Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T00:44:47.281Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Learning Approaches: Cross-Cultural Differences (Spain–Argentina) and Academic Achievement in College Students

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 June 2023

Agustín Freiberg-Hoffmann
Affiliation:
Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (Argentina) Universidad de Buenos Aires (Argentina)
Agustín Romero-Medina*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Murcia (Spain)
Beatriz López-Fernández
Affiliation:
Universidad de Murcia (Spain)
Mercedes Fernández-Liporace
Affiliation:
Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (Argentina) Universidad de Buenos Aires (Argentina)
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Agustín Romero-Medina. Universidad de Murcia. Departamento de Psicología Básica y Metodología. Campus de Espinardo, Facultad de Psicología. 30100 Murcia (Spain). E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Learning approaches describe the students’ degree of cognitive commitment to learning in diverse types of academic tasks and educational environments. Even though from a micro-level perspective different profiles of approaches have been identified in high-achievement undergraduates attending several majors, such profiles have not been examined from a macro-level approach in terms of distinct educational cultures. Therefore, the research involved two studies conducted on undergraduates from Argentina and Spain: The first one was aimed at analyzing the psychometric features of the Approaches and Study Skills Inventory for Students (ASSIST) whereas the second was focused on examining the learning approaches profiles of high and low achievers attending the same major (Psychology) in two different educational cultures (Spain and Argentina). The scale’s original internal structure, examined on a sample of 400 participants (50% Spanish), was verified except for one item, which was fatherly eliminated. The resulting structure was tested and proven verified in a new sample (N = 1,334; 58.3% Spanish) by confirmatory factor analysis, factorial invariance, and internal consistency studies. External validity evidence was examined as well. Additionally, norms to be used in the professional field were calculated.

Profiles of learning approaches by academic achievement from each country were examined by latent class analysis. In both cases, high achievers reported higher and more frequent use of the Deep and Strategic approaches and lower and less frequent usage of the Surface one. Further studies should replicate these analyses in undergraduates attending other majors in order to test the hypothesis sustaining these findings’ generalization.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de la Psicología de Madrid

Learning Approaches

The expression learning approaches was coined by Marton and Säljö (Reference Marton and Säljö1976). Such a notion describes the students’ cognitive commitment to learning which relies on the type of activity and the academic environment; the latter can boost learning processes as well as learning strategies, even when they could pursue opposite goals: Either the significant and long-term acquisition and integration of knowledge or the memorization and the information’s automatic reproduction (Biggs, 1999/Reference Biggs2005).

Research on learning approaches—Student Approaches to Learning, SAL—started in the 1970s, and focused on understanding how that variable interacted with the educational environment. To do so, researchers conducted an experiment examining different types of learning deployed by students when dealing with a unique academic task. The research problem was to describe how the same learning content was comprehended by different individuals in different ways. Thus, participants were exposed to ideas and principles presented in texts, and the object to be analyzed was the way in which information was perceived and processed, namely learning approaches (Marton & Säljö, Reference Marton and Säljö1976). As a result of the experiment, two learning approaches were identified, labeled Deep and Surface according to the observed student´s degree of engagement and cognitive effort.

In short, high-achievement students dealt with materials intending to understand their meaning and implications—Deep-level processing—, due to their high engagement with learning. Conversely, undergraduates exhibit low engagement with learning processed information by heart; recalling and reproducing it afterward without investing any effort in its comprehension—surface-level processing. Such dynamics prevented them from achieving satisfactory academic results.

Marton and Säljö’s (Reference Marton and Säljö1976) contributions were initially referred to as the processes involved in text comprehension and aimed at analyzing the specific meaning assigned to a particular text, principle, idea, and so on by a specific individual. As new findings were collected, studies were fatherly broadened by other authors, extending the notion of learning approaches to every learning academic situation. Moreover, not only the Deep and Surface approaches remained identified as an exclusive pair: A third type, named Achieving or Strategic was also added to the previous approaches (Biggs, Reference Biggs1979; Entwistle et al., Reference Entwistle, Hanley and Hounsell1979).

Each one of these three approaches is based on the students’ self-perceptions regarding their beliefs on their ways of studying and learning (Romero-Medina et al., Reference Romero-Medina, Hidalgo-Montesinos, González-Javier, Carrillo-Verdejo, Pedraja Linares, García-Sevilla and Pérez-Sánchez2013). Such self-perceptions involve motives and meta-strategies employed in diverse learning situations.

The Deep approach stands out as it is related to intrinsic motivation: It entails a sound interest in understanding learning materials as well as integrating new information with long-term knowledge (Zhao & Qin, Reference Zhao and Qin2021). On the contrary, the Surface approach involves memorizing and recalling information as the preferred way of learning, guided by extrinsic motivation (Cheung et al., Reference Cheung, Ho, Tsang and Lau2022). The Strategic approach would represent an intermediate position that leads students to adapt their behaviors to the requirements of the specific learning situation, either comprehending or memorizing content thus enhancing academic performance. Being strategic implies planning how to use the proper skills and strategies as a result of a realistic perception of the evaluation requirements (Entwistle, Reference Entwistle2001). This approach has been also named organized studying in more recent research (Asikainen & Katajavuori, Reference Asikainen and Katajavuori2022; Lindblom-Ylänne et al., Reference Lindblom-Ylänne, Parpala and Postareff2019). Such expression emphasizes the role of self-regulated learning linked to planning, time management, and industry (Parpala et al., Reference Parpala, Katajavuori, Haarala-Muhonen and Asikainen2021).

It is worth mentioning that, even though in general terms it is understood that some of the approaches predominate over the rest of them, expecting mixed profiles with more than one approach standing out is also feasible (Alsayed et al., Reference Alsayed, Alshammari, Pasay-an and Dator2021).

Studies have verified the association between academic achievement and learning approaches: Whilst it resulted positive for the Deep and Strategic types, it was found negative for the Surface one (Biggs et al., Reference Biggs, Kember and Leung2001; De la Fuente et al., Reference de la Fuente, Sander, Kauffman and Yilmaz Soylu2020; Sam, Reference Sam2020; Zamora-Menéndez et al., Reference Zamora-Menéndez, Gil Flores and de Besa Gutiérrez2020).

Learning Approaches and Educative Context

It is important to highlight that learning approaches let to assess teaching environments indirectly since the students’ motivation and strategies depend, to a great extent, on teaching and evaluation methods (Takase et al., Reference Takase, Imai, Niitani and Okada2019). So, using a Deep approach seems more likely in less regulated learning environments which, concurrently, promote evaluation methods requiring conceptual understanding and integration. Contrariwise, more regulated environments suit students preferring the Surface approach. They are prone to invest less time in learning. Consequently, they deal with high amounts of information without prioritizing understanding. This explains why they prefer multiple-choice tests (Biggs & Tang, Reference Biggs and Tang2011; Gandía Herrero & Romero Medina, Reference Gandía Herrero and Romero Medina2019). Distinctively, the Strategic approach—organized studying—is suitable to be used in any kind of learning environment; when combined with either the Deep or the Surface, it enables a better academic performance (Entwistle, Reference Entwistle2001).

Educational environments—involving teaching and evaluation methods—arise as a matter of interest in view of their influence on learning approaches. Hence, diverse types of environments have been taken into consideration by researchers, according to their different levels of proximity to students—i.e., each educational centre’s teaching context, the educational environment of each country, and the specific culture corresponding to each national identity. Dennehy (Reference Dennehy2015) has drawn attention to cultural differences across nations, underlining their distinctive educational cultures, which entail a wide variety of educational styles and educational history, actualized by means of different teaching methods as well as differences in the resources and procedures implemented by educational organizations.

Some studies reported statistically significant differences in learning approaches by country, and those differences were even more significant when distinguishing Western heritage from Asian countries (Dennehy, Reference Dennehy2015; Niles, Reference Niles1995). These results drive to describe distinct ways of interaction between the educational context and learning processes occurring in different educational cultures.

Present Study

Up to date, studies on learning approaches involve two major subjects deserving to be mentioned. On the one hand, instruments to assess the construct have been developed. Two widely used scales are available to be used in college students: The Revised Two Factor Study Process Questionnaire (R–SPQ–2F; Biggs et al., Reference Biggs, Kember and Leung2001), and the Approaches and Study Skills Inventory for Students (ASSIST; Tait et al., Reference Tait, Entwistle, McCune and Rust1998). The former measures only Deep and Surface approaches whereas the latter includes the measurement of the Strategic as well.

On the other hand, learning approaches have been analyzed from a micro-level perspective in different countries, identifying profiles of approaches in students from different majors in terms of their associations with academic performance (Bansal et al., Reference Bansal, Bansal and White2021; Freiberg-Hoffmann et al., Reference Freiberg-Hoffmann, Fernández-Liporace and Ledesma2017; Smarandache et al., Reference Smarandache, Maricutoiu, Ilie, Iancu and Mladenovici2022). Moreover, a thorough review of the literature of interest conducted on several databases—i.e., Elsevier, Springer, Taylor & Francis, APA PsycNET, Wiley—suggested that profiles have not been examined from a macro-level standpoint, taking into consideration each country´s educational culture. Bearing that in mind, this study was aimed at examining the influence of the cultural educational context on learning approaches profiles exhibited by undergraduates. Such analysis acquires importance in view that it has not been conducted before: It comprises the comparison between two Spanish speaker countries, one from Europe and the other from Latin America, specifically Spain and Argentina. Therefore, pointing out the main differences between the educative contexts that may have an impact on college students learning approaches in both regions appear relevant. For instance, whilst most Latin American countries assign on average between 2.5% and 3.5% of their gross domestic product to the educative system, the Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) member countries allocate 4.5%. This not only does affect teachers’ salaries but also the investment in Information and Communications Technology (ICT) and educational facilities. Investment in Investigation and Development (I+D) projects as well as academic mobility are also lower in Latin America. The latter is due to the scarce number of students applying for fellowships (2%) in comparison with their counterparts in Western Europe, the United States of America, Australia, and Canada (70%). Such a difference in percentages results in a brain drain in Latin American countries (López-Segrera, Reference López-Segrera2016). When specifically comparing the educative contexts, the gross attendance ratio in college is higher in Argentina (78.2%) than in Spain (65.1%). That can be partly explained by the admission systems in force: Whereas in Argentina all students can provisionally attend classes up to the time when they have to pass several courses to be finally admitted, Spain previews a limited admission according to the number of places available, selecting students by considering Grade Point Average (GPA) and an exam. Such difference entails more students in class—Argentina higher than Spain. That logically restricts teaching methods. Gender differences are reported as well: Spain shows 54% of women in higher education whereas Argentina reports 62% (García de Fanelli, Reference García de Fanelli2018; Motes & Osorio, Reference Motes and Osorio2020).

On the grounds of the distinctions above described, comparing psychological variables acquires importance, resulting in several cross-cultural studies analyzing such constructs in undergraduates from different countries. Their findings offered major information on the similarities and differences in the students’ profiles and on the internal structures of the tests used in each country (Freiberg-Hoffmann et al., Reference Freiberg-Hoffmann, Romero-Medina, Curione and Marôco2022; Martínez-Fernández & Vermunt, Reference Martínez-Fernández and Vermunt2013; Pastor Seller et al., Reference Pastor Seller, Farías Gramegna, Rujano Roque and Sáez González2019).

Regarding the existing theoretical and empirical background, it was examined whether high academic achievement profiles correspond to higher scores for Deep and Strategic approaches, and low scores for the Surface one. Thus, the goals for two separate studies were as follows: (a) Analyzing psychometric features of the ASSIST—which measures learning approaches—in terms of its internal structure, internal consistency, factorial invariance, external validity evidence as well as the dimensions’ scores and norms for samples composed of Psychology undergraduates from both countries; and, (b) analyzing and comparing learning approaches profiles in high and low performing Psychology students from both academic contexts—Spain and Argentina.

In order to bring clarity, the two studies were fully described separately—method, results, and discussion.

Study 1: ASSIST Questionnaire Psychometric Features

Method

Participants

The exploratory factor analysis and internal consistency study was conducted on a sample composed of 400 Psychology undergraduates (50% Argentina, 50% Spain;18.1% males, 81.9% females) between 18 and 51 years old (M age = 21.66; SD = 5). The ages of the Argentinean group were between 18 and 50 years old (M age = 21.94; SD = 5.04; 21.2% males, 78.8% females). In the Spanish group, ages ranged from 18 to 51 (M age = 21.40; SD = 4.97; 15% males, 85% females).

The sample used for the confirmatory factor analysis, the factorial invariance analysis, and the comparison of internal consistency indices was composed of 1,334 Psychology undergraduates (33.4% males, 66.6% females) from 18 to 61 years old (M age = 21.73; SD = 4.97). 41.7% of them were from Argentina (47.8% males, 52.2% females), with ages between 18 and 56 years old (M age = 22.7; SD = 5.10) whereas 58.3% were Spanish (23.2% males, 76.8% females) from 18 to 61 years old (M age = 21.01; SD = 4.76).

Instruments

The Spanish translation by Hidalgo-Montesinos et al. (Reference Hidalgo-Montesinos, Romero-Medina, González-Javier, García-Sevilla, Pérez-Sánchez, Carrillo-Verdejo, Díaz-Herrero, Godoy-Fernández, García-Montalvo, Pedraja, Chaparro, Bermejo-Bravo, Fernández-Ros and Peñaranda-Ortega2009) of the Approaches and Study Skills Inventory for Students (ASSIST; Tait et al., Reference Tait, Entwistle, McCune and Rust1998) was employed. It is composed of 18 statements responded to by a 5-option Likert scale. Three scales represent the approaches: Deep approach—Items 2, 6, 10, 12, 15, 17—, Surface—Items 1, 4, 8, 14, 16, 18—, and Strategic—Items 3, 5, 7, 9, 11, 13.

Each country gathered additional sociodemographic and academic information such as gender, age, grade point average, number of passed courses, and years that went by from college admission. The two latter variables were used to estimate an indicator of academic achievement.

Procedures

In both studies and in both countries, measures were taken in group sessions conducted in class. Participants volunteered and signed an informed consent where the study goals were explained. The anonymity was also notified and thoroughly preserved. Participant universities—Murcia and Buenos Aires—endorsed the study, allowed by their ethics committees.

Data Analysis

To perform the exploratory factor analysis, a parallel analysis according to Horn, and the minimum rank factor analysis were calculated in order to determine the number of factors to be retained. Results from the 95th percentile and higher were taken into consideration (Lim & Jahng, Reference Lim and Jahng2019). Then, an exploratory factor analysis with oblimin direct and Kaiser normalization was run. Items with loadings of .40 or higher in a unique factor and factorial simplicity indices higher than .50 were retained (Fleming & Merino-Soto, Reference Fleming and Merino-Soto2005).

The internal consistency was estimated by ordinal alpha coefficients.

Framed on the confirmatory factor analysis step, the resulting model was tested in terms of fit and was also compared with the fit of the model posed by the scale’s authors. To do so, the robust maximum likelihood method (RML), recommended for ordinal variables was employed (Kiliç & Doğan, Reference Kiliç and Doğan2021). The models’ fit was tested using the normed fit index (NFI), non-normed fit index (NNFI), and comparative fit index (CFI), taking values over .90 as acceptable. Root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) indices (RMSEA) and standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR) were employed as well, assuming values below.08 as adequate. The model parsimony was tested by the consistent Akaike information criterion (CAIC), with lower coefficients indicating a better fit and parsimony (Wang & Wang, Reference Wang and Wang2020).

The factorial invariance analysis used country, gender, and age as split variables. As for age, students were divided into two groups using the median as the cut-off point (20 years) since it is a statistical indicator non-sensitive to extreme values. Those groups were: Younger students (18 to 20 years old) and older undergraduates (21 to 61). The weighted least squares (WLS) method was employed since it is suitable to compare estimations between large samples, a low number of latent and observed variables, and items with multiple categories (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Millsap, West, Tein, Tanaka and Grimm2017). Three nested models with different restriction levels were tested. The invariance was interpreted with CFI and RMSEA indices since they are not sensitive to the sample size (van Zyl & ten Klooster, Reference van Zyl and ten Klooster2022). Moreover, those two indices are recommended for analyses with samples of different sizes and ordinal variables (Rojas Torres et al., Reference Rojas Torres, Rojas Rojas and Brizuela Rodríguez2018). Ordinal alphas were calculated for each country and compared via the Feldt formula (Feldt et al., Reference Feldt, Woodruff and Salih1987).

Academic achievement was the criterion variable used for the predictive validity evidence analysis. Since data related to this variable were gathered differently in both countries, they were analyzed separately. The grade point average reported by the students was used in the Spanish group. The ratio between the number of passed courses and the years that went by from college admission was calculated as an estimator in the case of Argentina. Both measures were suggested as valid for analyzing academic achievement (Rodríguez-Ayán & Ruiz-Díaz, Reference Rodríguez-Ayán and Ruíz-Díaz2011; Ruiz et al., Reference Ruiz, Ruiz and Odstrcil2007). As for measuring learning approaches, the summated rating method was used: it is the addition of each response to the items in order to calculate the scales’ total score (American Psychological Association, 2015).

Regarding norms, a scores’ mean-differences analysis between countries was run for the three dimensions of the scale as a first step. Later, scores were grouped by tertiles—High, Medium, Low—for each dimension—Deep, Surface, Strategic—and country.

Factor 10.9 (Lorenzo-Seva & Ferrando, Reference Lorenzo-Seva and Ferrando2019) and SIMLOAD (Fleming & Merino-Soto, Reference Fleming and Merino-Soto2005) were the statistical software packages used to calculate the exploratory factor analysis. LISREL 8.8 was employed for the confirmatory factor and the factorial invariance analyses. Ordinal alpha coefficients were compared via the AlphaTest package (Merino-Soto & Lautenschlager, Reference Merino-Soto and Lautenschlager2003). The mean differences analysis and the norms calculation were conducted with SPSS 21.

Results

Exploratory Factor Analysis

The parallel analysis suggested the extraction of three factors. Therefore, three latent variables were isolated in the exploratory factor analysis. Only Item #11 was eliminated. So, the model retained 17 out of the 18 analyzed items, explaining 68.4% of the common variance. Every factor explained similar percentages of variance, also obtaining optimal internal consistency indicators (see Table 1).

Table 1. ASSIST. Exploratory Factor Analysis

Note. FDI = factor determinacy index; SR = sensitivity ratio; EPTD = expected percentage of true differences.

Confirmatory Factor Analysis

The 3-factor resulting model was examined by confirmatory factor analysis (see Figure 1) and compared with the original theoretical model.

Figure 1. ASSIST. 3-Factor resulting Model.

Both models achieved adequate fit indices (see Table 2). NFI, NNFI, and CFI indices were higher than .90 for both models whereas RMSEA was lower than .08. SRMR reached an adequate value for the model which excluded item 11 whereas it was slightly higher than .08 for the model including item 11; that implied a better fit for the former model. As for CAIC coefficients, the resulting model got a lower value, indicating its higher parsimony when compared with its theoretical rival.

Table 2. ASSIST. Model Fit and Model Comparison (Resulting vs. Original)

The above-mentioned coefficients pointed out the superiority of the resulting model over the theoretically hypothesized. Accordingly, the following analyses were conducted exclusively on the former.

Factorial Invariance Analysis

The factorial invariance of the resulting model was tested by comparing samples split by country, gender, and age. Different restriction levels were applied (configural, metric, scale). Results verified the model´s invariance for all the constriction levels taken into consideration (configural, metric, scale) and for every segmentation (country, gender, and age) (see Table 3). When comparing CFI and RMSEA indices obtained for the least restricted model with those found for the more constrained models, differences were non-significant. Such a result favors the hypothesis stating the metric invariance of the scale in the two samples compared in the study.

Table 3. ASSIST. Factorial Invariance Analysis

Comparison of Internal Consistency Indices

Internal consistency indices for the Spanish and the Argentinean samples were optimal and did not verify statistically significant differences (p > .01) (see Table 4). Such a finding implies an equivalent homogeneity level of the items composing each dimension for both countries.

Table 4. ASSIST. Internal Consistency for Dimensions. Argentina vs. Spain Comparison

Predictive Validity Evidence Analysis regarding Academic Achievement

The influence of learning approaches on academic achievement was analyzed in both countries’ samples. The Spanish sample verified the significant influence of all approaches, positive for Deep and Strategic, and negative for the Surface one. A significant effect for the Strategic and Surface approaches was found in the Argentinean sample—proven positive for the former and negative for the latter (see Table 5). The Strategic approach is the one that contributes to explaining academic achievement with the highest percentage of variance in both countries.

Table 5. Regression Models for Learning Approaches and Academic Achievement by Country

The equivalence of scores by country was tested in order to make a decision on the pertinence of calculating separate norms. Scores differed significantly for the three scales, and the Spanish sample obtained higher scores every time. Thus, separate norms were required (see Table 6).

Table 6. Differences in approaches by Country

As above mentioned, data were split into tertiles, classifying the approaches scores as Low, Medium, and High (Table 7).

Table 7. ASSIST. Norms for Argentina and Spain

Discussion

This first study has been developed to analyze the ASSIST’s psychometric features in college students from Spain and Argentina, therefore guaranteeing its proper functioning in both populations. Results are examined below.

Analyzing the ASSIST´s internal structure in the whole sample was the first step. The parallel analysis suggested extracting three factors, and that was consistent with the original theoretical model (Tait et al., Reference Tait, Entwistle, McCune and Rust1998). Such 3-factor model, isolated in the exploratory factor analysis, explained 68.4% of the common variance. It retained all the items except for one, #11. Its malfunctioning could be due to it was the unique negative statement in the original version—“I don’t find it at all difficult to motivate myself”. Standards in psychometrics recommend avoiding negative clauses since they usually introduce confusion, thus distorting responses (Pais et al., Reference Pais, Silva, Guimarães, Povo, Coelho, Silva-Pereira, Lourinho, Ferreira and Severo2016; Pham et al., Reference Pham, Besanko and Devitt2018). The remaining items obtained loadings and factorial simplicity indices indicating their high representativity and exclusivity regarding the factor they loaded on (Boateng et al., Reference Boateng, Neilands, Frongillo, Melgar-Quiñonez and Young2018).

Additionally, the H replicability indices, higher than .80, indicated that those retained items represented factors properly.

Consequently, it is assumed that the latent variables were accurately defined; hence, it is likely for them to be replicated in further studies (Ferrando & Lorenzo-Seva, Reference Ferrando and Lorenzo-Seva2018).

Factor determinacy index (FDI), higher than .90, marginal reliability indices higher than .80, sensitivity ratio (SR) higher than 2, and expected percentage of true differences (EPTD) higher than 90% drive to state that the scale is suitable to be employed in the professional field (Ferrando & Lorenzo-Seva, Reference Ferrando and Lorenzo-Seva2018).

The low inter-factorial loadings lead to asserting the parsimony of the structure. Given their independence, each factor explains different aspects of the concept, avoiding redundant information (Watkins, Reference Watkins2018).

The comparison between the original theoretical model and the one resulting from the exploratory factor analysis confirmed the better fit and parsimony of the latter.

The metric invariance of the resulting model was verified not only by country but also by gender and age, generalizing its stability across countries—Spain and Argentina—, genders, and age groups; which guarantees unbiased measures (Davidov et al., Reference Davidov, Schmidt, Billiet and Meuleman2018).

Regarding the scores’ internal consistency, optimal indices were achieved for the three dimensions in both countries. Such indices were compared by country, without verifying significant differences; the effect sizes were nil or small (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988).

The predictive validity evidence analysis, using academic achievement as a criterion, verified the positive influence of the Strategic approach and the negative influence of the Surface approach on achievement. The Deep approach only explained academic performance in the Spanish sample, with no significance in the Argentinean one. Both results run in line with findings from previous studies (Entwistle et al., Reference Entwistle, McCune and Tait2013; Öhrstedt & Lindfors, Reference Öhrstedt and Lindfors2018; Sam, Reference Sam2020). It has been stated that the Deep approach solely is insufficient to predict academic achievement. The Strategic approach is also required. In other words, a proper conceptual understanding is not always enough to achieve a higher performance; the use of adequate study methods usually makes a substantial impact (Karagiannopoulou & Milienos, Reference Karagiannopoulou and Milienos2015).

Finally, the scores equivalence by country was examined. Significant differences were verified for every learning approach score. Therefore, they should be interpreted differently according to the country (American Educational Research Association [AERA], American Psychological Association [APA], & National Council on Measurement in Education [NCME], 2014). Consequently, separate norms were calculated. They can be useful for practitioners in the educational professional field who are interested in assessing learning approaches from Argentina or Spain by means of the ASSIST.

This study introduced a valid and reliable scale to assess learning approaches in undergraduates from Spain and Argentina. It is expected that educational psychologists will find it useful, regarding the extra benefit of regional norms enabling accurate interpretations according to the country.

Study 2: Learning Approaches Profiles in Low and High-Achievement undergraduates from Spain and Argentina

Method

Participants

The sample analyzed in the confirmatory factor study—Study 1—was also employed here. Thus, the same informed consent and the ethics committee endorsement apply.

Instruments

The version of ASSIST obtained in Study 1 was also used.

Procedures

In order to develop learning approaches profiles of Spanish and Argentinean low and high-performance undergraduates, categories corresponding to the three learning approaches dimensions resulting from Study 1 —High, Medium, Low—were employed. Besides, the samples were split by academic achievement into two categories—High and Low. These categorized variables were employed to test how the levels in the approaches entail profiles corresponding to academic success or academic failure. Following Altman (Reference Altman2014), tertiles were used in order to configure three categories of approaches, proceeding in the same way as for the norms; the median was employed to split the samples into halves by academic achievement. Then, a latent class analysis was conducted using the three dimensions of learning approaches and academic achievement. The Bayesian Information criteria (BIC), the sample size adjusted BIC (ssaBIC), and the Akaike information criteria (AIC) indices were employed to interpret the models fit; in all these cases, low values indicate a better fit. The Vuong-Lo-Mendel-Rubin (VLMR) and the Lo-Mendell-Rubin (LMR) statistics were also taken into consideration; significant values imply that models with a higher number of classes fit better (Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Qualter and Humphrey2019; Schreiber, Reference Schreiber2017; Weller et al., Reference Weller, Bowen and Faubert2020).

Data Analysis

The latent class analysis was run via the Mplus 7 software.

Results

The latent class analysis was conducted as follows: (a) The levels of the three separate learning approaches obtained in Study 1 were employed—High, Medium, Low—; (b) academic achievement was split into High and Low by the 50th percentile. Models with 1, 2, and 3 classes were tested independently for both countries’ samples.

The 2-class model obtained the best-fit indices for both countries (see Table 8). BIC and ssaBIC coefficients were the lowest. The AIC index was the lowest also for Argentina. However, the Spanish sample obtained lower values when compared to the 1-class model and higher when compared to the 3-class model. Nevertheless, the 2-class model was judged as suitable for both countries since BIC and ssaBIC are recommended as more precise indicators because they are adjusted according to the sample size. Furthermore, VLRM and LRT indices for the 2-class model verified a significant improvement (p < .01) when compared to the 3-class model. Finally, the 2-class model showed the highest theoretical consistency compared to the 1 and 3-class models. Therefore, the 2-class model was retained.

Table 8. Latent Class Analysis. Fit Indices

Note. LL = Loglikelihood; BIC = Bayesian Information criteria; ssaBIC = sample size adjusted BIC; AIC = Akaike information criteria; VLRM = Vuong-Lo-Mendel-Rubin; LMR = Lo-Mendell-Rubin.

As for the 2-class model interpretation in the Spanish sample, both classes showed a similar size (50%). When analyzing the likelihood of response to the indicators of each latent variable, Class 1 grouped undergraduates with a low Surface approach and high use of both Deep and Strategic approaches as well as high academic achievement. In opposition, Class 2 included undergraduates reporting a medium-high Surface approach, low use of Deep and Strategic approaches as well as low achievement (see Table 9).

Table 9. Learning Styles and Academic Achievement Class Analysis by Country

Regarding the Argentinean sample, Class 1 (58.1%) comprised more individuals than Class 2 (41.9%). Concerning the likelihood of belonging to each class according to the type of responses, slight differences with the Spanish sample emerged. On that account, Class 1 joined students with low Surface approach, medium-high Deep and Strategic approaches, and high academic achievement. Class 2 was composed of undergraduates with a high Surface approach, low Deep and Strategic approaches, and low academic achievement (Table 9).

Discussion

Study 2 analyzed learning approaches and academic achievement profiles. In both groups of undergraduates—Spain and Argentina—, high academic achievement corresponded to the most frequent use of the Deep and Strategic approaches and the less frequent use of the Surface one. In opposition, a low academic achievement matched with more frequent usage of the Surface approach and less preference for both Deep and Strategic approaches. Not only the high-achievement but also the low-achievement profile matched with the ones reported in previous studies referred to Psychology undergraduates from different countries (De Santis, Reference De Santis2018; Öhrstedt & Lindfors, Reference Öhrstedt and Lindfors2016).

When analyzing the above-reported findings in order to draw attention to the educational culture of each country, it is worthy to describe some differences between the Faculties of Psychology of Spain and Argentina examined in this research. Even though both are public, among the main differences the number of freshmen per year-term is rather different in the first place. Whilst in Spain admissions are limited depending on three application criteria—the students’ Grade Point Average, an exam as well as the number of places available—, vacancies are unlimited in Argentina requiring passing several courses instead. That is the reason why the Argentinean higher-education system involves a higher number of students compared to the Spanish one. Besides, most Spanish undergraduates are full-time students. Conversely, most Argentinean students have a job,—either part-time or full-time—while attending their courses. Additionally, Spanish universities give plenty of support to students and teachers in terms of adequate facilities and resources whereas Argentinean universities give limited assistance either to teachers or to undergraduates.

However, despite the differences above described, findings lead to state that even in different educational cultures good achievement is equally linked to approaches and learning strategies of a Deep and Strategic type—organized studying. That implies undergraduates´ learning approaches profile in a specific major does not differ when analyzed from a macro-level perspective in two different educational cultures (Spain-Argentina).

Such a conclusion is similar to the one obtained in micro-level studies, identifying similar profiles in other majors and verifying the same features, no matter the undergraduate’s gender, age, or other socio-demographic characteristics (Bansal et al., Reference Bansal, Bansal and White2021; Chonkar et al., Reference Chonkar, Ha, Chu, Ng, Lim, Ee, Ng and Tan2018; Joshi et al., Reference Joshi, Contractor, Ganjiwale and Patel2021).

However, as previously stated, profiles remained stable in both countries despite the differences reported in Study 1. In other words, the high-achievers profile is similar in both cultures whereas the degree of preference to use each approach is rather different. As above mentioned, such a high-achievers profile was also found in other majors: That result would contribute to strengthening evidence favoring the hypothesis stating that high-achievers show higher use of the Deep and Strategic approaches and low use of the Surface approach. However, further analyses with larger and wider samples, including undergraduates from other majors and countries arise as mandatory in order to support such an assertion.

Taking into consideration the importance of fostering both Deep and Strategic learning approaches in undergraduates, several recommendations useful for teachers and students contained in the literature of interest are here presented. From the students’ viewpoint, active participation by means of posing questions, reasoning analogically, designing concept maps, employing collaborative learning, using critical thinking, asking for feedback, and learning from exposition to the experience seems the most preferable way to reach successful learning. Boosting all those skills requires that the teacher promotes a class climate able to enhance trial and error learning, being benefitted from the peers’ previous knowledge, planning group activities, using the teaching-learning Socratic method, giving proper feedback, using evaluation methods aimed at enabling reflective thinking and conceptual integration in undergraduates (Azer et al., Reference Azer, Guerrero and Walsh2013; Biggs & Tang, Reference Biggs and Tang2011; Gandía Herrero & Romero Medina, Reference Gandía Herrero and Romero Medina2019).

The above-mentioned suggestions are only a few among all the possible actions that could be taken by teachers and students in order to achieve long-lasting and deep learning. In view of the differences between the Spanish and the Argentinean educational contexts, it is important to bear in mind that such actions are not to be taken all at once but according to diverse factors such as institutional resources, type of major, and number of students per class among others.

General Discussion

To sum up, a cross-cultural study focused on learning and study approaches used by Psychology undergraduates compared two higher education national cultures—one European—Spain—and the other, Latin American—Argentina.

Study 1

Study 1 obtained a version of the Approaches and Study Skills Inventory for Students (ASSIST) suitable to be used in individual assessments of learning approaches in higher education, either in Spain or Argentina. The psychometric analyses conducted replicated the 3-factor model posed by the scale’s authors (Tait et al., Reference Tait, Entwistle, McCune and Rust1998), with optimal internal consistency indices obtained in both samples. Differences in the approaches’ scores between countries suggested that they should be interpreted using separate norms.

Predictive validity evidence using academic achievement as the criterion showed similar behavior in both countries: the Surface approach as a negative influence and the Strategic, as positively affecting. The Deep approach only verified a significant and positive effect on academic achievement for the Spanish sample. Such a finding suggests that Deep approach itself is not enough to improve achievement; such enhancement seems feasible exclusively when the Strategic approach is added to the equation. In other words, academic achievement requires organization and a planned effort in the use of study strategies: Understanding contents is not enough; they should be properly fixed to obtain good results in evaluations.

Study 2

Study 2 examined learning approaches in undergraduates with high and low academic achievement in both countries, by means of latent class analysis. As a result, a 2-class model was obtained for both samples: Class 1, comprising high-achievement-low-Surface-approach-high-Deep-high-Strategic-approaches students; Class 2, including low-achievement-medium/high-Surface-approach-low-Deep-approach-low-Strategic-approach. The similarity between learning approaches profiles in undergraduates from both countries leads to conclude that the differences between educational contexts are not that important to affect learning processes and learning strategies.

Regarding the limitations of this research, only predictive validity evidence was examined whereas other sources of external validity should be analyzed fatherly—i.e., convergent validity evidence related to other scales assessing theoretically related variables. Besides, samples composed exclusively of Psychology undergraduates restrict generalization; so, examining learning approaches in students from other majors is mandatory. Plus, academic achievement was estimated differently according to the sample. Such criteria must be unified in further research.

This work has contributed in three different ways: (a) It showed that, even when differences between learning approaches by country/culture were verified, academic success was positively related to Deep and Strategic approach, and negatively to the Surface one; (b) it has adapted and analyzed the technical features of a scale to assess learning approaches, making it suitable for its use in undergraduates; (c) learning approaches profiles linked to high and low achievement were identified. Such three contributions entail interest from two different standpoints. One is theoretical since learning approaches profiles previously hypothesized as related to academic success and failure were verified. The other involves implications for the professional field. Assessing individual profiles with ASSIST in order to compare them with the ideal successful profile will let more accurate and tailored planning of teaching and evaluation methods.

Footnotes

Funding Statement: This study was developed with the support of a funding grant from the Agencia Nacional de Promoción de la Investigación, el Desarrollo Tecnológico y la Innovación (PICT–2020–SERIEA–00087) as well as another funding grant (PIP #11220200100352CO) and an external postdoctoral fellowship from the Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET).

Conflicts of Interest: None.

Data Sharing: The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Authorship credit: Conceptualization by Freiberg-Hoffmann, Romero-Medina, López-Fernández and Fernández-Liporace; methodology by Freiberg-Hoffmann; data collection by Freiberg-Hoffmann and Romero-Medina; visualization by Freiberg-Hoffmann; writing-original draft by Freiberg-Hoffmann, Romero-Medina, López-Fernández and Fernández-Liporace; writing-rewriting and editing by Freiberg-Hoffmann, Romero-Medina and Fernández-Liporace.

References

American Educational Research Association, American Psychological Association, & National Council on Measurement in Education. (2014). Standards for educational and psychological testing. American Educational Research Association. American Psychological Association, & National Council on Measurement in Education.Google Scholar
Alsayed, S., Alshammari, F., Pasay-an, E., & Dator, W. L. (2021). Investigating the learning approaches of students in nursing education. Journal of Taibah University Medical Sciences, 16(1), 4349. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtumed.2020.10.008Google ScholarPubMed
Altman, D. G. (2014). Categorizing continuous variables. Wiley StatsRef: Statistics Reference Online. Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118445112.stat04857Google Scholar
American Psychological Association (2015). APA dictionary of psychology. (2nd ed.). Retrieved September 9, 2022, from https://dictionary.apa.org/Google Scholar
Asikainen, H., & Katajavuori, N. (2022). First-year experience in the COVID-19 situation and the association between students’ approaches to learning, study-related burnout and experiences of online studying. Social Sciences, 11, Article 390. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci11090390Google Scholar
Azer, S. A., Guerrero, A. P. S., & Walsh, A. (2013). Enhancing learning approaches: Practical tips for students and teachers. Medical Teacher, 35(6), 433443. https://doi.org/10.3109/0142159x.2013.775413CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bansal, S., Bansal, M., & White, S. (2021). Association between learning approaches and medical student academic progression during preclinical training. Advances in Medical Education and Practice, 12, 13431351. https://doi.org/10.2147/amep.s329204CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biggs, J. (1979). Individual differences in study processes and the quality of learning outcomes. Higher Education, 8, 381394. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01680526Google Scholar
Biggs, J. (2005). Calidad del aprendizaje universitario [Teaching for quality learning at University] (P. Manzano, Trans.). Narcea. (Original work published in 1999).Google Scholar
Biggs, J., Kember, D., & Leung, D. Y. P. (2001). The revised two-factor study process questionnaire: R–SPQ–2F. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 71(1), 133149. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709901158433CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biggs, J., & Tang, C. (2011). Teaching for quality learning at university. Mc Graw Hill.Google Scholar
Boateng, G. O., Neilands, T. B., Frongillo, E. A., Melgar-Quiñonez, H. R., & Young, S. L. (2018). Best practices for developing and validating scales for health, social, and behavioral research: A primer. Frontiers in Public Health, 6, Article 149. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2018.00149CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheung, K., Ho, C. S. W., Tsang, H., & Lau, E. (2022). Comparing the learning approaches of transfer students and direct entrants in an Asian higher education context. Sustainability, 14, Article 2523. https://doi.org/10.3390/su14052523Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2?>nd?> Ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Chonkar, S. P., Ha, T. C., Chu, S. S. H., Ng, A. X., Lim, M. L. S., Ee, T. X., Ng, M. J., & Tan, K. H. (2018). The predominant learning approaches of medical students. BMC Medical Education, 18(1), Article 17. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-018-1122-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidov, E., Schmidt, P., Billiet, J., & Meuleman, B. (2018). Cross-cultural analysis. Methods and applications. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315537078Google Scholar
de la Fuente, J., Sander, P., Kauffman, D. F., & Yilmaz Soylu, M. (2020). Differential effects of self- vs. external-regulation on learning approaches, academic achievement, and satisfaction in undergraduate students. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, Article 543884. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.543884Google ScholarPubMed
Dennehy, E. (2015). Learning approaches and cultural influences: A comparative study of Confucian and western-heritage students. Journal of Further and Higher Education, 39(6), 818838. https://doi.org/10.1080/0309877X.2013.869561Google Scholar
De Santis, K. K. (2018, April 2018). Learning approaches in the Research Methods module in psychology. Resonanz. https://blogs.uni-bremen.de/resonanz/2018/04/20/psychology/Google Scholar
Entwistle, N. (2001). Styles of learning and approaches to studying in higher education. Kybernetes, 30(5/6), 593603. http://doi.org/10.1108/03684920110391823CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Entwistle, N., Hanley, M., & Hounsell, D. (1979). Identifying distinctive approaches to studying. Higher Education, 8(4), 365380. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf01680525CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Entwistle, N. J., McCune, V., & Tait, H., (2013). Approaches and Study Skills Inventory for Students (ASSIST). https://www.researchgate.net/publication/50390092_Approaches_to_learning_and_studying_inventory_ASSIST_3rd_editionGoogle Scholar
Feldt, L. S., Woodruff, D. J., & Salih, F. A. (1987). Statistical inference for coefficient alpha. Applied Psychological Measurement, 11, 93103. http://doi.org/10.1177/014662168701100107Google Scholar
Ferrando, P. J., & Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2018). Assessing the quality and appropriateness of factor solutions and factor score estimates in exploratory item factor analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 78, 762780. http://doi.org/10.1177/0013164417719308Google ScholarPubMed
Fleming, J. S., & Merino-Soto, C. (2005). Medidas de simplicidad y ajuste factorial: Un enfoque para la evaluación de escalas construidas factorialmente [Simplicity measures and factorial fit: An approach to the evaluation of factorially constructed scales]. Revista de Psicología PUCP, 23(2), 250266. https://doi.org/10.18800/psico.200502.002Google Scholar
Freiberg-Hoffmann, A., Fernández-Liporace, M., & Ledesma, R. (2017). Enfoques de aprendizaje en estudiantes universitarios de Buenos Aires [Learning approaches in university students from Buenos Aires]. Pensando Psicología, 13(21), 1932.Google Scholar
Freiberg-Hoffmann, A., Romero-Medina, A., Curione, K., & Marôco, J. (2022). Adaptación y validación transcultural al español del University Student Engagement Inventory [Cross-cultural adaptation and validation of the University Student Engagement Inventory into Spanish]. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 54, 187-195. https://doi.org/10.14349/rlp.2022.v54.21Google Scholar
Gandía Herrero, P., & Romero Medina, A. (2019). La exigencia cognitiva en los exámenes tipo test en contexto universitario y su relación con los enfoques de aprendizaje, la autorregulación, los métodos docentes y el rendimiento académico [The cognitive requirement in the multiple-choice exams in the university context and its relationship with learning approaches, self-regulation, teaching methods and academic performance]. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education, 9(3), 177187. https://doi.org/10.30552/ejihpe.v9i3.333Google Scholar
García de Fanelli, A. (2018). Panorama de la educación superior en Iberoamérica a través de los indicadores de la Red Indices [Overview of higher education in Ibero-America through the indicators of the Indices Network]. Red Iberoamericana de Indicadores de Educación Superior y Observatorio Iberoamericano de la Ciencia, la Tecnología y la Sociedad. http://www.redindices.org/attachments/article/85/Panorama%20de%20la%20educaci%C3%B3n%20superior%20iberoamericana%20versi%C3%B3n%20Octubre%202018.pdfGoogle Scholar
Hidalgo-Montesinos, M., Romero-Medina, A., González-Javier, F., García-Sevilla, J., Pérez-Sánchez, M., Carrillo-Verdejo, E., Díaz-Herrero, A., Godoy-Fernández, C., García-Montalvo, C., Pedraja, M., Chaparro, M., Bermejo-Bravo, F., Fernández-Ros, , & Peñaranda-Ortega, M. (2009, September 22–24). Herramientas de evaluación de los estilos de aprendizaje en universitarios: Traducción y adaptación del cuestionario ASSIST [Tools for evaluating learning styles in university students: Translation and adaptation of the ASSIST questionnaire] [Conference session]. IV Jornadas sobre el Espacio Europeo de Educación Superior: “El Espacio Europeo de Educación Superior en el horizonte del 2010”, Murcia, Spain.Google Scholar
Joshi, A. S., Contractor, J. B., Ganjiwale, J. D., & Patel, M. H. (2021). Identification of predominant learning approaches in medical students using ASSIST in an Indian medical school: A cross-sectional survey. Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research, 15, JC01–JC06. https://doi.org/10.7860/jcdr/2021/49689.15626Google Scholar
Karagiannopoulou, E., & Milienos, F. S. (2015). Testing two path models to explore relationships between students’ experiences of the teaching–learning environment, approaches to learning and academic achievement. Educational Psychology, 35(1), 2652. http://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2014.895800Google Scholar
Kiliç, A. F., & Doğan, N. (2021). Comparison of confirmatory factor analysis estimation methods on mixed-format data. International Journal of Assessment Tools in Education, 8(1), 2137. https://doi.org/10.21449/ijate.782351Google Scholar
Lim, S., & Jahng, S. (2019). Determining the number of factors using parallel analysis and its recent variants. Psychological Methods, 24(4), 452467. http://doi.org/10.1037/met0000230CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindblom-Ylänne, S., Parpala, A., & Postareff, L. (2019). What constitutes the surface approach to learning in the light of new empirical evidence? Studies in Higher Education, 44, 21832195. http://doi.org/10.1080/03075079.2018.1482267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Y., Millsap, R. E., West, S. G., Tein, J.-Y., Tanaka, R., & Grimm, K. J. (2017). Testing measurement invariance in longitudinal data with ordered-categorical measures. Psychological Methods, 22(3), 486506. http://doi.org/10.1037/met0000075Google ScholarPubMed
López-Segrera, F. L. (2016). Educación superior comparada: Tendencias mundiales y de América Latina y Caribe [Comparative higher education: Worldwide trends and of Latin America and the Caribbean]., Avaliação: Revista Da Avaliação Da Educação Superior (Campinas) 21(1), 1332. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1414-40772016000100002Google Scholar
Lorenzo-Seva, U., & Ferrando, P. J. (2019). Manual of the Program FACTOR. Department de Psicologia Universitat Rovira I Virgili. http://psico.fcep.urv.es/utilitats/factor/Google Scholar
Martínez-Fernández, J. R., & Vermunt, J. D. (2013). A cross-cultural analysis of the patterns of learning and academic performance of Spanish and Latin-American undergraduates. Studies in Higher Education, 40(2), 278295. https://doi.org/10.1080/03075079.2013.823934CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marton, F., & Säljö, R. (1976). On qualitative differences in learning: I. Outcome and process. Symposium: Learning processes and strategies. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 46, 411. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.1976.tb02980.xGoogle Scholar
Merino-Soto, C., & Lautenschlager, G. (2003). Comparación estadística de la confiabilidad alfa de Cronbach: Aplicaciones en la medición educacional y psicológica [Statistical comparison of the Cronbach ́s alpha reliability: Applications in the educational and psychological measurement]. Revista de Psicología de la Universidad de Chile, 12(2), 127136. https://doi.org/10.5354/0719-0581.2012.17668Google Scholar
Motes, N. & Osorio, L. (2020). Rasgos de la educación superior en Iberoamérica a través de la Red Indices. Red Iberoamericana de Indicadores de Educación Superior. http://www.redindices.org/novedades/134-rasgos-de-la-educacion-superior-en-iberoamerica-a-traves-de-la-red-indicesGoogle Scholar
Niles, F. S. (1995). Cultural differences in learning motivation and learning strategies: A comparison of overseas and Australian students at an Australian university. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 19(3), 369385. https://doi.org/10.1016/0147-1767(94)00025-SGoogle Scholar
Öhrstedt, M., & Lindfors, P. (2016). Linkages between approaches to learning, perceived stress and expected and actual academic outcomes among first-semester psychology students. Journal of Further and Higher Education, 42(1), 116129. https://doi.org/10.1080/0309877X.2016.1206856Google Scholar
Öhrstedt, M., & Lindfors, P. (2018). First-semester students’ capacity to predict academic achievement as related to approaches to learning. Journal of Further and Higher Education, 43(10), 14201432. https://doi.org/10.1080/0309877X.2018.1490950CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pais, J., Silva, A., Guimarães, B., Povo, A., Coelho, E., Silva-Pereira, F., Lourinho, I., Ferreira, M. A., & Severo, M. (2016). Do item-writing flaws reduce examinations psychometric quality? BMC Research Notes, 9(1), Article 399. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-016-2202-4Google ScholarPubMed
Parpala, A., Katajavuori, N., Haarala-Muhonen, A., & Asikainen, H. (2021). How did students with different learning profiles experience ‘normal’ and online teaching situation during COVID–19 Spring? Social Sciences, 10 , Article 337. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci10090337CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pastor Seller, E., Farías Gramegna, A. J., Rujano Roque, R., & Sáez González, R. (2019). Social and academic profile of the university student of social work a comparative study in Spain, Argentina, and Venezuela. Social Work Education, 39(6), 765784. https://doi.org/10.1080/02615479.2019.1653838Google Scholar
Petersen, K. J., Qualter, P., & Humphrey, N. (2019). The application of latent class analysis for investigating population child mental health: A systematic review. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 1214. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01214CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pham, H., Besanko, J., & Devitt, P. (2018). Examining the impact of specific types of item-writing flaws on student performance and psychometric properties of the multiple choice question [Version 1]. MedEdPublish, 7(4), Article 225. https://doi.org/10.15694/mep.2018.0000225.1Google Scholar
Rodríguez-Ayán, M. N., & Ruíz-Díaz, M. A. (2011). Indicadores de rendimiento de estudiantes universitarios: Calificaciones versus créditos acumulados [University students’ academic achievement indicators: Grades versus cumulative credits]. Revista de Educación, 355, 467492. https://doi.org/10-4438/1988-592X-RE-2011-355-033Google Scholar
Rojas Torres, L., Rojas Rojas, G., & Brizuela Rodríguez, A. (2018). The use of measurement invariance with dichotomous variables as evidence of validity. Evaluar, 18(2), 4558. https://doi.org/10.35670/1667-4545.v18.n2.20807Google Scholar
Romero-Medina, A., Hidalgo-Montesinos, M., González-Javier, F., Carrillo-Verdejo, E., Pedraja Linares, M. J., García-Sevilla, J., Pérez-Sánchez, M. A. (2013). Enfoques de aprendizaje en estudiantes universitarios: Comparación de resultados con los cuestionarios ASSIST y R–SPQ–2F [Learning approaches in university students: Comparison of results with the ASSIST and R–SPQ–2F questionnaires]. Revista de Investigación Educativa, 31(2), 375391. https://doi.org/10.6018/rie.31.2.151851Google Scholar
Ruiz, E., Ruiz, G. A., & Odstrcil, M. (2007). Metodología para realizar el seguimiento académico de alumnos universitarios [Methodology for academic monitoring of university students]. Revista Iberoamericana de Educación, 42(3), 19. https://rieoei.org/historico/deloslectores/1590Ruiz.pdfGoogle Scholar
Sam, L. (2020). Relationship between learning approaches and academic achievement of accounting education students. International Journal of Scientific and Research Publications, 10(7), 919924. http://doi.org/10.29322/IJSRP.10.07.2020.p103103Google Scholar
Schreiber, J. B. (2017). Latent class analysis: An example for reporting results. Research in Social and Administrative Pharmacy, 13(6), 11961201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sapharm.2016.11.011Google ScholarPubMed
Smarandache, I. G., Maricutoiu, L. P., Ilie, M. D., Iancu, D. E., & Mladenovici, V. (2022). Students’ approach to learning: Evidence regarding the importance of the interest-to-effort ratio. Higher Education Research & Development, 41(2), 546561. https://doi.org/10.1080/07294360.2020.1865283CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tait, H., Entwistle, N. J., & McCune, V. (1998). ASSIST. A reconceptualization of the Approaches to Studying Inventory. In Rust, C. (Ed.), Improving students as learners (pp. 262271). Oxford Brookes University.Google Scholar
Takase, M., Imai, T., Niitani, M., & Okada, M. (2019). Teaching context contributing to nursing students’ adoption of a deep approach to learning. Journal of Professional Nursing, 35, 379388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.profnurs.2019.04.006Google ScholarPubMed
van Zyl, L. E., & ten Klooster, P. M. (2022). Exploratory structural equation modeling: Practical guidelines and tutorial with a convenient online tool for Mplus. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, Article 795672. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.795672Google ScholarPubMed
Wang, J., & Wang, X. (2020). Structural equation modeling: Applications using Mplus. Willey.Google Scholar
Watkins, M. W. (2018). Exploratory factor analysis: A guide to best practice. Journal of Black Psychology, 44(3), 219246. https://doi.org/10.1177/0095798418771807Google Scholar
Weller, B. E., Bowen, N. K., & Faubert, S. J. (2020). Latent class analysis: A guide to best practice. Journal of Black Psychology, 46(4), 287311. https://doi.org/10.1177/0095798420930932Google Scholar
Zamora-Menéndez, Á., Gil Flores, J., & de Besa Gutiérrez, M.R. (2020). Enfoques de aprendizaje y perspectiva temporal: Persistencia en estudiantes universitarios [Learning approaches, time perspective and persistence in university students]. Educación XX1, 23(2), 1739. https://doi.org/10.5944/educXX1.25552Google Scholar
Zhao, J., & Qin, Y. (2021). Perceived teacher autonomy support and students’ deep learning: The mediating role of self-efficacy and the moderating role of perceived peer support. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 652796. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.652796Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. ASSIST. Exploratory Factor Analysis

Figure 1

Figure 1. ASSIST. 3-Factor resulting Model.

Figure 2

Table 2. ASSIST. Model Fit and Model Comparison (Resulting vs. Original)

Figure 3

Table 3. ASSIST. Factorial Invariance Analysis

Figure 4

Table 4. ASSIST. Internal Consistency for Dimensions. Argentina vs. Spain Comparison

Figure 5

Table 5. Regression Models for Learning Approaches and Academic Achievement by Country

Figure 6

Table 6. Differences in approaches by Country

Figure 7

Table 7. ASSIST. Norms for Argentina and Spain

Figure 8

Table 8. Latent Class Analysis. Fit Indices

Figure 9

Table 9. Learning Styles and Academic Achievement Class Analysis by Country