Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T11:45:59.772Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

“The Fathers have eaten Sour Grapes, and the Children’s Teeth are set on Edge”: Differentiating the Emotional Experiences of Grima and Disgust

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 October 2018

Inge Schweiger Gallo*
Affiliation:
Universidad Complutense de Madrid (Spain)
Miryam Rodríguez Monter
Affiliation:
Universidad Internacional de La Rioja (Spain)
José Luis Álvaro-Estramiana
Affiliation:
Universidad Complutense de Madrid (Spain)
Juan Ignacio Durán
Affiliation:
Centro Universitario Cardenal Cisneros (Spain)
José-Miguel Fernández-Dols
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (Spain)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Inge Schweiger Gallo. Universidad Complutense de Madrid. Facultad de Ciencias Políticas y Sociología. Departamento de Antropología Social y Psicología Social. Campus de Somosaguas 28040 Madrid (Spain). E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The Spanish term grima refers to the aversive emotional experience typically evoked when one hears, for example, a scratch upon a chalkboard. Whereas Spanish speakers can distinguish between the concepts of grima and disgust, English and German speakers lack a specific word for this experience and typically label grima as disgust. In the present research, we tested the degree of differentiation between the two aversive experiences in Spanish speakers. Study 1 addressed whether Spanish speakers apply spontaneously the term grima rather than disgust to grima-eliciting experiences. Study 2 systematically addressed the constitutive features of both grima and disgust by mapping their internal structures. Results showed that the noise of a chulk on a blackboard and scraping fingernails on a blackboard, along with the physical manifestation of goose bumps, were the most typical features of the category. Whereas both grima and disgust were characterized as unpleasant sensations, t(193) = 1.21, ns, they differed with respect to their physiological signatures (e.g., producing shivers was characteristic of grima, as compared to disgust, t(194) = 12.02, p = .001, d = 1.72) and elicitors (e.g., a fractured bone was a characteristic elicitor of grima; t(193) = 5.78, p = .001, d = .83, whereas pederasts and pedophiles were the most characteristic elicitor of disgust, t(193) = 8.46, p = .001, d = 1.21). Thus, both grima and disgust are conceptually different experiences, whose shared features hold different degrees of typicality. The present research suggests that grima and disgust are two distinct emotion concepts.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

The present studies were financed by the Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación’s (MICINN) grant I+D+i PSI2009-07066 to Inge Schweiger Gallo, Miryam Rodriguez Monter and José Luis Álvaro. We would like to thank Noemy Sanz for assistance during part of data acquisition of Study 1.

References

Cisler, J. M., Reardon, J. M., Williams, N. L., & Lohr, J. M. (2007). Anxiety sensitivity and disgust sensitivity interact to predict contamination fear. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 935946. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curtis, V., Aunger, R., & Rabie, T. (2004). Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proceedings of the Royal Society Biological Sciences, B, 271, S131S133. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2003.0144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curtis, V., & Biran, A. (2001). Dirt, disgust, and disease: Is hygiene in our genes? Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 44, 1731. https://doi.org/10.1353/pbm.2001.0001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delin, P. S., & Winefield, A. H. (1973). On having the teeth set on edge: Preliminary inquiry. Psychological Reports, 33, 379382. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1973.33.2.379CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delin, P. S., & Winefield, A. H. (1977). Questionnaire study of the teeth on edge phenomenon. Psychological Reports, 41, 823827. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1977.41.3.823CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, B. (1988). Prototype analysis of the concepts of love and commitment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 557579. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.55.4.557CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, B., & Russell, J. A. (1984). Concept of emotion viewed from a prototype perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 464486. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.113.3.464CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gregg, A. P., Hart, C. M., Sedikides, C., & Kumashiro, M. (2008). Everyday conceptions of modesty: a prototype analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 978992. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208316734CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J., McCauley, C., & Rozin, P. (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 701713. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(94)90212-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurtado de Mendoza, A., Fernández-Dols, J. M., Parrott, W. G., & Carrera, P. (2010). Emotion terms, category structure, and the problem of translation: The case of shame and vergüenza. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 661680. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930902958255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kupfer, T. R. (2018). Why are injuries disgusting? Comparing pathogen avoidance and empathy accounts. Emotion. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/0.1037/emo0000395Google Scholar
Lambert, N. M., Graham, S. M., & Fincham, F. D. (2009). A prototype analysis of gratitude: Varieties of gratitude experiences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 11931207. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167209338071CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marks, I. M. (1987). Fears, phobias and rituals: Panic, anxiety, and their disorders. Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Nederkoorn, S., Rassin, E., Candel, I., & Horselenberg, R. (2000). Disgust and psychopathological symptoms in a nonclinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 11631167. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00263-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 303321. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014823CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olatunji, B. O., Sawchuk, C. N., Lohr, J. M., & de Jong, P. J. (2004). Disgust domains in the prediction of contamination fear. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 93104. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00102-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosch, E. H. (1973). Natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 328350. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(73)90017-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological Review, 94, 2341. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.94.1.23CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (1993). Disgust. In Lewis, M. & Haviland, J. (Eds.), The handbook of emotions (pp. 575594). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. (1999). Disgust: The body and soul emotion. In Dalgleish, T. & Power, M. (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 429445). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2008). Disgust. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., & Barrett, L. F. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 757776). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1991). In defense of a prototype approach to emotion concepts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 3747. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.60.1.37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. A. (2009). Emotion, core affect, and psychological construction. Cognition & Emotion, 23, 12591283. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930902809375CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. A., & Fehr, B. (1994). Fuzzy concepts in a fuzzy hierarchy: Varieties of anger. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 186205. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schweiger Gallo, I., Fernández-Dols, J. M., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Keil, A. (2017). Grima: A distinct emotion concept? Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 131. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00131CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schweiger Gallo, I., Rodríguez Monter, M., Álvaro, J. L., & Fernández-Dols, J. M. (2018). Disentangling the emotional experience of grima. Psychological Studies, 63, 7077. doi: 10.1007/s12646-018-0441-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shenhav, A., & Mendes, W. B. (2014). Aiming for the stomach and hitting the heart: Dissociable triggers and sources for disgust reactions. Emotion, 14, 301309. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034644CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seibt, B., Schubert, T. W., Zickfeld, J. H., & Fiske, A. P. (2017). Interpersonal closeness and morality predict feelings of being moved. Emotion, 17, 389394. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000271CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Set someone’s teeth on edge (2010). In Webster’s New World College Dictionary (4th ed.). Retrieved from https://www.collinsdictionary.com/.Google Scholar
Tolin, D. F., Woods, C. M., & Abramowitz, J. S. (2006). Disgust sensitivity and obsessive-compulsive symptoms in a nonclinical sample. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 37, 3040. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2005.09.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., & Griskevicius, V. (2009). Microbes, mating, and morality: Individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 103122. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015474CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed