409Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Psychological problems and associated factors at 6 years of age: Differences between sexes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 April 2014

Griselda Esparó*
Affiliation:
Preventive Medicine and Public Health, Rovira i Virgili University, Spain
Josepa Canals
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Rovira i Virgili University, Spain
Margarita Torrente
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Rovira i Virgili University, Spain
Joan D. Fernández-Ballart
Affiliation:
Preventive Medicine and Public Health, Rovira i Virgili University, Spain
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Griselda Esparó-Hidalgo, Unidad de Medicina Preventiva y Salud Publica, Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud y Medicina, Universidad Rovira I Virgili, Sant Llorenç 21, E43201 Reus, Cataluña (Spain). E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

In a non-clinical group of 130 children (65 boys and 65 girls), we evaluated the relationships between psychological problems using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) reported by parents, the Inattention Overactivity With Aggression (IOWA) scale reported by teachers, individual factors (Intellectual quotient [IQ], temperament and heart rate) and environmental factors (stress events, mother's profession and being or not being an only child). We found no differences between the sexes in the prevalence of total psychological problems in the clinical range, but girls had significantly more borderline total problems than boys. Girls tended to have more externalizing problems than boys. In boys, there were more links between individual and environmental factors and psychological problems, especially externalizing problems. A high score in psychological problems assessed by the CBCL affected the school performance of boys and the social performance of girls. For boys, IQ was significantly lower when the score for total behavioral problems was higher, and for girls IQ was significantly lower when the score for externalizing problems was higher. Understanding the different levels of vulnerability of the sexes at different periods of development may help to improve the treatment children in this age group receive.

Hemos evaluado, en un grupo de 130 niños (65 niños y 65 niñas) de 6 años de edad, las relaciones que se establecen entre problemas psicológicos (CBCL, obtenido de los padres y IOWA, obtenido de los maestros), factores individuales (cociente intelectual [C.I.], temperamento y frecuencia cardiaca) y factores ambientales (sucesos vitales, profesión materna y tener o no tener hermanos). No se han hallado diferencias entre sexos en la prevalecía de problemas totales de tipo clínico, pero las niñas han presentado significativamente más problemas en el rango límite que los niños. Se ha observado una tendencia a presentar más problemas exteriorizados en las niñas. En los varones se ha encontrado un mayor número de asociaciones entre los factores individuales y ambientales y los problemas psicológicos, principalmente los de tipo exteriorizado. Los problemas psicológicos han afectado la competencia escolar en los niños y la competencia social en las niñas. El C.I. se ha encontrado significativamente más bajo en la puntuación de problemas totales en niños y en la puntuación de problemas exteriorizados en niñas. La comprensión de diferentes vulnerabilidades entre sexos, en los diversos periodos evolutivos, puede ayudar a una mejor intervención en los niños de este grupo de edad.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Achenbach, T.M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/ 4-18 and 1991 profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont.Google Scholar
Anderson, J., Williams, S., McGee, R., & Silva, P. (1989). Cognitive and social correlates of DSM-III disorders in preadolescent children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 842846.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bilenberg, N. (1999). The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) and related material: Standardization and validation in Danish population based on clinically based samples. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 100, 237 (supplement).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bilenberg, N., & Horder, K. (1998). Behavioral symptoms among children and adolescents. Screening with the help of a questionnaire in a group of children aged between 4 and 17. Ügeskrift für Laeger, 160, 44234428.Google Scholar
Calkins, S.D., & Dedmon, S.E. (2000). Physiological and behavioral regulation in two-year-old children with aggressive/destructive behavior problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 103118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, S.B. (1994). Hard-to-manage preschool boys: Externalizing behavior, social competence, and family context at 2-year follow-up. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 22, 147166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Canals, J., Fernández-Ballart, J., & Esparó, G. (2003) Evolution of neonatal behavior assessment scale scores in the first month of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 26, 227237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Canals, J., Martí-Henneberg, C., Fernández-Ballart, J., & Domènech, E.(1995). A longitudinal study of depression in an urban Spanish pubertal population. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 4, 102111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colder, C.R., Mott, J.A., & Berman, A.S. (2002). The interactive effects of infant activity level and fear on growth trajectories of early childhood behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crijnen, A.A.M., Achenbach, T.M., & Verhulst, F.C. (1999). Problems reported by parents of children in multiple cultures: The Child Behavior Checklist syndrome constructs. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 569574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, M., & Emory, E. (1995) Sex differences in neonatal stress reactivity. Child Development, 66, 1427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dietz, K.R., Lavigne, J.V., Arend, R., & Rosenbaum, D. (1997). Relation between intelligence and psychopathology among preschoolers. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 26, 99107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giancola, P., Mezzich, A., & Tarter, R. (1998). Executive cognitive functioning, temperament, and antisocial behavior in conduct disordered adolescent females. Journal of Abnormal Pyschology, 107, 629641.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R., Simonoff, E., & Stevenson, J. (1995). The relationship of child IQ, parental IQ and sibling IQ to child behavioural deviance scores. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 409425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howlin, P., & Rutter, M. (1987). The consequences of language delay for other aspects of development. In Yule, W. & Rutter, M. (Eds.), Language development and disorders (pp. 271294). London: MacKeith.Google Scholar
Jacobson, K.C., Prescott, C.A., & Kendler, K.S. (2002). Sex differences in the genetic and environmental influences on the development of antisocial behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 395416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J., Reznick, J.S., & Snidman, N. (1998). Biological bases of childhood shyness. Science, 240, 167173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keenan, K., & Shaw, D. (1997). Developmental and social influences on young girls' early problem behavior. Psychology Bulletin, 121, 95113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kingston, L., & Prior, M. (1995). The development of patterns of stable, transient, and school-age onset aggressive behavior in young children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 348358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langbehn, D.R., Cadoret, R.J., Yates, W.R., Troughton, E.P., & Stewart, M.A (1998). Distinct contributions of conduct and oppositional defiant symptoms to adult antisocial behavior: Evidence from an adoption study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 821829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsson, B., & Frisk, M. (1999). Social competence and emotional/behaviour problems in 6-16-year-old Swedish schoolchildren. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 8, 2433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lerner, J.V., & Vicary, J.R. (1984). Difficult temperament and drug use. Journal of Drug Education, 14, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, D., Yager, C., Cobham-Portorreal, C., Klein, N., Showalker, C., & Anthony, A. (1991). A follow-up from the female delinquents: maternal contributions to the perpetuation of deviance. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 197201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loney, J., & Milich, R. (1982). Hyperactivity, inattention and aggression in clinical practice. Advances in Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 3, 113147.Google Scholar
McGee, R., Partridge, F., Williams, S., & Silva, P.A. (1991). Twelve-year follow-up of preschool hyperactive children. Journal American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 224232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pianta, R.C., & Caldwell, C.B. (1990). Stability of externalizing symptoms from kindergarten to first grade and factors related to instability. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 247258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pianta, R.C., & Castaldi, J.(1989). Stability of internalizing symptoms from kindergarten to first grade and factors related to instability. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 305316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prior, M., Smart, D., Sanson, A., & Oberklaid, F. (2000). Does shy-inhibited temperament in childhood lead to anxiety problems in adolescence? Journal American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 461468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raine, A. (1993). The psychopathology of crime: criminal behavior as a clinical disorder. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raine, A., Venables, P.H., & Mednick, S.A. (1997). Low Resting Heart Rate at Age 3 Years Predisposes to Aggression at age 11 years: Evidence from the Mauritius Child Health Project. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 14571464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57, 316331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Giller, H., & Hagell, A. (1998). Antisocial behavior by young people. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sanson, A., Oberklaid, F., Pedlow, R., & Prior, M. (1991). Risk indicators: Assessment of infancy predictors of pre-school behavioural maladjustment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 609626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarpa, A., & Raine, A. (1997). Psychophysiology of anger and violent behavior. The Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 20, 375394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, C.E., Snidman, N., & Kagan, J. (1999). Adolescent social anxiety as an outcome of inhibited temperament in childhood. Journal American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 10081015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinhausen, H.C., Winkler-Metzke, C., Meier, M., & Kannenberg, R. (1998). Prevalence of child and adolescent psychiatric disorders: The ZÜrich epidemiological study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 98, 262271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tschann, J.M., Kaiser, P., Chesney, M.A., Alkon, A., & Boyce, T. (1996). Resilience and vulnerability among preschool children: Family functioning, temperament, and behavioral problems. Journal American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 184192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, E.F. (2002). Adolescent neurodevelopment and psychopathology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 2428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1996). WPPSI, Escala de Inteligencia de Weschler para preescolar y primaria. Manual adaptación española. Madrid: TEA.Google Scholar
White, J.L., Moffitt, T.E., & Silva, P.A (1989). A prospective replication of the protective effects of IQ in subjects at high risk for juvenile delinquency. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 719724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Windle, M., & Lerner, R.M. (1986). Reassessing the dimensions of temperament individuality across the life span: The revised dimensions of temperament survey (DOTS-R). Journal of Adolescent Research, 1, 213230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zahn-Waxler, C., Cole, P.M., Welsh, J.D., & Fox, N.A. (1995). Psychophysiological correlates of empathy and prosocial behaviors in preschool children with behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 2748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar