Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T12:31:01.559Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Attitude Strength: What’s New?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 April 2023

Richard E. Petty
Affiliation:
The Ohio State University (USA)
Joseph J. Siev
Affiliation:
The Ohio State University (USA)
Pablo Briñol
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (Spain)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Attitude strength (what makes attitudes durable and impactful) has become an important topic in the domain of social influence. We review three areas in which the traditional view of attitude strength has been modified or updated since the publication of Petty and Krosnick’s 1995 edited book on the topic. First, although it was widely assumed that there were different categories of strength variables (i.e., operative versus meta-cognitive), it may now be better to recognize that each strength property can be measured both structurally and subjectively and that each measure is useful. Second, although scholars assumed that virtually all persuasion techniques would work better on weaker than stronger attitudes, recent research suggests that some techniques might actually work better on stronger than weaker attitudes. Third, although stronger attitudes often guide behavior better than weaker ones, when strength is challenged or weak attitudes are threatening, people can be motivated to act to demonstrate or restore certainty. This can result in weaker attitudes leading to more extreme behavior.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de la Psicología de Madrid

Despite the longstanding status of attitudes as the most indispensable concept in social psychology (Allport, Reference Allport and Murchison1935), it has had a troubled history in regard to its predictive power (e.g., Wicker, Reference Wicker1971). That is, although there is widespread agreement that attitudes refer to a person’s general evaluations (e.g., good-bad) of objects, issues, and people (e.g., Petty & Cacioppo, Reference Petty and Cacioppo1981; Thurstone, Reference Thurstone1928), research has not always supported the seemingly obvious notion that people’s overall attitudes (e.g., supporting the environment) would guide their actions (e.g., recycling cardboard boxes; Weigel & Newman, Reference Weigel and Newman1976). Thus, some social psychologists turned to the notion that some attitudes were more consequential than others. For example, Festinger (Reference Festinger1957) pointed to attitude importance as one such moderating variable in his wide-ranging theory of cognitive dissonance. Attitude importance remains a relevant concept in the literature today (Howe & Krosnick, Reference Howe and Krosnick2017).

The notion that some attitudes could be more consequential than others has also been popular in fields outside of social psychology, especially among survey researchers in sociology and political science (e.g., Sample & Warland, Reference Sample and Warland1973; Schuman & Johnson, Reference Schuman and Johnson1976). By the late 1980s, social psychologists had coalesced around calling this difference in attitudes, attitude strength, but as Raden (Reference Raden1985) noted, this term was “not defined with any precision” and it did “not appear to have any agreed-upon meaning” (p. 312). A decade later, Krosnick and Petty (Reference Krosnick, Petty, Petty and Krosnick1995) defined attitude strength in terms of its consequences. That is, attitude strength (much like strength in a person) was defined in terms of what makes attitudes “durable and impactful.” This definition has attained considerable acceptance (e.g., Luttrell & Sawicki, Reference Luttrell and Sawicki2020). In this definition, durability referred to the extent to which an attitude persisted over time and resisted change. Impact referred to whether an attitude influenced other judgments, information processing, and behavior.

This definition, however, did not address the question of what it is that determines whether attitudes are indeed durable and impactful. As this review will make clear, the response to this question is more complex than the simple definition of strength might suggest. For example, in an early paper, Scott (Reference Scott, Lindzey and Aronson1968) identified 10 ways in which attitudes could differ besides their overall positivity/negativity. Some of these attitude properties have received considerable research attention over the years as strength features of attitudes (e.g., ambivalence), but others have not been studied much at all (e.g., overtness). Still other strength features of attitudes that have become very popular over time were completely ignored by Scott (e.g., attitude certainty). Subsequently, Raden (Reference Raden1985) and Krosnick and Petty (Reference Krosnick, Petty, Petty and Krosnick1995) referred to some of these attitude properties as attitude-strength dimensions, attributes, and/or aspects. Luttrell and Sawicki (Reference Luttrell and Sawicki2020) referred to them as predictors of strength to distinguish them from the “defining features.”Footnote 1 Regardless of the label applied, scholars agree that many of these measurable aspects of attitudes are linked to strength outcomes in some way, usually in one direction (e.g., more certainty is associated with greater strength consequences), but as described shortly, sometimes the direction can be reversed.

Research on the various strength attributes occurred mostly in isolation for many years culminating in an edited volume on strength (Attitude Strength: Antecedents and Consequences, Petty & Krosnick, Reference Petty and Krosnick1995) in which a variety of authors reviewed research on their favored strength dimension. A primary goal of the current article is to review what was assumed about attitude strength around the time of this 1995 book, and then specify some key advances in understanding since then. Although research on attitude strength has become a vast enterprise with much interesting work being conducted, our brief review focuses primarily on what we see as some of the most critical differences in assumptions now versus then.

We begin, however, with an assumption that has not changed. That is, by 1995, numerous researchers aimed to address the question of whether attitude strength features constituted one overall strength factor (i.e., the various properties were correlated with each other forming a unified construct), or whether there were several factor clusters, or perhaps every identified strength property was unique in some way. In 1985, Raden concluded that it is “far from certain that attitude strength is a global, unitary property,” (p. 312). Since then, a number of researchers have proposed different ways of clustering the various attitude strength dimensions (e.g., Abelson, Reference Abelson1988; Erber et al.,Reference Erber, Hodges, Wilson, Petty and Krosnick1995; Phillip-Muller et al., Reference Phillip-Muller, Wallace and Wegener2020; Pomerantz et al., Reference Pomerantz, Chaiken and Tordesillas1995; see Visser et al., Reference Visser, Bizer and Krosnick2006, for a review), but no one approach to clustering has achieved consensus or dominated the literature. Thus, we suggest that Krosnick and Petty’s (Reference Krosnick, Petty, Petty and Krosnick1995) conclusion that “the various attributes of attitudes that may contribute to strength are best thought of as distinct from one another” (p. 17), remains valid today.Footnote 2

Identifying and Measuring Attitude Strength Indicators

New Strength Indicators

Although we noted that it still seems best to treat the numerous attitude strength indicators as conceptually and empirically distinct, there has been some progress in identifying new attitude strength indicators and in conceptualizing the measurement of the various strength dimensions. In the Petty and Krosnick (Reference Petty and Krosnick1995) volume, the dimensions of strength covered in one or more chapters were: Accessibility, certainty, elaboration, extremity, importance, involvement, knowledge, and structural consistency. Perhaps the most studied new dimension of strength identified since then is the extent to which the attitude is based in one’s morals (Skitka et al., Reference Skitka, Bauman and Sargis2005). This strength feature has been linked to nearly all of the strength consequences of interest (e.g., morally based attitudes tend to be more resistant to change than attitudes based in practicality; see Skitka et al., Reference Skitka, Hanson, Morgan and Wisneski2021).

Another dimension is whether the attitude is more based on affect or cognition. Although this is a long-studied feature of attitudes (e.g., Rosenberg & Hovland, Reference Rosenberg, Hovland, Rosenberg, Hovland, McGuire, Abelson and Brehm1960), it is only relatively recent evidence that suggests it is a strength property (e.g., affectively based attitudes are more accessible and less likely to change over time than cognitively based attitudes; Giner-Sorolla, Reference Giner-Sorolla2004; Rocklage & Luttrell, Reference Rocklage and Luttrell2021). Three other dimensions that are gaining traction as strength properties include whether the attitude is self-defining, linked to one’s identity, or based more in negativity than positivity. That is, attitudes and choices that are tied to one’s sense of self (Zunick et al., Reference Zunick, Teeny and Fazio2017), linked to a particular group identity (Xu & Petty, Reference Xu and Pettyin press), and based more on opposition to one’s non-preferred option than support for one’s preferred option (Bizer & Petty, Reference Bizer and Petty2005) appear to be stronger, at least in some circumstances (cf., Catapano & Tormala, Reference Catapano and Tormala2021; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Lelkes, Hawkins and Thedoridis2022).

Two Methods of Measuring Strength Indicators

With respect to measuring the various strength properties that have been identified, perhaps the most influential proposal in the late 1990s stemmed from John Bassili (Reference Bassili1996) who categorized the measurement of strength dimensions into what he called operative and meta-cognitive approaches (see also Wegener et al., Reference Wegener, Downing, Krosnick, Petty, Petty and Krosnick1995). In essence, meta-cognitive properties were assessed with subjective impressions of one’s attitudes. For example, people would be asked if they were certain of their opinions, or if an attitude was important. In contrast, operative properties were those aspects of attitudes that were assessed with more objective and structural measures. For example, a researcher could measure how quickly an attitude came to mind for accessibility or how much knowledge a person was able to list about the attitude object. The different strength properties were then categorized as to whether they were typically assessed with subjective reports (e.g., certainty), making them meta-cognitive, or more objective measures (e.g., accessibility), making them operative. Furthermore, Bassili argued that “operative measures of attitude strength have an advantage in gauging the strength of an attitude” (p. 638). Meta-attitudinal properties were not viewed as useful because they lacked a structural basis (i.e., “impressions of attitudes are seldom represented in memory,” p. 638).

In contrast to considering some strength dimensions as operative and others as meta-cognitive, an alternative approach is to recognize that many of the strength dimensions can be and have been measured with both kinds of assessments. For example, knowledge has been assessed by asking people how much knowledge they believed they possessed (subjective) and also by measuring the amount of knowledge they could list (objective; Davidson et al., Reference Davidson, Yantis, Norwood and Montano1985). Ambivalence has been assessed both by measuring how many positive and negative reactions people have to an attitude object (objective measure; Kaplan, Reference Kaplan1972) and also by asking them how conflicted they feel about the object (subjective measure; Priester & Petty, Reference Priester and Petty1996). Furthermore, dimensions that Bassili identified as operative can and have been assessed with subjective measures. For example, attitude accessibility, typically assessed with a reaction time procedure (Schuette & Fazio, Reference Schuette and Fazio1995), has also been measured by asking people to report how quickly their attitude comes to mind (Tormala et al., Reference Tormala, Clarkson and Henderson2011). And, dimensions such as certainty that typically were assessed with self-reports (e.g., Petrocelli et al., Reference Petrocelli, Tormala and Rucker2007), could also be assessed in a more objective/structural manner. For example, in one study, Petty and colleagues (Reference Petty, Tormala, Briñol and Jarvis2006) used an implicit association test (IAT; Greenwald et al., Reference Greenwald, McGhee and Schwartz1998) to examine if the attitude object was more quickly associated with words like “confident” and “sure” rather than “doubtful” and “skeptical.”

Krosnick and Petty (Reference Krosnick, Petty, Petty and Krosnick1995) originally recognized that when a given dimension was assessed both subjectively and objectively, the two different methods of assessment sometimes showed very low correlations with each other, even if the outcomes were similar. Furthermore, they suggested that this lack of relation between the two measures of the same construct might reflect either: (a) That each measure assessed the same construct but imperfectly, or (b) that each measure reflected information that was “useful, valid, and independent” (p. 16). There are several lines of more recent research that strongly support this second possibility.

One way of showing that each type of measurement of the same strength dimension might be independently useful is to disentangle them completely (i.e., break any correlation). For example, although the amount of thinking a person has done about an attitude object tends to correlate well with the perceived amount of thinking, Barden and Petty (Reference Barden and Petty2008) showed that the perceived amount of elaboration was sufficient to enhance attitude-behavior consistency even if it was completely dissociated from the actual amount of thinking. To demonstrate this, in one study college students read some strong arguments in favor of a new university internet policy. They then took a quiz on this message that was rigged to suggest that they performed well (an easy quiz) or poorly (a very difficult quiz). They were explicitly told that their quiz performance reflected how much they had thought about the message. Although this induction did not affect participants’ attitudes to the proposal, it did impact how much thought they believed they put into the message. Importantly, this enhanced perception of thought affected how certain they felt in their attitudes and how much they were willing to act on them even though their perception of elaboration was not linked to any actual amount of thought (see also Moreno et al., Reference Moreno, Requero, Santos, Paredes, Briñol and Petty2021, Reference Moreno, Briñol and Petty2023).

Other ways of showing the utility of each type of measure are to: (a) Demonstrate that they can predict unique variance in a given outcome, (b) can predict the same outcome, but in different situations, and (c) can predict different outcomes. This approach was taken in a series of studies by See and colleagues (Reference See, Petty and Fabrigar2008, Reference See, Fabrigar and Petty2013) examining the affective versus cognitive bases of attitudes. In particular, See and colleagues compared a traditional objective measure of the affective versus cognitive basis of attitudes (i.e., examining whether affect or cognition scales predicted attitudes better; Crites et al., Reference Crites, Fabrigar and Petty1994) with a newly developed subjective measure (i.e., simply asking people to report whether their attitudes were more affectively or cognitively based). These different measures were not correlated and each measure was shown to predict unique variance in a particular outcome (i.e., susceptibility to a persuasive message). Furthermore, each measure was shown to be better at predicting under different circumstances. For example, the subjective measure was better at predicting interest in information seeking when deliberation was high, but the structural measure was better when deliberation was low.Footnote 3 Finally, each measure was also shown to be capable of predicting different outcomes. In one study, the objective measure was associated with faster reading speed for relevant information (i.e., affective relative to cognitive information was processed faster as attitudes were more objectively affectively based), whereas the subjective measure predicted slower reading speed for relevant information. The explanation was that the objective measure was related to processing efficiency (ability) whereas the subjective measure reflected processing interest (motivation). These results clearly suggested that the different ways of assessing the same dimension could each be useful in their own right.

Given these different outcomes, See et al. (Reference See, Fabrigar and Petty2013) suggested that it could be that subjective and objective measures of many of the traditional dimensions of attitude strength (e.g., ambivalence), when assessed with objective versus subjective measures, could also operate differently. In sum, the traditional view of objective versus subjective measures of attitude strength dimensions was either that the objective method of measurement was superior, or that if the subjective measure predicted at all, it was because the self-report reflected the underlying structural construct. However, the new view is that objective and subjective measures of the same strength dimension can each be valuable and operate via different mechanisms in different situations. Thus, although some reviewers continue to treat studies employing objective and subjective measures of the same dimension similarly because the outcomes are often similar (e.g., Luttrell & Sawicki, Reference Luttrell and Sawicki2020), we suspect that in the future, more research will begin to show the utility of treating the different measures of the same dimension differently (see also, See et al., Reference See, Fabrigar and Petty2023).

Methods that Can Change Strong Attitudes More than Weak Ones

So far, we have seen that since the 1995 book on Attitude Strength was published, new dimensions of strength have been identified, and progress has been made in understanding the implications of different ways of measuring these constructs. Another area in which progress has been made is in understanding how to change strong attitudes. That is, the prevailing wisdom was that virtually all persuasion techniques would work better in changing weak rather than strong attitudes because the latter attitudes were generally more resistant to change. For example, the greater knowledge that accompanies strong attitudes would give people the ability to resist (e.g., counterargue) better and the higher certainty that accompanies these attitudes would provide higher motivation to resist. Because it was assumed that it is generally easier to change weak rather than strong attitudes, many persuasion studies have avoided examining attitude topics for which attitudes were likely to be strong (e.g., important topics like abortion).

Using Matched Messages

In contrast to the prevailing view, the accumulated research, and especially recent research, has suggested that it is possible to influence strong attitudes with appropriate techniques. Perhaps the earliest indication that strong attitudes could be changed with a particular type of appeal came from work on influencing cognitively versus affectively based attitudes. Recall, that contemporary research suggests that attitudes based primarily on emotion tend to be stronger than attitudes based primarily on cognition (Giner-Sorolla, Reference Giner-Sorolla2004; Rocklage & Luttrell, Reference Rocklage and Luttrell2021). This would indicate that the generic persuasive message, which typically uses persuasive arguments, should work better on attitudes that were primarily cognitively rather than affectively-based. Thus, it was noteworthy when Edwards (Reference Edwards1990) proposed that perhaps affectively-based attitudes could be changed more easily than cognitively-based attitudes if an emotional message was used. More specifically, the proposal was that persuasion would be best when the message was congruent with (or matched) the basis of the attitude.

This matching technique was originally shown to be effective in the domain of changing attitudes with different functional bases (e.g., using a self-image-oriented message to change an attitude based on social concerns; Snyder & DeBono, Reference Snyder and DeBono1985; see Teeny et al., Reference Teeny, Siev, Briñol and Petty2021, for a review of matching effects in persuasion). Indeed, Edwards was able to show that for an affectively-based (relatively strong) attitude, an affective message produced even more change than for the cognitively-based (relatively weak) attitude. Since the original research, this matching effect has been replicated consistently (e.g., Fabrigar & Petty, Reference Fabrigar and Petty1999; See et al., Reference See, Petty and Fabrigar2008), though there are some exceptions (Clarkson et al., Reference Clarkson, Tormala and Rucker2011; Millar & Millar, Reference Millar and Millar1990).

One reason that matching can work better than mismatching is that the match can motivate or enable people to think more carefully about the message than a mismatch (e.g., DeBono & Harnish, Reference DeBono and Harnish1988; see Teeny et al., Reference Teeny, Siev, Briñol and Petty2021). Tormala and colleagues (Reference Tormala, Rucker and Seger2008) relied on this matching idea to show how one could effectively appeal to people who were high rather than low in their attitude certainty. For a typical persuasive message, one reason that people high in certainty are difficult to change is that they are unlikely to process the message arguments. Since they are already certain of their view, they presumably think there is little to learn and thus their minds are closed to the message. Because of this, higher certainty is typically associated with greater resistance to persuasion (e.g., see Mello et al., Reference Mello, Garcia-Marques, Briñol, Cancela and Petty2020; Rucker et al., Reference Rucker, Tormala, Petty and Briñol2014). But, what if the message appeal was matched to people with high certainty? To investigate this, Tormala et al. (Reference Tormala, Rucker and Seger2008) first induced people to feel relatively high or low in their general confidence by generating prior instances in which they felt confidence or doubt (Petty et al., Reference Petty, Briñol and Tormala2002). Then, some participants received a message with strong arguments advocating for a change in university policy. The message was either said to be designed to promote confidence (confidence match condition) or they received no such information (control condition). In the control condition, those made to feel confident were less persuaded by the strong arguments than those who were made to feel doubtful, replicating work showing that enhanced confidence is associated with more resistance. However, when the message had the confidence frame, those in the confidence condition were more influenced than those in the doubt condition, reversing the traditional effect. Thus, just like presenting affective arguments to those with an affectively based attitude can overcome resistance, so too can presenting a message with a confidence frame to those who are feeling confident. In each case, the match could inspire people to process the strong arguments more carefully, engendering persuasion (Petty & Cacioppo, Reference Petty and Cacioppo1990).

Even more recently, the matching technique has been applied to changing attitudes that have a moral rather than a practical basis. As noted earlier, morally-based attitudes are seen as particularly strong and difficult to change (Luttrell, Petty, Briñol, & Wagner, Reference Luttrell, Petty, Briñol and Wagner2016; Skitka et al., Reference Skitka, Hanson, Morgan and Wisneski2021). In a series of studies applying the matching notion to this strength variable, Luttrell and colleagues (Reference Luttrell, Phillip–Muller and Petty2019) measured the extent to which participants’ attitudes on various topics (recycling, legalizing marijuana) were based on their morals. Then the participants were randomly assigned to receive a message making moral arguments (e.g., recycling releases toxic fumes that are harmful to pets…”) or practical ones (e.g., recycling requires an increase in trucks that increase traffic congestion). The practical message became increasingly less effective in influencing attitudes as the moral basis of those attitudes increased, consistent with the typical resistance shown to persuasion as attitudes become stronger. However, the moral message showed the opposite effect, becoming increasingly more effective as the moral basis of attitudes increased. In fact, the moral message had an easier time in persuading people with stronger (more moral) than weaker (more practical) attitudes (see also Luttrell & Petty, Reference Luttrell and Petty2021).

Using Two-Sided Messages

Although the matching technique has had some success in changing strong attitudes, implementation can be cumbersome. That is, changing strong attitudes with matched messages requires knowing the reason why the attitude is strong and then developing a message that is tailored to that attitude basis. In a recent series of studies, Xu and Petty (Reference Xu and Petty2022, in press) examined a strategy that they believed would be more generic than the matching approach. In particular, they argued that using a two-sided rather than the more typical one-sided message could be particularly effective for those whose attitudes were strong. One-sided messages only present arguments on the advocated side, but two-sided messages also acknowledge some good points on the opposite side in addition to presenting arguments for the preferred side. Research on one- versus two-sided messages has a long history in social psychology (Hovland et al., Reference Hovland, Janis and Kelley1953) and various moderators of their differential effectiveness have been uncovered (e.g., Rucker et al., Reference Rucker, Petty and Briñol2008; see Crowley & Hoyer, Reference Crowley and Hoyer1994, for a review). Yet, no prior research had examined the relative effectiveness of these different message types for relatively strong versus weak attitudes.

Xu and Petty (Reference Xu and Petty2022) reasoned that those who cared deeply about their attitudes (i.e., people with strong attitudes) would be especially appreciative of a message that recognized some validity to their side. Then, in accord with the principle of reciprocity (Cialdini & Goldstein, Reference Cialdini and Goldstein2004), they would be more willing to acknowledge some validity to the advocated side. Although everyone would presumably show some appreciation for their side being respected in the persuasive message, the acknowledgement should be especially appreciated by those with strong attitudes. In one study, the moral basis of people’s attitudes toward gun control was assessed and then the participants were presented with a persuasive message that advocated against their own view. The message was either entirely one-sided or presented both sides. That is, the two-sided message presented some arguments on the recipient’s side in addition to the same arguments as in the one-sided advocacy. To examine the reciprocity hypothesis, the recipient’s openness (receptiveness) to the side advocated was assessed (see Hussein & Tormala, Reference Hussein and Tormala2021; Minson et al., Reference Minson, Chen and Tinsley2020; Ottati & Stern, Reference Ottati and Sternin press). The results, depicted in Figure 1, showed that for the one-sided message, the typical attitude strength effect occurred – people were more resistant to the message the more morally based their attitudes were. However, for the two-sided message, the opposite occurred – the stronger the attitude was, the more open it was to the other side. In this study Xu and Petty also showed that appreciation for the author mediated this openness effect, and furthermore, the more open to the other side the recipient reported being, the more favorable their attitudes became toward the advocated position. In another study, the interaction between moral basis (attitude strength) and message-sidedness on openness was shown for a message advocating mask wearing during Covid–19 for people who were initially against it.

Figure 1. Interaction between the Extent to which Attitudes are Based on Morality and Message-Sidedness on Openness to a Counterattitudinal Message on Capital Punishment

Note. Adapted from Xu and Petty (Reference Xu and Petty2022).

In this work, Xu and Petty (Reference Xu and Petty2022) also demonstrated that to be effective, the two-sided message should mention a strong argument on the recipient’s side. If a weak argument was instead presented, the two-sided message was no longer any more effective than the one-sided communication (i.e., the two-sided message should respect the participant’s view). It is also noteworthy that in a series of subsequent studies, Xu and Petty (in press) showed that the relative effectiveness of a two- over a one-sided message held not only when strength was based in morality, but also when for a non-moral topic (e.g., dental hygiene), strength was indexed by a measure of attitude certainty. The power of a two-sided message to render strong attitudes more open to the other side was even shown in the domain of politics. That is, when attitude strength was indexed with a measure of identity with the participant’s political party, the higher the participant’s political identity was, the more open the person was to a two-sided message but the less open they were to a one-sided communication. In sum, although it is generally the case that strong attitudes are more resistant to change than weak ones, recent research has demonstrated that there are particular techniques that can be effective in rendering strong attitudes more open to change.

When Weak Attitudes Predict Behavior Better than Strong Ones

In the persuasion work just described, we have seen that particular kinds of messages can become more effective as attitude strength is increased. Thus, although at the time of the 1995 book, it was assumed that persuasion techniques generally do not work as well on stronger attitudes, our new understanding is that some techniques can actually work better on stronger attitudes. In this final section, we turn to some other situations in which the traditional wisdom regarding strong attitudes has been turned on its head. To illustrate this, we focus on attitude certainty as a variable whose traditional findings can be reversed in certain circumstances. Attitude certainty is an important strength dimension to understand since it is one of the most studied strength variables and over the longest period of time (Tormala & Rucker, Reference Tormala and Rucker2018). Indeed, even before the concept of attitude strength was widely recognized, researchers pointed to how two people could hold the very same judgment but vary in their judgmental confidence (Johnson, Reference Johnson1945). Most importantly, this early research even showed that measured certainty in attitudes could moderate attitude-behavior consistency, with more certain attitudes predicting behavior better (Sample & Warland, Reference Sample and Warland1973; Warland & Sample, Reference Warland and Sample1973).

Undermining Certainty

The early result that enhanced attitude certainty is related to greater attitude behavior consistency (ABC) has generally held over time (Rucker et al., Reference Rucker, Tormala, Petty and Briñol2014), but not universally. In particular, there is some reasonable evidence that undermining people’s feelings of certainty in their attitudes can sometimes motivate them to engage in certainty restoring actions. For example, in one study Gal and Rucker (Reference Gal and Rucker2010) had participants write their thoughts on animal testing of products with either their dominant or non-dominant hand. Prior research had shown that writing with the non-dominant hand undermines confidence in what was written (Briñol & Petty, Reference Briñol and Petty2003). Participants were then asked to write what they would say to someone they would like to convert to their own position. Those in the doubt condition (writing with the non-dominant hand) used significantly more words in their advocacy suggesting a greater need to bolster their own viewFootnote 4 (see also, Clarkson et al., Reference Clarkson, Smith, Tormala and Dugan2017; Sawicki & Wegener, Reference Sawicki and Wegener2018, for conceptually similar findings). In addition to restoring certainty by strongly advocating for one’s attitude, another potentially restoring action is to adopt more extreme beliefs and actions. Adopting extreme attitudes and actions signals to oneself and to others that conviction is high. The desire to remove uncertainty (and restore certainty) should be especially prominent when that uncertainty is threatening in some way. McGregor (Reference McGregor, Spencer, Fein, Zanna and Olson2003, Reference McGregor2006) argued that people could compensate for feelings of threating uncertainty by bolstering their attitudes which can render them more extreme than they were initially (see also Hart, Reference Hart2014; Heine et al., Reference Heine, Proulx and Vohs2006; Hogg, Reference Hogg2014; Horcajo et al., Reference Horcajo, Briñol, Paredes, Petty, DeMarree and See2022; McGregor et al., Reference McGregor, Zanna, Holmes and Spencer2001).

Relatively recent work on understanding fanatical behavior (e.g., engaging in aggression for one’s cause) has produced conceptually similar results. In particular, Gollwitzer and colleagues (Reference Gollwitzer, Olcaysoy Okten, Pizarro and Oettingen2022) examined what they called discordant knowing, a situation in which people feel certain of something but then learn that their position is opposed by other people. Learning that others disagree with you, especially liked or important others, can presumably shake one’s confidence and produce feelings of conflict regarding the attitude (i.e., subjective ambivalence, Priester & Petty, Reference Priester and Petty2001).Footnote 5 In a series of studies, Gollwitzer et al. found that discordant knowing was associated with a willingness to join extreme groups and vote for more extreme candidates.Footnote 6 Furthermore, this effect was mediated by feelings of threat associated with the disagreement from others. That is, when people had their highly certain attitudes challenged by learning that others disagreed with them, this produced a feeling of threat which resulted in higher support for extreme candidates and actions. In one study, participants who were certain about their moral views on abortion exhibited greater fanaticism in response to an article that challenged that view by indicating that most others opposed it. In other studies, the same fanaticism was not increased when people learned that others agreed with their views, even though this information enhanced their certainty. Thus, challenging, attacking, or attempting to undermine an attitude held with certainty seems to be the key (see also Oettingen et al., Reference Oettingen, Gollwitzer, Jung and Okten2022).

Uncertainty and Fanatical Behavior in Threatening Domains

The link between attempts to undermine confidence and the attraction to fanatical (extreme) groups led us to hypothesize that perhaps people simply walking around with uncertainty in their important attitudes would be similarly attracted to extreme actions especially if the attitude domain was perceived as threatening. It would be highly disconcerting to be unsure of one’s attitude in a domain where threat was perceived. More specifically, we (Siev, Petty, & Briñol, Reference Siev, Petty, Briñol, Kruglanski, Kopetz and Szumowska2022) hypothesized that although attitude certainty would be positively related to the willingness to engage in moderate or normal behavior (e.g., voting for one’s preferred candidate), it could be negatively related to willingness to engage in more extreme (fanatical) behavior (e.g., fighting and dying for one’s beliefs), especially if a threat was perceived. To examine this, we (Siev et al., Reference Siev, Petty, Paredes and Briñol2023) combined the data from 6 online studies conducted with Mechanical Turk and student samples we had conducted on the topic of Covid–19 in which we measured people’s attitudes, attitude certainty, and their willingness to engage in 20 attitude-consistent behaviors (up to N = 1,496 for the behavior with the most observations). We also assessed the perceived threat associated with Covid–19. The particular behaviors assessed ranged from wearing a mask to sacrificing one’s life for one’s beliefs. These behaviors were subsequently rated for their extremity by a separate group of participants (1= not at all extreme, 7 = very extreme; N = 69–72 depending on the behavior), and these normative ratings were used to predict the effects of attitude certainty on the behavioral willingness measures.

We conducted a multi-level model with attitude certainty and behavioral extremity (within-subjects) predicting behavioral willingness, where willingness ratings were nested within study, which was nested within type of behavior. This produced a significant certainty X extremity interaction, b = –.18, t(14.11) = –5.39, p < .0001, 95% CI [–.25, –.11], that is depicted in Figure 2. As expected, attitude certainty positively predicted willingness to engage in moderate behaviors (–1 SD in rated extremity), b = .32, t(14.70) = 4.23, p < .001, 95% CI [.16, .47] but this reversed for extreme behaviors (+1 SD extremity: b = –.23, t(15.42) = –3.40, p = .004), 95% CI [–.38, –.09]. Furthermore, analyses including the threat associated with the issues showed that it further moderated the results such that the negative relationship between certainty and intentions to engage in the extreme behaviors was especially prominent when perceived threat was high (Siev et al., Reference Siev, Petty, Paredes and Briñol2023).

Figure 2. The Effect of Attitude Certainty on Self-Reported Willingness to Engage in Attitude-Consistent Behaviors Was Strongly Positive for the Least Extreme Behaviors but Became Less Positive and Ultimately Significantly Negative as the Behaviors Considered Became More Extreme

Note. Data from Siev et al. (Reference Siev, Petty, Paredes and Briñol2023).

In sum, the accumulated evidence appears to suggest that when people’s attitudes on a variety of important issues become problematic (uncomfortable) in some way (e.g., because their high certainty is challenged, Gollwitzer et al., Reference Gollwitzer, Olcaysoy Okten, Pizarro and Oettingen2022, or they are uncertain of their views in a threatening domain, Siev et al., Reference Siev, Petty, Briñol, Kruglanski, Kopetz and Szumowska2022, Reference Siev, Petty, Paredes and Briñol2023), they can react by becoming more willing to engage in extreme and even violent behaviors in an attempt to compensate for that weakness by demonstrating strength.

Conclusions

In this brief review, we have focused on updating a few of the traditional perspectives on attitude strength that have been prominent in the literature. First, we noted that in contrast to the previous view that there were different types of attitude strength constructs with some (e.g., accessibility) being operative and others (e.g., certainty) being meta-cognitive, it might be preferrable to think of there being a variety of individual attitude strength dimensions, each of which can be measured with structural (objective) and meta-cognitive (subjective) assessments. Critically, this updated view holds that the different measures of the same attitude strength attribute can sometimes predict different outcomes, and can also predict unique variance in the same outcome, or predict the same outcome but under different circumstances because of different operating mechanisms. Second, we noted that although it is generally true that the various attitude strength indicators are predictive of an attitude being more difficult to change, there are some recently identified techniques that can actually be more effective in changing stronger than weaker attitudes. These techniques include matching the persuasive message to the basis of the attitude’s strength and using two- rather than one-sided messages.

Finally, we noted that although indicators of attitude strength are often positively associated with normal or mundane behaviors (e.g., voting, purchasing a consumer product), there are situations in which weaker attitudes are threatening to people (e.g., because the attitude domain is associated with threat and clarity is desired). In these situations, uncertainty can motivate extreme reactions. Lacking certainty can generally be unpleasant and disconcerting, but especially if the issue poses some perceived threat (Sawicki & Wegener, Reference Sawicki and Wegener2018). This can lead to efforts to compensate for that weakness, such as engaging in extreme action. In essence, in this situation, a strength variable (i.e., certainty) appears to be producing an effect opposite to its traditional strength role. However, it is important to note that this phenomenon is not the only one in which a strength variable has been associated with an effect opposite to its traditional one. In particular, several studies, noted briefly next, have shown that certainty has its traditional strength effect when attitudinal ambivalence is low, but it can have a reversed effect if the attitude is also ambivalent.

One way to think about this reversed effect for certainty when attitudes are ambivalent is that higher certainty (associated with strength) can magnify the impact of ambivalence (associated with weakness; Petty et al., Reference Petty, Briñol, Tormala, Wegener, Kruglanski and Higgins2007). This means that strength is having it traditional magnification role, but in this instance is magnifying the impact of ambivalence. Thus, with an ambivalent attitude, higher certainty produces more weakness rather than more strength, as people now feel certain of their attitude’s weakness. In the first study demonstrating this interesting outcome, Clarkson et al. (Reference Clarkson, Tormala and Rucker2008) found that manipulating certainty to be high increased resistance to persuasion when attitudes were low in ambivalence (the traditional finding), but when attitudes were ambivalent, enhancing certainty rendered attitudes more susceptible to persuasion (see Luttrell, Petty, & Briñol, Reference Luttrell, Petty and Briñol2016, Reference Luttrell, Petty and Briñol2020, for similar findings with respect to attitude persistence over time). A reversal of the link between higher attitude certainty producing greater attitude-behavior consistency was also demonstrated when ambivalence was high (Clarkson et al., Reference Clarkson, Tormala and Rucker2008; see Durso et al., Reference Durso, Briñol and Petty2016, for conceptually similar results, and Wallace et al., Reference Wallace, Patton, Luttrell, Sawicki, Fabrigar, Teeny, MacDonald, Petty and Wegener2020, for similar interactions between ambivalence and perceived knowledge).

In situations where certainty (or other strength variables) reverse their traditional effects, it is important to determine what moderates this different impact. In these reversal situations, rather than concluding that a variable that is considered a strength indicator has suddenly become an index of weakness, we suspect it will be better to continue to consider the variable (e.g., certainty) as a strength indicator and then view the reverse effect as occurring either because that strength from certainty is magnifying weakness (e.g., in the case of ambivalence; see also DeMarree et al., Reference DeMarree, Briñol and Petty2015; Wichman et al., Reference Wichman, Briñol, Petty, Rucker, Tormala and Weary2010) or because that strength itself is being challenged in some way. When strength is challenged or when strength is especially desired (e.g., when under threat), people can react to maintain, demonstrate, or enhance that strength. Thus, in some cases where people hold weak attitudes, this weakness is troublesome, leading people to aim to restore strength.

At the time of the 1995 Attitude Strength book, it was unclear how influential the construct of attitude strength would remain over time. It appears that much progress has been made in the intervening years in understanding how to measure strength, how to impact strong attitudes, and in understanding the mechanisms by which strength either determines behavior directly or in compensation for weakness. Perhaps most importantly, the concept of attitude strength has been applied to some of the most important societal questions of our time, such as understanding the determinants of fanaticism and support for violence.

Footnotes

Funding Statement: This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflicts of Interest: None

Data Sharing: N/A

1 We use all of these terms interchangeably.

2 One important reason Krosnick and Petty favored the independent constructs view was that some research had clearly shown that the different dimensions of strength could interact with each other in predicting outcomes (e.g., Tourangeau et al., Reference Tourangeau, Rasinski, Bradburn and D’Andrade1989).

3 Subjective (meta-cognitive) measures of strength are typically more likely to operate when people have some motivation and ability to consider their meta-cognitive reflections prior to responding (e.g., Moreno et al., Reference Moreno, Briñol and Petty2023; see Briñol & Petty, Reference Briñol and Petty2022, for a review).

4 Those who were induced to affirm their identities before the writing task did not show this effect because the affirmation presumably pre-empted the discomfort associated with the uncertainty.

5 In one study Gollwitzer et al. used a common ambivalence formula (Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Zanna, Griffin, Petty and Krosnick1995) to index discordant knowing (using measures of certainty in one’s own position and perceived social opposition to replace the positive and negative reactions in the original formula. Although this research looked at a particular kind of ambivalence (discrepancy between one’s attitude certainty and perceptions of others), it is reasonable to propose that a more typical ambivalence index composed of one’s personal positive and negative reactions (Priester & Petty, Reference Priester and Petty1996) could also contribute to fanaticism.

6 Similar effects were proposed for a related phenomenon called paradoxical knowing in which people recognize that their high certainty could be unfounded (e.g., it is based on something that is unknowable; see Gollwitzer & Oettingen, Reference Gollwitzer and Oettingen2019). As with discordant knowing, this situation would likely render people uncomfortable with their attitudes. We suggest that other forms of internal attitudinal conflict (e.g., between one’s current and desired attitudes, DeMarree et al., Reference DeMarree, Wheeler, Briñol and Petty2014) could also contribute to fanaticism.

References

Abelson, R. P. (1988). Conviction. American Psychologist, 43(4), 267275. http://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.43.4.267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allport, G. W. (1935). Attitudes. In Murchison, C. (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 798844). Clark University Press.Google Scholar
Barden, J., & Petty, R. E. (2008). The mere perception of elaboration creates attitude certainty: Exploring the thoughtfulness heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(3), 489509. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0012559CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bassili, J. N. (1996). Meta-judgmental versus operative indexes of psychological attributes: The case of attitude strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(4), 637653. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.4.637CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bizer, G. Y., & Petty, R. E. (2005). How we conceptualize our attitudes matters: The effects of valence framing on the resistance of political attitudes. Political Psychology, 26(4), 553568. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2005.00431.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2003). Overt head movements and persuasion: A self-validation analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(6), 11231139. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.6.1123CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2022). Self-validation theory: An integrative framework for understanding when thoughts become consequential. Psychological Review, 129(2), 340367. http://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000340CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catapano, R., & Tormala, Z. L. (2021). Do I support that it’s good or oppose that it’s bad? The effect of support-oppose framing on attitude sharing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 121(1), 2342. http://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000266CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cialdini, R. B., & Goldstein, N. J. (2004). Social influence: Compliance and conformity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 591621. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarkson, J., Smith, E., Tormala, Z., & Dugan, R. (2017). Group identification as a means of attitude restoration. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 68, 139145. http://doi.org/10.1016/J.Jesp.2016.07.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarkson, J. J., Tormala, Z. L., & Rucker, D. D. (2008). A new look at the consequences of attitude certainty: The amplification hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(4), 810825. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0013192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarkson, J. J., Tormala, Z. L., & Rucker, D. D. (2011). Cognitive and affective matching effects in persuasion: An amplification perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(11), 14151427. http://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211413394CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crites, S. L., Fabrigar, L. R., & Petty, R. E. (1994). Measuring the affective and cognitive properties of attitudes: Conceptual and methodological issues. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(6), 619634. http://doi.org/10.1177/0146167294206001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowley, A. E., & Hoyer, W. D. (1994). An integrative framework for understanding two-sided persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 20(4), 561574. http://doi.org/10.1086/209370CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, A. R., Yantis, S., Norwood, M., & Montano, D. E. (1985). Amount of information about the attitude object and attitude-behavior consistency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(5), 11841198. http://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.49.5.1184CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeBono, K. G., & Harnish, R. J. (1988). Source expertise, source attractiveness, and the processing of persuasive information: A functional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(4), 541546. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.55.4.541CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeMarree, K. G., Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2015). Reducing subjective ambivalence by creating doubt: A metacognitive approach. Social Psychology and Personality Science, 6, 731739. http://doi.org/10.1177/1948550615581497CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeMarree, K. G., Wheeler, S. C., Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2014). Wanting other attitudes: Actual-desired discrepancies predict feelings of ambivalence and ambivalence consequences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 53, 518. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2014.02.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durso, G. R. O., Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2016). From power to inaction: Ambivalence gives pause to the powerful. Psychological Science, 27(12), 16601666. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616669947CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, K. (1990). The interplay of affect and cognition in attitude formation and change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 202216. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.59.2.202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erber, M. W., Hodges, S. D., & Wilson, T. D. (1995). Attitude strength, attitude stability, and the effects of analyzing reasons. In Petty, R. E. & Krosnick, J. A. (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 433454). Psychology Press. http://doi.org/10.4324/9781315807041Google Scholar
Fabrigar, L. R., & Petty, R. E. (1999). The role of the affective and cognitive bases of attitudes in susceptibility to affectively and cognitively based persuasion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(3), 363381. http://doi.org/10.1177/0146167299025003008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford University Press. http://doi.org/10.1515/9781503620766CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gal, D., & Rucker, D. D. (2010). When in doubt, shout! Paradoxical influences of doubt on proselytizing. Psychological Science, 21(11), 17011707. http://doi.org/10.1177/095679761038595CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giner-Sorolla, R. (2004). Is affective material in attitudes more accessible than cognitive material? The moderating role of attitude basis. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34(6), 761780. http://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.229CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, A., & Oettingen, G. (2019). Paradoxical knowing: A shortcut to knowledge and its antisocial correlates. Social Psychology, 50(3), 145161. http://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000368CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, A., Olcaysoy Okten, I., Pizarro, A. O., & Oettingen, G. (2022). Discordant knowing: A social cognitive structure underlying fanaticism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 151(11), 28462878. http://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001219CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 14641480. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, J. (2014). Toward an integrative theory of psychological defense. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(1), 1939. http://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613506018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heine, S. J., Proulx, T., & Vohs, K. D. (2006). The meaning maintenance model: On the coherence of social motivations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(2), 88110. http://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1002_1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogg, M. A. (2014). From uncertainty to extremism: Social categorization and identity processes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(5), 338342. http://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414540168CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hovland, C. I., Janis, I. L., & Kelley, H. H. (1953). Communication and persuasion; psychological studies of opinion change. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Horcajo, J., Briñol, P., Paredes, B., Petty, R. E., DeMarree, K. G., & See, Y. H. M. (2022). Attitude polarization as a function of mortality salience: A meta-cognitive analysis. Psicothema, 34(2), 226232. http://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2021.334Google ScholarPubMed
Howe, L. C., & Krosnick, J. A. (2017). Attitude strength. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 327351. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033600CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hussein, M. A., & Tormala, Z. L. (2021). Undermining your case to enhance your impact: A framework for understanding the effects of acts of receptiveness in persuasion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 25(3), 229250. http://doi.org/10.1177/10888683211001269CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, D. M. (1945). A systematic treatment of judgment. Psychological Bulletin, 42(4), 193224. http://doi.org/10.1037/h0059291CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, K. J. (1972). On the ambivalence-indifference problem in attitude theory and measurement: A suggested modification of the semantic differential technique. Psychological Review, 77(5), 361372. http://doi.org/10.1037/h0032590Google Scholar
Krosnick, J. A., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Attitude strength: An overview. In Petty, R. E. & Krosnick, J. A. (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 124). Psychology Press. http://doi.org/10.4324/9781315807041Google Scholar
Lee, A. H. Y, Lelkes, Y., Hawkins, C. B., & Thedoridis, A. G. (2022). Negative partisanship is not more prevalent than positive partisanship. Nature Human Behaviour, 6, 951963. http://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-022-01348-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luttrell, A., & Petty, R. E. (2021). Evaluations of self-focused versus other-focused arguments for social distancing: An extension of moral matching effects. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(6), 946954. http://doi.org/10.1177/1948550620947853CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luttrell, A., Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2016). Ambivalence and certainty can interact to predict attitude stability over time. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 63, 5668. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2015.11.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luttrell, A., Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2020). The interactive effects of ambivalence and certainty on political opinion stability. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 8(2), 525541. http://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i2.1247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luttrell, A., Petty, R. E., Briñol, P., & Wagner, B. C. (2016). Making it moral: Merely labeling an attitude as moral increases its strength. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 65, 8293. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2016.04.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luttrell, A., Phillip–Muller, A., & Petty, R. E. (2019). Challenging moral attitudes with moral messages. Psychological Science, 30(8), 11361150. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619854706CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luttrell, A., & Sawicki, V. (2020). Attitude strength: Distinguishing predictors versus defining features. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 14(8), Article e12555. http://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12555CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, I. (2003). Defensive zeal: Compensatory conviction about attitudes, values, goals, groups, and self-definition in the face of personal uncertainty. In Spencer, S. J., Fein, S., & Zanna, M. P., & Olson, J. M. (Eds.), Motivated social perception: The Ontario Symposium (Vol. 9, pp. 7392). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
McGregor, I. (2006). Offensive defensiveness: Toward an integrative neuroscience of compensatory zeal after mortality salience, personal uncertainty, and other poignant self-threats. Psychological Inquiry, 17(4), 299308. http://doi.org/10.1080/10478400701366977CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, I., Zanna, M. P, Holmes, J. G., & Spencer, S. J. (2001). Compensatory conviction in the face of personal uncertainty: Going to extremes and being oneself. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(3), 472–88. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.3.472CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mello, J., Garcia-Marques, T., Briñol, P., Cancela, A., & Petty, R. E. (2020). The influence of physical attractiveness on attitude confidence and resistance to change. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 90, Article 104018. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2020.104018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Millar, M. G., & Millar, K. U. (1990). Attitude change as a function of attitude type and argument type. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(2), 217228. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.59.2.217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minson, J. A., Chen, F. S., & Tinsley, C. H. (2020). Why won’t you listen to me? Measuring receptiveness to opposing views. Management Science, 66(7), 30693094. http://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2019.3362CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moreno, L., Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2023). Extent of elaboration moderates reliance on meta-cognitive assessments: The case of attitude certainty [Manuscript submitted for publication]. Department of Social Psychology, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid.Google Scholar
Moreno, L., Requero, B., Santos, D., Paredes, B., Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2021). Attitudes and attitude certainty guiding pro-social behavior as a function of perceived elaboration. European Journal of Social Psychology, 51(6), 9901006. http://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2798CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oettingen, G., Gollwitzer, A., Jung, J., & Okten, I. O. (2022). Misplaced certainty in the context of conspiracy theories. Current Opinion in Psychology, (46) 101393. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101393CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ottati, V. C. & Stern, C. (Eds.) (in press). Divided: Open-mindedness and dogmatism in a polarized world. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Petrocelli, J. V., Tormala, Z. L., & Rucker, D. D. (2007). Unpacking attitude certainty: Attitude clarity and attitude correctness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 3041. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.30CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petty, R. E., Briñol, P., & Tormala, Z. L. (2002). Thought confidence as a determinant of persuasion: The self-validation hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 722741. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.5.722CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petty, R. E., Briñol, P., Tormala, Z. L., & Wegener, D. T. (2007). The role of meta-cognition in social judgment. In Kruglanski, A. W. & Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (2nd Ed., pp. 254284). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1981). Attitudes and persuasion: Classic and contemporary approaches. Routledge. http://doi.org/10.4324/9780429502156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1990). Involvement and persuasion: Tradition versus integration. Psychological Bulletin, 107(3), 367374. http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.107.3.367CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petty, R. E., & Krosnick, J. A. (Eds.) (1995). Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences. Psychology Press. http://doi.org/10.4324/9781315807041Google Scholar
Petty, R. E., Tormala, Z. L., Briñol, P., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (2006). Implicit ambivalence from attitude change: An exploration of the PAST model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(1), 2141. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.1.21CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillip-Muller, A. Z., Wallace, L. E., & Wegener, D. T. (2020). Where does moral conviction fit? A factor analytic approach examining antecedents to attitude strength. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 86, Article e103900. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103900CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pomerantz, E. M., Chaiken, S., & Tordesillas, R. S. (1995). Attitude strength and resistance processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(3), 408419. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.3.408CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Priester, J. M., & Petty, R. E. (1996). The gradual threshold model of ambivalence: Relating the positive and negative bases of attitudes to subjective ambivalence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(3), 431449. http://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.71.3.431CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Priester, J. R., & Petty, R. E. (2001). Extending the bases of subjective attitudinal ambivalence: Interpersonal and intrapersonal antecedents of evaluative tension. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1), 1934. http://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.80.1.19CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raden, D. (1985). Strength-related attitude dimensions. Social Psychology Quarterly, 48(4), 312330. http://doi.org/10.2307/2786693CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rocklage, M. D., & Luttrell, A. (2021). Attitudes based on feelings: Fixed or fleeting? Psychological Science, 32(3), 364380. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620965532CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, M. J., & Hovland, C. I. (1960). Cognitive, affective, and behavioral components of attitudes. In Rosenberg, M. J., Hovland, C. I., McGuire, W. J., Abelson, R. P., & Brehm, J. W. (Eds.), Attitude organization and change (pp. 114). Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Rucker, D. D., Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2008). What’s in a frame anyway? A meta-cognitive analysis of the impact of one versus two-sided message framing on attitude certainty. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18(2), 137149. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2008.01.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rucker, D. D., Tormala, Z. L., Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2014). Consumer conviction and commitment: An appraisal-based framework for attitude certainty. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24(1), 119136. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2013.07.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sample, J., & Warland, R. H. (1973). Attitude and prediction of behavior. Social Forces, 51(3), 292304. http://doi.org/10.2307/2577135CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sawicki, V., & Wegener, D. T. (2018). Metacognitive reflection as a moderator of attitude strength versus attitude bolstering: Implications for attitude similarity and attraction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44(5), 638652. http://doi.org/10.1177/0146167217744196CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuette, R. A., & Fazio, R. H. (1995). Attitude accessibility and motivation as determinants of biased processing: A test of the MODE model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(7), 704710. http://doi.org/10.1177/0146167295217005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuman, H., & Johnson, M. P. (1976). Attitudes and behavior. Annual Review of Sociology, 2, 161207. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.so.02.080176.001113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, W. A. (1968). Attitude measurement. In Lindzey, G. & Aronson, E. (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 204273). Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
See, Y. H. M., Petty, R. E., & Fabrigar, L. R. (2008). Affective and cognitive meta-bases of attitudes: Unique effects on information interest and persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(6), 938955. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.6.938CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
See, Y. H. M., Fabrigar, L. R., & Petty, R. E. (2013). Affective-cognitive meta-bases versus structural bases of attitudes predict processing interest versus efficiency. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39(8), 11111123. http://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213490807CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
See, Y. H. M., Fabrigar, L. R., & Petty, R. E. (2023). Dual construct attitude strength perspective: Theoretical implications of objective and subjective approaches to measuring the strength of attitudes [Manuscript submitted for publication]. Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore.Google Scholar
Siev, J. J., Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2022). Attitudinal extremism. In Kruglanski, A. W., Kopetz, C., & Szumowska, E. (Eds.). The psychology of extremism: A motivational perspective (pp. 3465). Routledge. http://doi.org/10.4324/9781003030898-4Google Scholar
Siev, J. J., Petty, R. E., Paredes, B., & Briñol, P. (2023). Behavioral extremity moderates the association between certainty in attitudes about Covid and willingness to engage in mitigation-related behaviors. [Manuscript submitted for publication]. Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University.Google Scholar
Skitka, L. J., Bauman, C. W, Sargis, E. G. (2005). Moral conviction: Another contributor to attitude strength or something more? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(6), 895917. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.6.895CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skitka, L. J., Hanson, B. E., Morgan, G. S, & Wisneski, D. C. (2021). The psychology of moral conviction. Annual Review of Psychology, 72, 347366. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-063020-030612CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snyder, M., & DeBono, K. G. (1985). Appeals to image and claims about quality: Understanding the psychology of advertising. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(3), 586597. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.49.3.586CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teeny, J. D., Siev, J. J., Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2021). A review and conceptual framework for understanding personalized matching effects in persuasion. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 31(2), 382414. http://doi.org/10.1002/jcpy.1198CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, M. M., Zanna, M. P., & Griffin, D. W. (1995). Let’s not be indifferent about (attitudinal) ambivalence. In Petty, R. E. & Krosnick, J. A. (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 361386). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Thurstone, L. L. (1928). Attitudes can be measured. American Journal of Sociology, 33, 529554. http://doi.org/10.1086/214483CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tormala, Z. L., Clarkson, J. J., & Henderson, M. D. (2011). Does fast or slow evaluation foster greater certainty? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 422434. http://doi.org/10.1177/0146167210397378CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tormala, Z. L., & Rucker, D. D. (2018). Attitude certainty: Antecedents, consequences, and new directions. Consumer Psychology Review, 1, 7289. http://doi.org/10.1002/arcp.1004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tormala, Z. L., Rucker, D. D., & Seger, C. R. (2008). When increased confidence yields increased thought: A confidence-matching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(1), 141147. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.11.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tourangeau, R., Rasinski, K. A., Bradburn, N., & D’Andrade, R. (1989). Belief accessibility and context effects in attitude measurement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25(5), 401421. http://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(89)90030-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Visser, P. S., Bizer, G. Y., & Krosnick, J. A. (2006). Exploring the latent structure of strength-related attitude attributes. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 167. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(06)38001-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallace, L. E., Patton, K. M., Luttrell, A., Sawicki, V., Fabrigar, L. R., Teeny, J., MacDonald, T. K., Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (2020). Perceived knowledge moderates the relation between subjective ambivalence and the impact of attitudes: An attitude strength perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 46(5), 709722. http://doi.org/10.1177/0146167219873492CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warland, R. H., & Sample, J. (1973a). Response certainty as a moderator variable in attitude measurement. Rural Sociology, 38(2), 174186.Google Scholar
Wegener, D. T., Downing, J., Krosnick, J. A., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Measures and manipulations of strength-related properties of attitudes: Current practice and future directions. In Petty, R. E. & Krosnick, J. A. (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 455487). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Weigel, R. H., & Newman, L. S. (1976). Increasing attitude-behavior correspondence by broadening the scope of the behavioral measure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33(6), 793802. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.33.6.793CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wichman, A. L., Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., Rucker, D. D., Tormala, Z. L., & Weary, G. (2010). Doubting one’s doubt: A formula for confidence? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 350355. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.10.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wicker, A. W. (1971). An examination of the “other variables” explanation of attitude-behavior inconsistency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 19(1), 1830. http://doi.org/10.1037/h0031090CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xu, M., & Petty, R. E. (2022). Two-sided messages promote openness for morally based attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 48(8), 11511166. http://doi.org/10.1177/0146167220988371CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, M., & Petty, R. E. (in press). Two-sided messages promote openness for a variety of deeply entrenched attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. http://doi.org/10.1177/01461672221128113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zunick, P. V., Teeny, J. D., & Fazio, R. H. (2017). Are some attitudes more self-defining than others? Assessing self-related attitude functions and their consequences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(8), 11361149. http://doi.org/10.1177/0146167217705121CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Interaction between the Extent to which Attitudes are Based on Morality and Message-Sidedness on Openness to a Counterattitudinal Message on Capital PunishmentNote. Adapted from Xu and Petty (2022).

Figure 1

Figure 2. The Effect of Attitude Certainty on Self-Reported Willingness to Engage in Attitude-Consistent Behaviors Was Strongly Positive for the Least Extreme Behaviors but Became Less Positive and Ultimately Significantly Negative as the Behaviors Considered Became More ExtremeNote. Data from Siev et al. (2023).