Hostname: page-component-f554764f5-c4bhq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-04-13T10:58:43.891Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Behaviour Problems in Preschoolers with and without Prematurity. A Multi-Informant Longitudinal Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 April 2025

Eva María Padilla-Muñoz
Affiliation:
Universidad de Sevilla, Spain
Alejandra Pereira-Cerro
Affiliation:
Universidad de Sevilla, Spain
María Merced Barbancho-Morant
Affiliation:
Universidad de Sevilla, Spain
María Dolores Lanzarote-Fernández*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Sevilla, Spain
*
Corresponding author: María Dolores Lanzarote Fernández; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Data on the behavioral development of preterm infants are inconclusive. The aim of this study was to explore behavioral development during preschool years, considering prematurity, measurement time, gender, and informant. This is a prospective longitudinal analytical observational study, with a sample of 98 parents and 98 teachers of children aged 4, 5, and 6 years with and without a history of prematurity, who were evaluated by the Child Behavior Checklist and Teacher’s Report Form. Parents and teachers of the preschoolers report average scores on all behavioral scales. We observed variability according to degree of prematurity, age, and informant. Teachers detected more attention difficulties in the very preterm group (VPTG) than in the born-at-term group at 4 years. Parents and teachers coincided in detecting greater withdrawal in the moderate and late preterm group (MTPG) compared to the born-at-term group and an increase in difficulties with increasing age. The General Linear Model revealed that moderate prematurity, the age of 6 years, and parental report have a greater risk of behavioral difficulties. The need for follow-up also in moderate preterm infants is emphasized, especially at 6 years of age and with multi-informants.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de la Psicología de Madrid

Introduction

It is clear that the development of preterm infants has improved with advances in perinatal medicine and neonatology. However, neither the rate of preterm births nor the detection of associated morbidities has decreased. Preterm birth is defined as occurring before 37 weeks of gestation and is divided into extreme (< 28 weeks), very preterm (28–32 weeks), and MTPG (32 < 37 weeks) (World Health Organization, 2023).

The available data on the evolution of this large at-risk group are not consistent to date. As Fisher et al. (Reference Fisher, Miley, Glazer, Gies, Parikh, Lam and Wade2024) point out, attention beyond the early years, with a preventive approach, is very scarce. In particular, studies on the behavior of PTG during the preschool stage are inconclusive. In the work by Narberhaus and Segarra (Reference Narberhaus and Segarra2004), the need for research to determine the long-term consequences of prematurity was already indicated. Thus, while some studies have indicated that, at 4–5 years of age, they did not display more behavioral problems than those born at term (O′ Meagher et al., Reference O’Meagher, Norris, Kemp and Anderson2019; Perez-Pereira & Baños, Reference Perez-Pereira and Baños2019), others stated that they occur more frequently (Johnson, Reference Johnson2007; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Matthews, Draper, Field, Manktelow, Marlow, Smith and Boyle2015; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Winchester and Sullivan2018). Buttha et al. (Reference Buttha, Cleves, Casey, Cradok and Anand2002), who reviewed the literature on the behavior of PTG and adolescents between 5 and 14 years of age, observed that 81% of the studies showed a higher rate of internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Similarly, the results of Scott et al. (Reference Scott, Winchester and Sullivan2018) indicate individual variation in the number of externalizing and internalizing problems over time. In other studies, preterm birth has also been associated with behavioral difficulties in the medium and long term (Arpi & Ferrari, Reference Arpi and Ferrari2013; García et al., Reference García, Cruz-Quintana, Sosa, De la Cruz, Mañas and Pérez-García2012; Quesada et al., Reference Quesada, Tristão, Pratesi and Wolf2014; Salt & Redshaw, Reference Salt and Redshaw2006; Schonhaut et al., Reference Schonhaut, Pérez and Astudillo2012), and it constitutes a risk factor for their quality of life (Aparecida et al., Reference Aparecida, Pereira and Benzaquen2016).

More specifically, an inverse relationship has been found between behavioral problems and gestational age at birth in preschoolers (Pallás Alonso, Reference Pallás Alonso2012). Cross-sectional studies show that extremely preterm infants at 2.5 years of age have behavioral outcomes within the mid-range, although there were more Internalizing, Externalizing and Total Problems than in full-term infants (Månsson et al., Reference Månsson, Stjernqvist and Bäckström2014). An increased risk of behavioral problems in the preschool stage has also been reported in very preterm infants. That is, both 3-year-olds (Delobel-Ayoub et al., Reference Delobel-Ayoub, Kaminski, Marret, Burguet, Marchand and Guyen2006) and 4-year-olds (Jones et al., Reference Jones, Champion and Woodward2013) had poorer emotional and behavioral adjustment, as well as subtle social difficulties that could influence their relationships with others. Also at age 4, Kelly et al. (Reference Kelly, Arcoleo, D’Agata and Sullivan2023) observed higher scores on internalizing behaviors. At age 5, emotional and behavioral problems have been found to affect children academically (Reijneveld et al., Reference Reijneveld, de Klein and van de Baar2006).

Even in the MPTG, at 4 years of age, it has been reported that there is a risk factor for emotional and behavioral disorders (Des Haan et al., Reference Des Haan, de Kroon, van Dokkum, Kerstjens, Reijneveld and Bos2019). Similarly, Johnson et al. (Reference Johnson, Matthews, Draper, Field, Manktelow, Marlow, Smith and Boyle2015) found that birth before 36 weeks of gestation implies a specific risk of delayed social competence at 2 years of age. This may be indicative of an increased risk of emotional and behavioral disorders later in childhood. Labayru et al. (Reference Labayru, Aliri, Santos, Arrizabalaga, Estevez, Cancela, Gaztañaga, Marti and Sistiaga2021) also reported high percentages of clinically relevant scores in moderate preterm infants, as did Jin et al. (Reference Jin, Yoon, Song, Kim and Chung2020), who reported more executive, attention and memory deficits in this same group.

In contrast, at the beginning of primary school, studies have been more numerous, with more solid conclusions. It has been observed that both very preterm and preterm infants in general presented more behavioral and emotional problems compared to those born at term (Aarnoudse-Moens et al., Reference Aarnoudse-Moens, Weisglas-Kuperus, van Goudoever and Oosterlaan2009; Alcántara-Cabanal et al., Reference Alcántara-Canabal, Fernández-Baizán, Solís-Sánchez, Arias and Méndez2020; Iriondo et al., Reference Iriondo, Póo and Ibáñez2006; Samuelsson et al., Reference Samuelsson, Holsti, Adamsson, Serenius, Hägglöf and Farooqi2017). At later ages, this trend seems to continue (Johns, Reference Johns2019; Klein et al., Reference Klein, Gaspardo, Martinez and Martins2015), with internalizing symptomatology standing out in very preterm infants (Faure et al., Reference Faure, Habersaat, Harari, Müller-Nix, Borghini, Ansermet, Tolsa and Urben2017; Larsen et al., Reference Larsen, Kochhar, Wolke, Draper, Marlow and Johnson2024; Samuelsson et al., Reference Samuelsson, Holsti, Adamsson, Serenius, Hägglöf and Farooqi2017). In short, the findings on behavior problems in preterm preschoolers and, more specifically, in the ages evaluated, continue to be scarce and inconclusive, and most of the data are obtained through cross-sectional studies.

Significant differences in behavior have also been detected in terms of gender (Alcántara-Cabanal et al., Reference Alcántara-Canabal, Fernández-Baizán, Solís-Sánchez, Arias and Méndez2020). Most studies show that boys present more difficulties than girls (Aparecida et al., Reference Aparecida, Pereira and Benzaquen2016; Des Haan et al., Reference Des Haan, de Kroon, van Dokkum, Kerstjens, Reijneveld and Bos2019), which can be considered a risk factor for quality of life (Aparecida et al., Reference Aparecida, Pereira and Benzaquen2016). In the same vein, Samuelsson et al. (Reference Samuelsson, Holsti, Adamsson, Serenius, Hägglöf and Farooqi2017) state that parents and teachers report more general behavioral problems in boys and more anxiety problems in girls born very preterm. Bul and van Baar (Reference Bul and van Baar2012) stated that teachers also detected more problems in preterm boys.

Longitudinal studies covering this stage would be of interest (Loureiro et al., Reference Loureiro, Agut, Bonorat and Martínez-Biarge2019; Rodríguez et al., Reference Rodríguez, Vivas, Cangelosi and Schapira2008), as they would obtain information from different informants and instruments (Camerota et al., Reference Camerota, Bagley, McGowan, Carter, Check, Dansereau, DellaGrotta, Helderman, Hofheimer, Loncar, Neal, O’Shea, Pastyrnak, Smith, Everson and Lester2024), which could improve the detection of difficulties. In this respect, Allotey’s meta-analysis concludes that it is important that parents, educators, health professionals, and policymakers take into consideration the added academic, emotional, and behavioral needs of those born with a history of prematurity (Allotey et al., Reference Allotey, Zamora, Cheong-See, Kalidindi, Arroyo-Manzano, Asztalos, Van Der Post, Mol, Moore, Birtles, Khan and Thangaratinam2018).

The literature reveals that there continues to be a lack of consensus on the behavior of preschool children with and without prematurity, recommending the consideration of different sources of information and other variables that could explain this variability. Thus, the main objective of this study was to evaluate the behavior problems of a group of preterm children (PTG) and a term group (TG) at 4, 5, and 6 years of age through the study of internalizing and externalizing problems, using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2001a, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2001b) and the Teacher’s Report Form (TRF) (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2001a, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2001b). The gender of the children and the information provided by parents and teachers were taken into account, which improves the validity of the results. The first specific objective was to evaluate the behavior problems of children during the preschool years (4, 5 and 6 years) attending to prematurity, the prematurity level, and the informant. The second objective was to analyze the predictive capacity of these variables on the behavior of the preschoolers with and without prematurity.

Method

A prospective longitudinal observational analytical study was carried out with a case–control design. The study sample was selected by purposive, convenience, non-probabilistic sampling. It included a group of PTG without severe sequelae at 4 years and a non-premature group, both homogeneous in gender and chronological age. The sample consisted of 98 parents and 98 teachers of preschoolers born before 37 weeks of gestation without severe sequelae (i.e., presence of cerebral palsy, severe maturational delay and/or significant sensory deficits) and born-at-term preschoolers, both groups without clinical complications or educational gaps, at 4 years of age. At the time of the assessment, all of them were in the appropriate class for their age group.

Parents were contacted by the neonatology unit of a public hospital in Southern Spain to provide them with information about this study and to request their participation through the consent form developed for this study. The number of children born with less than 37 weeks of gestation, admitted to the NICU of a third-level hospital in a period of two years was 116, 14 of whom presented severe sequelae (defined above). The total sample consisted of 102 cases, 25 of whom did not agree to participate or could not be contacted, and another five eventually decided not to attend (data available on request). The sample is representative of the total population, with a total of 72 children included, with a margin of error of 0.05. The reference values for the sample size were taken: (α = 0.05), 95% confidence level (z = 1.96), and p = 0.2. The calculation of the p value was based on the work of Gómez et al. (Reference Gómez, Sánchez, García and Segovia2019), where the percentage of premature infants with transient sequelae was 20%. During follow-up, 23 cases had to be discarded due to missing data on various measures. The missing data for each variable in each measure (4, 5 and 6 years) were considered as Missing at Random (MAR), and the mean was used as an imputation criterion (Figure 1).

Figure 1. Preterm children born.

The research was approved by the Autonomous Community Biomedical Research Ethics Coordinating Committee (1188-N-16) and complies with the Declaration of Helsinki. The families who formed the preterm group gave the researchers authorization to contact their children’s school to recruit the sample of full-term preschoolers (TG). There was therefore a similarity not only in gender and age, but also in sociodemographic characteristics and educational context.

They were presented with the research and informed consent. The group was formed with peers from the same class, matched for gender and age. Finally, the PTG children were grouped into a VPTG, made up of parents and teachers of children born before 32 weeks of gestation (23 < 32 weeks), and a group of moderate (N = 8) and late (N = 10) preterm infants, for children born between 32 and 37 weeks of gestation (Figure 2). Sociodemographic variables of the sample of premature infants are shown in Figure 3.

Figure 2. Sample data flow.

Figure 3. Sociodemographic variables of the sample of premature infants.

GE = Gestational Age; χ (dt) = mean and standard deviation; Interval = highest and lowest score; N (%)= group size and percentage of total; GE (weeks) = gestational age at birth by weeks.

The evaluation of the groups was matched in gestational age at birth, gender, and time. Information was collected at three different points in time (4, 5 and 6 years; ± two months) from parents and teachers of the preschool groups. The behavior of the preschoolers was assessed by the CBCL (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2001a, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2001b) and the TRF (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2001a, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2001b). Two versions of each scale were used: one aimed at children between one-and-a-half and five years of age and the other between six and eighteen years of age, which differed in some scales. In total, data were obtained from 12 scales, with some differences according to age and context: Emotional Reactivity, Anxiety and Depression, Somatic Complaints, Social Problems, Thought Problems, Withdrawal, Sleeping Problems, Attention Problems, Oppositional Conduct, Aggressive Behavior, Internalizing Problems, Externalizing Problems and Total Problems. For all scales (criteria variables) the normal range is < 93 percentile. This test has been widely used with preterm infants at different stages (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Miley, Glazer, Gies, Parikh, Lam and Wade2024) and has recently been recommended as a screening test to detect all potentially needy preterm infants (Camerota et al., Reference Camerota, Bagley, McGowan, Carter, Check, Dansereau, DellaGrotta, Helderman, Hofheimer, Loncar, Neal, O’Shea, Pastyrnak, Smith, Everson and Lester2024).

Based on Bono et al. (Reference Bono, Núñez and Arnau2010), the present study follows a mixed, quasi-experimental design, with no manipulation of variables, and the participants are evaluated at the same time point (cross-sectional component) and at three time points (longitudinal component). The analyses were performed using SPSS v. 25.0 for Windows. Three types of analyses were conducted: (1) Descriptive, both for the explanatory factors and for the criterion dimensions measured with the inventory. (2) Non-parametric comparative (Mann–Whitney U-test), to explore the relationships between the prematurity subgroups and the non-prematurity group, as a function of the measurement time and informants. (3) Multivariates; for those cases in which the normality and linearity criteria were not met, a General Linear Model was used to assess the effects of the independent variables (IV, prematurity, time, gender and informant) on each dependent variable (DV, dimensions of behavior) (α = .05), and post hoc multiple comparisons in those non-categorical factors that were significant. That n represents the number of measurements taken for each variable. The partial eta squared values (η) to interpret the effect size were: .01 = small effect, .06 = moderate effect, and .14 = large effect. The observed power was considered to be adequate from a value of .08 (Cárdenas & Arancibia, Reference Cárdenas and Arancibia2019).

Results

Firstly, the descriptive analyses by groups (PTG and TG) and by degree of prematurity (VPTG and MPTG) (Supplementary MaterialFigures 1–6) placed behaviors within the mean range in all groups and ages, both in parents and teachers.

Paired Mann–Whitney U-test was used between groups (PTG vs TG) and degree of prematurity (VPTG vs TG, MPTG vs TG and VPTG vs MPTG). When comparing the results of PTG and TG at age 4, significant differences were found in teachers in Withdrawal (PTG: 56.94; TG: 55.06; p = .045; r = .203), Attention Problems (PTG: 54.29; TG: 52.78; p =. 009; r = .263), Aggressive Behavior (PTG: 53.63; TG: 52.78; p = .034; r = .214), Externalizing Problems (PTG: 49.20; TG: 48.53; p = .019; r = .237) and Total Problems (PTG: 49.73; TG: 48.51; p = .046; r = .202). Also, between VPTG and TG in the same behavior with higher mean scores in the VPTG (Withdrawal, VPTG: 55.65; TG: 52.35; p = .011; r = .285; Attention Problems, VPTG: x56.16; TG: 52.88; p = .001; r = .361; Aggressive Behavior, VPTG: 53.00; TG: 52.04; p = .013; r = .278; Externalizing Problems, VPTG: 51.78; TG: 46.92; p = .002; r = .351, and Total Problems, VPTG: 51.65; TG: 46.71; p =. 010; r = .288). At 5 years were lower mean scores in Somatic Complaints in Very Preterm (VPTG: 50.00; TG: 52.10; p = .002; r = .346; and with MPTG: 52.06; p = .0001; r = .528). At age six, teachers identified a higher incidence of moderate preterm withdrawal (MPTG: 58.22; TG: 54.69; p = .029; r = .268) and internalizing problems (MPTG: 55.33; TG: 50.45; p = .012; r = .308) compared to those born at term.

It should be noted that parents do not report differences based on prematurity in general. However, parents of MPTG, at 4 and 5 years indicated more Withdrawal than Term parents (MPTG: 58.28; TG: 55.06; p = .047; r = .24; and MPTG: 57.94; TG: 53.88; p = .013; r = .305). Also, there are different at 5 years in Total Problems (MPTG: 51.28; TG: 46.08; p = .046; r = .244).

Finally, significant differences by parents were found according to the degree of prematurity (VPTG-MPTG). Thus, at 4 years of age, they observed more Internalizing Problems (VPTG: 47.97; MPTG: 54.00; p = .025; r = .320) and Total Problems (VPTG: 48.23; MPTG: 52.33; p = .036; r = .299). At 5 years of age, they showed differences in Emotional Reactivity (VPTG: 51.81; MPTG: 55.44; p = .011; r = .364), Withdrawal (VPTG: 53.45; MPTG: 57.94; p =. 015; r = .346), Internalizing Problems (VPTG: 46.90; MPTG: 54.39; p = .013; r = .354) and Total Problems (VPTG: 44.94; MPTG: 51.28; p = .027; r = .316). While at age 6 they only reported more Social Problems (VPTG: 52.71; MPTG: 55.44; p = .041; r = .292).

To respond to the second objective, a General Linear Model was carried out to evaluate the effects of the independent variables (IV: degree of prematurity, measurement time, gender and informant) on each dependent variable (DV: all the dimensions measured through CBCL and TRF (α = .05). Then, post hoc multiple comparisons were performed for the three-category factors that were significant. Next, we present the mean scores obtained, attending to the IV (Tables 1 to 4).

Table 1. Behavioral variables and prematurity

Note: n = number of measurements taken for each variable.

Table 2. Behavioral variables and time of measurement

Note: n = number of measurements taken for each variable.

Table 3. Behavioral variables and informant

Note: n = number of measurements taken for each variable.

Table 4. Behavioral variables and gender

Note: n: number of measurements taken for each variable.

A total of seven valid models were obtained for the following dependent variables: Somatic Complaints, Withdrawal, Attention Problems, Aggressive Behavior, Internalizing Problems, Externalizing Problems and Total Problems. In all cases, a medium-large eta-squared (.089–.124) and adequate power (≥ .8) were obtained (Table 5).

Table 5. Influence of prematurity, time of measurement, sex and informants on behaviour

Note: DDVV = dependent variables; Total = Preterm, years, gender, and reporter; SC = Somatic complaints; W = Withdrawal; AP = Attention problems; AB = Aggressive Behaviour; IP = Internalizing problems; EP = Externalising problems; TP = Total problems; pow = power observed; Sig*: .05 y Sig**: .01.

Attending to the influence of each independent variable individually, gender was not significant on its own but only as part of the model.

Prematurity was identified as a significant predictor of Somatic Complaints, Withdrawal, Attention Problems, Internalizing Problems and Total Problems. That is, there were differences in all these dimensions as a function of the degree of prematurity. In all cases, the significance was <0.05, although Attention Problems and Total Problems did not have sufficient predictive power (< 0.08), and, in the others, the effect size was small. In the post hoc analyses, the moderately premature children presented greater scores in Withdrawal (p = .001), Attention Problems (p = .010), Internalizing Problems (p = .005) and Total Problems (p = .021), whereas the very premature children showed greater scores in Somatic Complaints (p = .001) and Internalizing Problems (p = .017).

The variable Measurement Time (4, 5 and 6 years) can also be considered a predictive variable for some of the analyzed emotional and behavioral dimensions, regardless of prematurity, gender, and informant. In this case, according to the post hoc analyzes, the children presented greater difficulties at the age of 6 years than at the age of 5 years in Attention Problems (p = .001), Aggressive Behavior (p = .001), Internalizing Problems (p = .001), Externalizing Problems (p = .001) and Total Problems (p = .001), as well as compared to the 4-year-old in Aggressive Behavior (p = .002). More difficulties were also observed at the age of 4 years than at the age of 5 years in Externalizing Problems (p = .049) and Total Problems (p = .042). The predictive power was adequate for all cases, except for Somatic Complaints, and the effect sizes were small and moderate.

Lastly, the independent variable Informant was detected as predictive of the dependent variables Somatic Complaints (p = .001), Aggressive Behavior (p = .016), and Internalizing Problems (p = .002). The parents reported more difficulties than the teachers in these dimensions, regardless of prematurity, measurement time, and gender. The predictive power was not adequate for Aggressive Behavior, and the effect size was small in all three cases.

Discussion

The group of preschool children born preterm without clinical complications or educational gaps had more behavioral problems, as assessed by teachers, than those born at term. The main differences are found at the age of 4 years, especially in the very preterm group. Teachers also observe more internalization problems at the end of the stage, at 6 years of age. However, in general, parents report fewer differences between preterm and non-preterm children. And they report worse results for moderately PTG than for very preterm children.

The teachers reported that, at 4 years of age, very preterm infants showed more difficulties with withdrawal and attention, among others, than term infants. This may indicate a greater risk of presenting behavioral, attention, and/or internalizing symptomatology problems at later stages, especially at school (Aarnoudse-Moens et al., Reference Aarnoudse-Moens, Weisglas-Kuperus, van Goudoever and Oosterlaan2009; Alcántara-Cabanal et al., Reference Alcántara-Canabal, Fernández-Baizán, Solís-Sánchez, Arias and Méndez2020; Arpi & Ferrari, Reference Arpi and Ferrari2013; Kelly et al., Reference Kelly, Arcoleo, D’Agata and Sullivan2023; Perez-Pereira & Baños, Reference Perez-Pereira and Baños2019; Samuelsson et al., Reference Samuelsson, Holsti, Adamsson, Serenius, Hägglöf and Farooqi2017; Schonhaut et al., Reference Schonhaut, Pérez and Astudillo2012).

On the other hand, we found that the preterm group showed more Withdrawal symptomatology in both contexts compared to those born at term. These data are in line with other studies (Bul & van Baar, Reference Bul and van Baar2012; Labayru et al., Reference Labayru, Aliri, Santos, Arrizabalaga, Estevez, Cancela, Gaztañaga, Marti and Sistiaga2021; Samuelsson et al., Reference Samuelsson, Holsti, Adamsson, Serenius, Hägglöf and Farooqi2017), which emphasize that preterm birth was associated with more behavioral difficulties and differed from those found by Scott et al. (Reference Scott, Winchester and Sullivan2018). Furthermore, Perez-Pereira and Baños (Reference Perez-Pereira and Baños2019) found no differences, concluding that behavioral problems are only characteristic of those born very or extremely preterm and not of premature babies in general.

When comparing the subgroups of preterm infants, the parents of the MTPG found more behavioral difficulties than the parents of the very preterm group, especially at ages 4 and 5. The teachers only reported more Somatic Complaints at 5 years of age in this subgroup. They were also associated with more behavioral and attention difficulties (Bul & van Baar, Reference Bul and van Baar2012), and they presented more affective, anxious, and somatic symptoms at school age; these data are consistent with our results at the end of the preschool stage. Recent studies are along the same lines, supporting the growing evidence of risk factors for emotional and behavioral disorders in the MTPG, such as male gender, perinatal infection, and maternal smoking (Des Haan et al., Reference Des Haan, de Kroon, van Dokkum, Kerstjens, Reijneveld and Bos2019). Thus, we concur with the work indicating that births after 32 weeks of gestation should be considered, not only because most of the complications of preterm birth are less frequent and less severe in this population, but also due to the larger number of births per year and their morbidity and mortality (Schonhaut et al., Reference Schonhaut, Pérez and Astudillo2012).

Other studies report a greater likelihood of emotional and behavioral problems in very PTG (Aparecida et al., Reference Aparecida, Pereira and Benzaquen2016; Delobel-Ayoub et al., Reference Delobel-Ayoub, Kaminski, Marret, Burguet, Marchand and Guyen2006; Reijneveld et al., Reference Reijneveld, de Klein and van de Baar2006; Samuelsson et al., Reference Samuelsson, Holsti, Adamsson, Serenius, Hägglöf and Farooqi2017). These results may be due to a more focused follow-up in the very preterm children, as opposed to the moderate and late preterm, whose difficulties may not become apparent until later stages of schooling. In agreement with Arpi and Ferrari (Reference Arpi and Ferrari2013), it is important to emphasize the importance of both long-term follow-up programs and the aspects that may influence them, since behavioral problems in infancy predict more severe difficulties at later stages. This is necessary in both born very prematurely (Rodríguez et al., Reference Rodríguez, Vivas, Cangelosi and Schapira2008) and moderately preterm infants, who have shown more behavioral problems than those born at term and very preterm infants.

The findings of the comparisons between prematurity categories, measurement time, and informants were supported by the multivariate analyses conducted. Thus, the linear models that were valid show that the measurement time is the factor with the greatest influence on emotional and behavioral problems in preschool children, followed by the variable prematurity and informant. That is, the results indicate that, at the age of 6 years, there is a greater risk of presenting difficulties in the preschool stage, that moderately premature children are the most vulnerable group, and that parents identify a greater risk than the teachers. On the contrary, the variable gender had no separate influence on behavior in preschool age, unlike what has been observed in previous studies (Alcántara-Cabanal et al., Reference Alcántara-Canabal, Fernández-Baizán, Solís-Sánchez, Arias and Méndez2020; Aparecida et al., Reference Aparecida, Pereira and Benzaquen2016; Bul & van Baar, Reference Bul and van Baar2012; Des Haan et al., Reference Des Haan, de Kroon, van Dokkum, Kerstjens, Reijneveld and Bos2019).

These manifestations are in line with the findings of Månsson et al. (Reference Månsson, Stjernqvist and Bäckström2014). It is also noteworthy that, at 6 years of age, the teachers of the MTPG reported an increase in many of the behaviors assessed, including the three global scales. Therefore, both parents and teachers coincide in an increase in behavioral difficulties at the age of 6 years. This behavioral evolution could be related to an increase in demands due to the change of educational stage and to the existence of more silent periods in development, which has been pointed out by some authors (Arpi & Ferrari, Reference Arpi and Ferrari2013; Narberhaus & Segarra, Reference Narberhaus and Segarra2004).

The comparison between parents’ and teachers’ views on behavior shows disagreements in the perception of behavior, which highlights the importance of including different informants and contexts. Our results are consistent with those of Bul and Van Baar (Reference Bul and van Baar2012), who found subtle differences in behavior reported by parents and teachers. In addition, they found a greater presence of behavioral and attention difficulties in children born moderately preterm compared to those born at term. Moreover, Labayru et al. (Reference Labayru, Aliri, Santos, Arrizabalaga, Estevez, Cancela, Gaztañaga, Marti and Sistiaga2021) reported that moderate preterm infants are a more vulnerable population and need neuropsychological follow-up to identify emerging difficulties. They advise neurodevelopmental assessment beyond two years of age in this population.

On the other hand, parents experience more difficulties than teachers at the end of preschool and at the beginning of elementary school. We believe that parents could benefit from the support and guidance of professionals in this important transition period in the face of possible sequelae of prematurity in the medium and long term (Iriondo et al., Reference Iriondo, Póo and Ibáñez2006; Salt & Redshaw, Reference Salt and Redshaw2006). Furthermore, Faure et al. (Reference Faure, Habersaat, Harari, Müller-Nix, Borghini, Ansermet, Tolsa and Urben2017) point out the importance of maternal sensitivity in preventing long-term internalization problems in very preterm infants.

Finally, it should be noted that there is no clear agreement in the literature on the predominance of internalizing or externalizing behaviors in preterm infants (Buttha et al., Reference Buttha, Cleves, Casey, Cradok and Anand2002; García et al., Reference García, Cruz-Quintana, Sosa, De la Cruz, Mañas and Pérez-García2012; Klein et al., Reference Klein, Gaspardo, Martinez and Martins2015; Quesada et al., Reference Quesada, Tristão, Pratesi and Wolf2014). This lack of consensus is also observed in the case of very preterm infants (Johnson, Reference Johnson2007). Our findings highlight the presence of internalizing symptomatology in both contexts, which is related to those found in other studies (Aarnoudse-Moens et al., Reference Aarnoudse-Moens, Weisglas-Kuperus, van Goudoever and Oosterlaan2009; Alcántara-Cabanal et al., Reference Alcántara-Canabal, Fernández-Baizán, Solís-Sánchez, Arias and Méndez2020; Samuelsson et al., Reference Samuelsson, Holsti, Adamsson, Serenius, Hägglöf and Farooqi2017). More specifically, we can point out that, in agreement with other authors (Aarnoudse-Moens et al., Reference Aarnoudse-Moens, Weisglas-Kuperus, van Goudoever and Oosterlaan2009; Alcántara-Cabanal et al., Reference Alcántara-Canabal, Fernández-Baizán, Solís-Sánchez, Arias and Méndez2020; Buttha et al., Reference Buttha, Cleves, Casey, Cradok and Anand2002), the main problems in preschoolers born preterm are associated with peer relationships and internalizing symptomatology in general.

Based on the results, we believe that the longitudinal assessment of behavior in preterm preschoolers through information from parents and teachers is essential for the early detection of possible difficulties. Both perspectives complement each other and facilitate a more adequate vision of the child, allowing for interventions adapted to the child’s needs and developmental contexts. Furthermore, we can better understand how different health and educational interventions affect the later development of children born preterm through this type of research. According to Loureiro et al. (Reference Loureiro, Agut, Bonorat and Martínez-Biarge2019), it is clearly a challenge, although, like these authors, we believe it is essential to improve their developmental capacity and well-being in the context in which they live.

As the main limitation of this study, we found that the sample of the subgroups is small, especially that of moderately and late preterm children; thus, the results of this group should be interpreted with caution. It is also important to point out as a possible limitation that the sample of premature infants was recruited from a single hospital; therefore, the generalization of the results to the rest of the population born premature in this community cannot be established. Assessing the possible relationship between parents’ emotional state and children’s behavior (Perez-Pereira & Baños, Reference Perez-Pereira and Baños2019) would be another limitation.

As a proposal for future research, we consider it necessary to expand the sample of the group of moderately and late preterm infants, those born in other hospitals, as well as to assess the emotional state of the parents and their influence on the behavior of their children. For example, Salomäki et al. (Reference Salomäki, Junttila, Setänen, Rautava, Huhtala, Leppänen, Lehtonen and Korja2023) found associations between parental emotional health and social functioning in preterm infants before the age of 4 years. In this sense, we agree with Fisher et al. (Reference Fisher, Miley, Glazer, Gies, Parikh, Lam and Wade2024) in that more intervention studies are needed to improve parenting and prevent future behavioral problems.

According to recent studies, the need to continue to support longitudinal studies that include the preschool stage and follow-ups up to 12 years of age is justified, since subtle social difficulties may appear in the preschool stage (Jones et al., Reference Jones, Champion and Woodward2013), mainly in the group of extremely preterm births (Larsen et al., 2024), since, as Samuelsson et al. (Reference Samuelsson, Holsti, Adamsson, Serenius, Hägglöf and Farooqi2017) state, this profile might be recognizable in adolescence. However, we found that both parents and teachers identified a greater risk of difficulties in the moderately premature children than in those born at term and even the very premature children. For this reason, the extension of follow-up to all preterm infants would facilitate the incorporation of specific self-reports and would allow us to delve further into the relationship between internalizing and externalizing behaviors, as well as into the possible influence of other variables, using other measurement instruments, in line with the recommendation of Camerota et al. (Reference Camerota, Bagley, McGowan, Carter, Check, Dansereau, DellaGrotta, Helderman, Hofheimer, Loncar, Neal, O’Shea, Pastyrnak, Smith, Everson and Lester2024). Finally, studying the influence of contextual variables on the development of PTG is a line of research that should be emphasized, allowing families to express themselves and to be able to resolve their doubts and reduce their insecurity.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/SJP.2025.7.

Data availability statement

Deposito de Investigación de la Universidad de Sevilla. URL: http://idus.us.es.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the premature children, their parents and their teachers, the children of the control group and their families, and our families.

Author contribution

Conceptualization: E.M.P.M. and M.D.L.F; Literature search: A.V.P.C. and M.D.L.F; Methodology: E.M.P.M., M.D.L.F., A.V.P.C. and M.M.B.M; Figures & tables: A.V.P.C.; Data collection: M.M.B.M. and E.M.P.M.; Data interpretation: M.D.L.F., A.V.P.C., E.V.P.M. and M.M.B.M.; Writing-original draft: A.V.P.C., E.M.P.C. and M.D.L.F.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the Spanish Ministerio de Salud, and the Regional Government of Andalusia (PI-0397/2007) (1999/07).

Competing interests

The authors declare none.

References

Aarnoudse-Moens, C. S., Weisglas-Kuperus, N., van Goudoever, J. B., & Oosterlaan, J. (2009). Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics, 124(2), 717728. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-2816CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001a). Manual for the ASEBA preschool-age forms & profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth and Families.Google Scholar
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001b). Manual for the AS EBA school-age forms & profiles. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth and Families.Google Scholar
Alcántara-Canabal, L., Fernández-Baizán, C., Solís-Sánchez, G., Arias, J. L., & Méndez, M. (2020). Identificación de problemas de conducta y emocionales en niños prematuros en el ámbito de atención primaria. Atención Primaria, 52(2), 104111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aprim.2018.11.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allotey, J., Zamora, J., Cheong-See, F., Kalidindi, M., Arroyo-Manzano, D., Asztalos, E., Van Der Post, J., Mol, B., Moore, D., Birtles, D., Khan, K., & Thangaratinam, S. (2018). Cognitive, motor, behavioral and academic performances of children born preterm: A meta-analysis and systematic review involving 64 061 children. BJOG: An International Journal of Obstetrics & Gynaecology, 125(1), 1625. https://doi.org/10.1111/1471-0528.14832CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aparecida, J., Pereira, F. H., & Benzaquen, G. (2016). Quality of life of preterm children: Risk and protective factors. Paidéia, 26(65), 325332.Google Scholar
Arpi, E., & Ferrari, F. (2013). Preterm birth and behavior problems in infants and preschool-age children: A review of the recent literature. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 55(9), 788796. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.12142CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bono, R., Núñez, M. I., & Arnau, J. (2010). Tema 1. Planteamiento de estudios experimentales y cuasiexperimentales [Topic 1. Approach to experimental and quasi-experimental studies [unpublished]. http://www.ub.edu/disin/content/tema-1-planteamiento-de-estudios-experimentales-y-cuasi-experimentalesGoogle Scholar
Bul, K. C. M., & van Baar, A. L. (2012). Behavior problems in relation to sustained selective attention skills of moderately preterm children. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 24, 111123. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-011-9258-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buttha, A., Cleves, M. N., Casey, P., Cradok, M., & Anand, K. (2002). Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of schoolaged children who were born preterm. A meta-analysis. JAMA, 288(6), 728737. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.288.6.728Google Scholar
Camerota, M., Bagley, J., McGowan, E. C., Carter, B. S., Check, J., Dansereau, L. M., DellaGrotta, S. A., Helderman, J. B., Hofheimer, J. A., Loncar, C. M., Neal, C. R., O’Shea, T. M., Pastyrnak, S. L., Smith, L. M., Everson, T. M., & Lester, B. M. (2024). Agreement between two behavioral rating scales in preschoolers born very preterm. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics: JDBP, 45(1), e72e78. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0000000000001238CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cárdenas, M., & Arancibia, H. (2019). Potencia estadística y cálculo del tamaño del efecto en GPower: Complementos a las pruebas de significación estadística y su aplicación en psicología [Statistical power and effect size calculation in GPower: Complements to statistical significance testing and its application in psychology]. Salud y Sociedad, 5(2), 210224. https://www.redalyc.org/pdf/4397/439742475006.pdfCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delobel-Ayoub, M., Kaminski, M., Marret, S., Burguet, A., Marchand, L., Guyen, S. N, & EPIPAGE Study Group. (2006). Behavioral outcome at three years of age in very preterm infants: The Epipage Study. Pediatrics, 117(6), 19962005. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2005-2310CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faure, N., Habersaat, S., Harari, M. M., Müller-Nix, C., Borghini, A., Ansermet, F., Tolsa, J. F., & Urben, S. (2017). Maternal sensitivity: A resilience factor against internalizing symptoms in early adolescents born very preterm? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45(4), 671680. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-016-0194-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, A. P., Miley, A. E., Glazer, S., Gies, L. M., Parikh, N. A., Lam, L., & Wade, S. L. (2024). Feasibility and acceptability of an online parenting intervention to address behavior problems in moderately to extremely preterm pre-school and school-age children. Child: Care, Health and Development, 50(1), e13209. https://doi.org/10.1111/cch.13209CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García, O., Cruz-Quintana, F., Sosa, M. A., De la Cruz, J., Mañas, M., & Pérez-García, M. (2012). Alteraciones neuropsicológicas y emocionales en niños prematuros de muy bajo peso al nacer [Neuropsychological and emotional alterations in very low birth weight preterm infants]. Revista Argentina de Ciencias del Comportamiento, 4(2), 310. https://doi.org/10.32348/1852.4206.v4.n2.5169CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gómez, C., Sánchez, J. J., García, F., & Segovia, J. M. (2019). Morbilidades del neurodesarrollo asociadas con el nacimiento pretérmino con peso ≤1500 gramos entre 1993 y 2011 en España: estudio de una muestra de 1200 casos [Neurodevelopmental morbidities associated with preterm birth weighing ≤1500 grams between 1993 and 2011 in Spain: Study of a sample of 1200 cases]. Revista Española de Discapacidad, 7, 2947. https://doi.org/10.5569/2340-5104.07.01.02CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Des Haan, P. J., de Kroon, M. L. A., van Dokkum, N. H., Kerstjens, J. M., Reijneveld, S. A., & Bos, A. F. (2019). Risk factors for emotional and al problems in moderately-late preterms. PLoS ONE, 14(5), e0216468. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0216468CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iriondo, M., Póo, P., & Ibáñez, M. (2006). Seguimiento del recién nacido de riesgo [Follow-up of the newborn at risk]. Anales de Pediatría Continuada, 4(6), 344353.Google Scholar
Jin, J. H., Yoon, S. W., Song, J., Kim, S. W., & Chung, H. J. (2020). Long-term cognitive, executive, and behavioral outcomes of moderate and late preterm at school age. Clinical and Experimental Pediatrics, 63(6), 219. https://doi.org/10.3345/kjp.2019.00647CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johns, C. (2019). The worsening trajectory of social impairment in preterm born young adults and its association with altered amygdala functional connectivity. [Doctoral dissertation, University of Yale]. Yale Medicine Thesis Digital Library. 3506. https://elischolar.library.yale.edu/ymtdl/3506Google Scholar
Johnson, S. (2007). Cognitive and behavioral outcomes following very preterm birth. Seminars in Fetal Neonatal Medicine, 2(5), 363373. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2007.05.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, S., Matthews, R., Draper, E. S., Field, D. J., Manktelow, B. N., Marlow, N., Smith, L. K., & Boyle, E. M. (2015). Early emergence of delayed social competence in infants born late and moderately preterm. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics: JDBP, 36(9), 690699. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0000000000000222CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, K. M., Champion, P. R., & Woodward, L. J. (2013) Social competence of preschool children born very preterm. Early Human Development, 89, 795802. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.06.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, M. M., Arcoleo, K., D’Agata, A. L., & Sullivan, M. C. (2023). A test of differential susceptibility in behavior trajectories of preterm infants from preschool to adulthood. Research in Nursing & Health, 46(1), 8092. https://doi.org/10.1002/nur.22275CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, V. C., Gaspardo, C. M., Martinez, F. E., & Martins, M. B. (2015). Neonatal characteristics and temperament predict behavior problems in children born preterm. Journal of Human Growth and Development, 25(3), 331340. https://doi.org/10.7322/jhgd.103750CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labayru, G., Aliri, J., Santos, A., Arrizabalaga, A., Estevez, M., Cancela, V., Gaztañaga, M., Marti, I., & Sistiaga, A. (2021). Small for gestational age moderate to late preterm children: A neuropsychological follow-up. Developmental Neuropsychology, 46(4), 277287. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2021.1939349CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsen, J., Kochhar, P., Wolke, D., Draper, E. S., Marlow, N., & Johnson, S. (2024). Comparing behavioral outcomes in children born extremely preterm between 2006 and 1995: The EPICure studies. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 33(5), 15171528. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-023-02258-wCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loureiro, B., Agut, T., Bonorat, N., & Martínez-Biarge, M. (2019). Seguimiento a medio-largo plazo de los niños prematuros y sus familias en nuestro país [Medium to long-term follow-up of premature infants and their families in our country]. Anales de Pediatría, 91(3), 139141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anpedi.2019.06.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Månsson, J., Stjernqvist, K., & Bäckström, M. (2014). Behavioral outcomes at corrected age 2.5 years in children born extremely preterm. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 35(7), 435442. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0000000000000082CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Meagher, S., Norris, K., Kemp, N., & Anderson, P. (2019). Parent and teacher reporting of executive function and behavioral difficulties in preterm and term children at kindergarten. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 9(2), 153164. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2018.1550404CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narberhaus, A., & Segarra, D. (2004). Trastornos neuropsicológicos y del desarrollo en el prematuro [Neuropsychological and developmental disorders in the premature infant]. Anales de Psicología, 20, 317326.Google Scholar
Pallás Alonso, C. R. (2012). Actividades preventivas y de promoción de la salud para niños prematuros con edad gestacional menor de 32 semanas o peso menor de 1500 gr.: Desde el alta hospitalaria hasta los siete años (Parte 2) [Preventive and health promotion activities for preterm infants with gestational age less than 32 weeks or weight less than 1500 grams: From hospital discharge to seven years of age (Part 2)]. Revista de Pediatría de Atención Primaria, 14(55), 249263. https://doi.org/10.4321/S1139-76322012000400014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perez-Pereira, M., & Baños, L. (2019). ¿Los niños prematuros saludables tienen problemas de conducta? [Do healthy preterm children have behavior problems?]. Anales de Psicología, 35(3), 397404. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.35.3.332531CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quesada, A. A., Tristão, R. M., Pratesi, R., & Wolf, O. T. (2014). Hyper-responsiveness to acute stress, emotional problems and poorer memory in former preterm children. Stress, 17(5), 389399. https://doi.org/10.3109/10253890.2014.949667CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reijneveld, S. A., de Klein, M. J. K., & van de Baar, A. L. (2006). Behavioral and emotional problems in very preterm and very low birthweight infants at age 5 years. Archives of Disease in Childhood Fetal and Neonatal, 91, 423428. https://doi.org/10.1136/adc.2006.093674CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodríguez, G., Vivas, S., Cangelosi, M., & Schapira, I. (2008). Avatares en prematuros de 3 a 5 años y sus madres. Revista del Hospital Materno Infantil Ramón Sardá, 27(4), 152154.Google Scholar
Salomäki, S., Junttila, N., Setänen, S., Rautava, P., Huhtala, M., Leppänen, M., Lehtonen, L., & Korja, R. (2023). Longitudinal associations between parental early psychological distress and children’s emotional and behavioral problems during early childhood and self-reported social functioning in 11-year-old children born very preterm. European Journal of Developmental Psychology , Early online. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2023.2276484Google Scholar
Salt, A., & Redshaw, M. (2006). Neurodevelopmental follow-up after preterm birth: Follow up after two years. Early Human Development, 82(3), 185197. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2005.12.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samuelsson, M., Holsti, A., Adamsson, M., Serenius, F., Hägglöf, B., & Farooqi, A. (2017) Behavioral patterns in adolescents born at 23 to 25 weeks of gestation. Pediatrics 140 (1), 111. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2017-0199CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schonhaut, L., Pérez, M., & Astudillo, J. (2012). Prematuros tardíos: un grupo de riesgo de morbilidad a corto y largo plazo [Late preterm infants: A group at risk of short- and long-term morbidity]. Revista Chilena de Pediatría, 83(3), 217223. https://doi.org/10.4067/S0370-41062012000300002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, A., Winchester, S. B., & Sullivan, M. C. (2018). Trajectories of problem behaviors from 4 to 23 years in former preterm infants. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 42(2), 237247. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025417692899CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization. (2023). Nacimientos prematuros [Premature births]. https://www.who.int/es/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/preterm-birth.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Preterm children born.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Sample data flow.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Sociodemographic variables of the sample of premature infants.GE = Gestational Age; χ (dt) = mean and standard deviation; Interval = highest and lowest score; N (%)= group size and percentage of total; GE (weeks) = gestational age at birth by weeks.

Figure 3

Table 1. Behavioral variables and prematurity

Figure 4

Table 2. Behavioral variables and time of measurement

Figure 5

Table 3. Behavioral variables and informant

Figure 6

Table 4. Behavioral variables and gender

Figure 7

Table 5. Influence of prematurity, time of measurement, sex and informants on behaviour

Supplementary material: File

Padilla-Muñoz et al. supplementary material

Padilla-Muñoz et al. supplementary material
Download Padilla-Muñoz et al. supplementary material(File)
File 145.6 KB