Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T03:09:11.994Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How Chronological Age, Theory of Mind, and Yield are Interrelated to Memory and Suggestion in Young Children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 October 2022

Nieves Pérez-Mata*
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (Spain)
Amparo Moreno
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (Spain)
Margarita Diges
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (Spain)
Miriam Peláez
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (Spain)
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Nieves Pérez-Mata. Universidad Autónoma de Madrid. Facultad de Psicología. Departamento de Psicología Básica. Calle Iván Pavlov, No. 6, Ciudad Universitaria de Cantoblanco. 28049 Madrid (Spain). E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

We investigated the interrelations between chronological age, theory of mind (ToM), Yield (as a measure of individual suggestibility), memory and acceptance of experimental suggestion in a sample of children between 3 and 7 years old (N = 106). One week after participants interacted with ‘a Teacher’, they were asked to recall activities carried out with the Teacher (direct experience) and the contents of a story read to them by the Teacher (indirect experience). Data were examined with an analysis of developmental trajectories, which allows establishing the predictor value of socio-cognitive developmental factors regardless of participants’ chronological age. It also estimates predictor values in interaction with the age and determines whether age is the best predictor for performance. As in previous research, results showed that chronological age was the main predictor of memory performance, both for direct experience (i.e., activities performed) and indirect experience (i.e., contents of the story). However, ToM and Yield, together with participants’ ages, modulated their acceptance of the external suggestions received (presented only once, one week after the event). A turning point was observed at age 4.6. Below this age, the greater the mentalist skills (higher ToM), the lower was the vulnerability to external suggestion. Still, children below this age characterized individually as being suggestible (Yield medium or high) were more vulnerable to suggestion the younger they were. Thus, developmental socio-cognitive factors might modulate young children’s vulnerability to external suggestions, even if received only once.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

The authors thank Soledad Herrera, María López Rodil and Nuria Malibrán for their help in the collecting and coding the data; Karina Solcoff assisted in the preparation of the material. We also thank Dr. Pilar Aivar for her suggestions and comments prior to submission. Finally, special thanks to parents, children and school staff of the two schools - C. E. I. P. Vicente Aleixandre in Torrejón de Ardoz (Madrid) and E. I. Bärber Inhelder in Madrid - who participated in the present study.

Conflicts of interest: None.

Funding statement: This work was supported by the Transfer Knowledge Project 088501–FUAM (Foundation of the Universidad Autónoma de Madrid) co-directed by the first and third authors, and the National Research SEJ2004–07655 financed by Dirección General de Investigación Científica y Técnica (DGICYT).

Data Sharing: The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions.

References

Azzopardi, C., Eirich, R., Rash, C. L., MacDonald, S., & Madigan, S. (2019). A meta-analysis of the prevalence of child sexual abuse disclosure in forensic settings. Child Abuse & Neglect, 93, 291304. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.11.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2012). Reliability of children’s testimony in the era of developmental reversals. Developmental Review, 32(3), 224267. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2012.06.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bright-Paul, A., Jarrold, C., & Wright, D. B. (2008). Theory-of-mind developmental influences suggestibility and source monitoring. Developmental Psychology, 44(4), 10551068. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.4.1055CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruck, M., & Ceci, S. J. (1999). The suggestibility of children’s memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 419439. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.419CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruck, M., & Melnyk, L. (2004). Individual differences in children’s suggestibility: A review and synthesis. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(8), 947996. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1070CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caprin, C., Benedan, L., Ciaccia, D., Mazza, E., Messineo, S., & Piuri, E. (2016). True and false memories in middle childhood: The relationship with cognitive functioning. Psychology, Crime & Law, 22, 473494. https://doi.org/10.1080/1068316X.2016.1168420CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (2006). Children’s suggestibility: Characteristic and mechanisms. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 34, 247281. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2407(06)80009-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ceci, S. J., & Howe, M. J. A. (1978). Age-related differences in free recall as a function of retrieval flexibility. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 26(3), 432442. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(78)90123-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ceci, S. J., Toglia, M. P., & Ross, D. F. (1988). On remembering… more or less: A trace strength interpretation of developmental differences in suggestibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117(2), 250262. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.117.2.201Google Scholar
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 148153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diges, M., Moreno, A., & Pérez-Mata, N. (2010). Efectos de sugestión en preescolares: Capacidades mentalistas y diferencias individuales en sugestionabilidad [Suggestion effects in preschoolers: Mentalist skills and individual differences in suggestibility]. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 33(2), 235254. https://doi.org//10.1174/021037010791114634CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elischberger, H. B. (2005). The effects of prior knowledge on children’s memory and suggestibility. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92(3), 247275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2005.05.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garven, S., Wood, J. M., Malpass, R. S., & Shaw, J. S. (1998). More than suggestion. The effect of interviewing techniques from the McMartin Preschool case. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83(3), 347359. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.83.3.347CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gobbo, C., Mega, C., & Pipe, M.-E. (2002). Does the nature of the experience influence suggestibility? A study of children’s event memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 81(4), 502530. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.2002.2662CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gudjonsson, G. H. (2003). The psychology of interrogations and confessions. A handbook. John Wiley & Sons. http://doi.org/10.1002/9780470713297Google Scholar
Guéguen, N., Lourel, M., Pascual, A., & Mouda, F. (2011). L’importance d’un effet: Méthodologie simple de détermination et d’évaluation de l’«effect size» [A simple method to determine and evaluate “effect size”]. Cahiers D’études et de Recherches Francophones /Sante, 21(2), 103105. https://doi.org/10.1684/san.2011.0240Google Scholar
Hughes, C., Devine, R. T., Ensor, R., Koyasu, M., Mizokawa, A., & Lecce, S. (2014). Lost in translation? Comparing British, Japanese, and Italian children’s theory-of-mind performance. Child Development Research, 2014, 110. https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/893492CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karpinski, A. C., & Scullin, M. H. (2009). Suggestibility under pressure: Theory of mind, executive function, and suggestibility in preschoolers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30(6), 749763. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2009.05.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleinknecht, E., & Beike, D. R. (2004). How knowing and doing inform an autobiography: Relations among preschoolers’ theory of mind, narrative, and event memory skills. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 745764. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1030CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klemfuss, J. Z., & Olaguez, A. P. (2020). Individual differences in children’s suggestibility: An updated review. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 29, 158182. https://doi.org/10.1080/10538712.2018.1508108CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malloy, L. C., & Quas, J. A. (2009). Children’s suggestibility: Areas of consensus and controversy. In Kuehnle, K. & Connell, M. (Eds.), The evaluation of child sexual abuse allegations. A comprehensive guide to assessment and testimony (pp. 267298). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
McCarthy, D. (2004). Escalas McCarthy de aptitudes y psicomotricidad para niños [The McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities] (MSCA). Madrid TEA.Google Scholar
Melinder, A., Endestad, T., & Magnussen, S. (2006). Relations between episodic memory, suggestibility, theory of mind, and cognitive inhibition in the preschool child. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 47(6), 485495. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2006.00542.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melinder, A., Scullin, M. H., Gunnerød, V., & Nyborg, E. (2005). Generalizability of a two-factor measure of pre-school age children’s suggestibility in Norway and the USA. Psychology, Crime & Law, 11(2), 123145. https://doi.org/10.1080/10683160512331316299CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ornstein, P. A., Gordon, B. N., & Larus, D. M. (1992). Children’s memory for a personal experienced event: Implications for testimony. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 6(1), 4960. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2350060103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Otgaar, H., Chan, J. C. K., Calado, B., & La Rooy, D. (2019). Immediate interviewing increases children’s suggestibility in the short term, but not in the long term. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 24(1), 2440. https://doi.org/10.1111/lcrp.12137CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peláez, M., Pérez-Mata, N., & Diges, M. (2019). Influencia del conocimiento previo y la repetición de entrevistas: memoria y sugestión en una muestra de preescolares [Influence of prior knowledge and repetition of interviews: Memory and suggestion in a sample of preschool students]. Colombia Forense, 6(1), 123. https://doi.org/10.16925/2145-9649.2019.01.02Google Scholar
Peláez, M., Pérez-Mata, N., & Diges, M. (2021). How does prior knowledge affect children’s memory and suggestibility? Pracana, C. & Wang, W. (Eds.), Psychological applications & developments VII. Advances in psychology and psychological trends series (pp. 317329). InScience Press. https://doi.org/10.36315/2021padVII27Google Scholar
Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Perner, J. (2000). Memory and theory of mind. In Tulving, E. & Craik, F. I. M. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 317–312). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Perner, J., Kloo, D., & Gornik, E. (2007). Episodic memory development: Theory of mind is part of re-experiencing experienced events. Infant and Child Development, 16(5), 471490. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.517CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J., & Ruffman, T. (1995). Episodic memory and autonoetic consciousness: Developmental evidence and a theory of childhood amnesia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59(3), 516548. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1995.1024CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Principe, G. F., Greenhoot, A. F., & Ceci, S. J. (2014). Young children’s eyewitness testimony memory. In Perfect, T. & Lindsay, D. S. (Eds.), The SAGE Handbook of applied memory (pp. 633653). SAGE Publications. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446294703.n35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Principe, G. F., Kanaya, T., Ceci, S. J., & Singh, M. (2006). Believing is seeing: How rumors can engender false memories in preschoolers. Psychological Science, 17(3), 243248. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01692.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quas, J. A., Wallin, A. R., Papini, S. P., Lench, H., & Scullin, M. H. (2005). Suggestibility, social support, and memory for a novel experience in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 91(4), 315341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2005.03.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quesque, F., & Rossetti, Y. (2020). What do theory-of-mind tasks actually measure? Theory and practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(2) 384396. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619896607CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Repacholi, B. M., & Gopnik, A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14-and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 33(1), 1221. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.12CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sauerland, M., Brackmann, N., & Otgaar, H. (2019). Rapport: Little effect on children’s, adolescents’, and adults’ statement quantity, accuracy, and suggestibility. Journal of Child Custody, 15(4), 268285. https://doi.org/10.1080/15379418.2018.1509759CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scullin, M. H., & Bonner, K. (2006). Theory of mind, inhibitory control, and preschool-age children’s suggestibility in different interviewing contexts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93(2), 120138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2005.09.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, T. D., Raman, S. R., Madigan, S., Waldman, J., & Shouldice, M. (2018). Anogenital findings in 3,569 pediatric examinations for sexual abuse/assault. Journal of Pediatric and Adolescent Gynecology, 31, 7983. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpag.2017.10.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, M. S. C., Annaz, D., Ansari, D., Scerif, G., Jarrold, C., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2009). Using developmental trajectories to understand developmental disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(2), 336358. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/07-0144)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tousignant, J. P., Hall, D., & Loftus, E. F. (1986). Discrepancy detection and vulnerability to misleading postevent information. Memory & Cognition, 14(4), 329338. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202511CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 26(1), 112. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0080017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welch-Ross, M. (2000). A mental-state reasoning model of suggestibility and memory source monitoring. In Roberts, K. P. & Blades, M. (Eds.), Children’s source monitoring (pp. 227256). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M. (2012). Theory of mind: Better methods, clearer findings, more development. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9(3), 313330. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2012.680297CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523541. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westra, E., & Carruthers, P. (2017). Pragmatic development explains the Theory-of-Mind Scale. Cognition, 158, 165176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.10.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, J. M., & Garven, S. (2000). How sexual abuse interviews go astray: Implications for prosecutors, police, and child protection services. Child Maltreatment, 5(2), 109118. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077559500005002003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed