Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T12:58:39.120Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How Impulsivity is Related to Intelligence and Academic Achievement

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 April 2014

Andreu Vigil-Coleṭ*
Affiliation:
Universidad Rovira y Virgili
Fabia Morales-Vives
Affiliation:
Universidad Rovira y Virgili
*
Correspondence should be addressed to Andreu Vigil-Coleṭ, Universidad Rovira y Virgili, Facultad de Ciencias de la Educación y Psicología, Crtra. Valls s/n, 43007 – Tarragona (Spain). E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This study investigated the relationships between impulsivity, intelligence, and academic failure in a sample of 241 secondary school students who completed Thurstone's (1938) Primary Mental Abilities (PMA) test and Dickman's (1990) and Barratt's (1985) impulsivity questionnaires (DII and BIS-10, respectively). Results show an inverse relationship between impulsivity and intelligence, specific to the scales with higher loadings on crystallized intelligence, and a positive relationship between impulsivity and academic failure. These results indicate that impulsivity is not directly related to intelligence and may act as a moderator variable between individuals' resources and their achievements.

El presente estudio analiza las relaciones existentes entre impulsividad, inteligencia y fracaso escolar en una muestra de 241 estudiantes de secundaria utilizando el test de Aptitudes Mentales Primarias de Thurstone (1938) y los cuestionarios de Dickman (1990) y Barratt (1985) de impulsividad (DII y BIS-10, respectivamente). Los resultados muestran una relación inversa entre impulsividad e inteligencia que es específica de las escalas que presentan mayor saturación en inteligencia cristalizada así como una relación positiva entre la impulsividad y el fracaso escolar. Estos resultados parecen indicar que la impulsividad no está directamente relacionada con la inteligencia y que esta puede actuar como variable moderadora entre los recursos del individuo y sus logros.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, P.L. (1994). Intelligence, attention and learning: Maximal and typical performance. In Detterman, D.K. (Ed.), Current topics in human intelligence. Vol. 4: Theories of intelligence (pp. 127). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Ackerman, P.L., & Heggestad, E.D. (1997). Intelligence, personality and interests: Evidence for overlapping traits. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 219245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashton, M.C., Lee, K., Vernon, P.A., & Lang, K.L. (2000). Fluid intelligence, crystallized intelligence, and the openness/intellect factor. Journal of Research in Personality, 34, 198207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Austin, E.J., Deary, I.J., & Gibson, G.J. (1997). Relationships between ability and personality: Three hypotheses tested. Intelligence, 25, 4970.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barratt, E.S. (1985). Impulsiveness subtraits: Arousal and information processing. In Spence, J.T. & Itard, C.E. (Eds.), Motivation, emotion and personality (pp. 137146). Amsterdam: Elsevier. (Spanish adaptation: Luengo, Carrillo-de-la-Peña, & Otero, 1991.)Google Scholar
Bates, T.C., & Shieles, A. (2003). Crystallized intelligence as a product of speed and drive for experience: The relationships of inspection time and openness to Gc. Intelligence, 31, 275287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, T.C., & Rock, A. (2004). Personality and information processing speed: Independent influences on intelligent performance. Intelligence, 32, 3346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brand, C.R. (1984). Intelligence and inspection time: An ontogenetic relationship? In Turner, C.H. (Ed.), The biology of human intelligence (pp. 5364). North Humberside, UK: Natterton.Google Scholar
Brand, C.R., & Deary, I.J. (1982). Intelligence and inspection time. In Eysenck, H.J. (Ed.), A model for intelligence (pp. 133148). New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brebner, J., & Stough, C. (1995). Theoretical and empirical relationships between personality and intelligence. In Saklofske, D.H. & Zeidner, M. (Eds.), International handbook of personality and intelligence (pp. 321348). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunas-Wagstaff, J., Bergquist, A., & Wagstaff, G.F. (1994). Cognitive correlates of functional and dysfunctional impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 289292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chico, E., Tous, J.M., Lorenzo-Seva, U., & Vigil-Colet, A. (2003). Spanish adaptation of Dickman's Impulsivity Inventory, its relationship to Eysenck's Personality Questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 18831892.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claes, L., Vertommen, H., & Braspenning, N. (2000). Psychometric properties of the Dickman Impulsivity Inventory. Personality and Individual Differences. 29, 2736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demetriou, A., Kyriakides, L., & Avraamidou, C. (2003). The missing link in the relations between intelligence and personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 547581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickman, S.J. (1990). Functional and dysfunctional impulsivity: Personality and cognitive correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Personality, 58, 95102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickman, S.J. (1993). Impulsivity and information processing. In McCown, W., Shure, M.., & Johnson, J. (Eds.), The impulsive client: Theory, research and treatment (pp. 151184). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickman, S.J. (2000). Impulsivity, arousal and attention. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 563581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, H.J. (1987). Speed of information processing, RT, and the theory of intelligence (pp. 5785). In Vernon, P.A. (Ed.), Intelligence and speed of information processing. New York: Ablex.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H.J. (1994). Personality and intelligence: Psychometric and experimental approaches. In Sternberg, R.J. & Ruzgis, P. (Eds.), Personality and intelligence (pp. 331). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Expósito, J., & Andrés-Pueyo, A. (1997). The effects of impulsivity on the perceptual and decision stages in a choice reaction time task. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 693697.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fink, A.D., & McCown, W.G. (1993). Impulsivity in children and adolescents: Measurement, causes and treatment. In McCown, W., Shure, M.., & Johnson, J. (Eds.), The impulsive client, theory, research and treatment (pp. 279308). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furnham, A., Forde, L., & Cotter, T. (1988). Personality and intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 187192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goff, M., & Ackermann, P.L. (1992). Personality-intelligence relations: Assessment of typical intellectual engagement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 537552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helmers, K.F., Young, S.N., & Pihl, R.O. (1995). Assessment of measures of impulsivity in healthy male volunteers. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 927935.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, A.R. (1981). Bias in mental testing. London. Methunen.Google Scholar
Jensen, A.R. (1993). Why is reaction time correlated with psychometric g? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 5356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, A.R. (1998). The g factor. London: Praeger.Google Scholar
Luengo, M.A, Carrillo-de-la-Peña, M.T, & Otero, J.M. (1991). The components of impulsiveness: A comparison of the I7 Impulsiveness Questionnaire and the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 657667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynam, D., Moffitt, T., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1993). Explaining the relation between IQ and delinquency: Class, race, test motivation, school failure or self-control? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 2, 187196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMurran, M., Blair, M., & Egan, V. (2002). An investigation of the correlations between aggression, impulsiveness, social problem-solving, and alcohol use. Aggressive Behavior, 28, 439445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Messer, S.B. (1976). Reflection-impulsivity: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 10261052.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patton, J.H. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 768774.3.0.CO;2-1>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, L.H., & Rabbitt, P.M.A. (1995). Impulsivity and speed-accuracy strategies in intelligence test performance. Intelligence, 21, 1329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rindermann, H., & Neubauer, A. (2001). The influence of personality on three aspects of cognitive performance: Processing speed, intelligence and school performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 829842.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schweizer, K. (2002). Does impulsivity influence performance in reasoning? Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 10311043.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thurstone, L. L. (1938). Primary mental abilities. Chicago: Univeristy of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(Spanish adaptation: Aptitudes mentales primarias. Madrid: TEA, 1989).Google Scholar
Weijers, H.G., Wiesbeck, G.A., & Böning, J. (2001). Reflection-impulsivity, personality and performance: A psychometric and validity study of the Matching Familiar Figures Test in detoxified alcoholics. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 731754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeidner, M. (1995). Personality trait correlates of intelligence. In Saklofske, D.H. & Zeidner, M. (Eds.), International handbook of personality and intelligence (pp. 299320). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar