Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T07:41:55.943Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Are they Animals or Machines? Measuring Dehumanization

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2013

Rocío Martínez*
Affiliation:
University of Granada (Spain)
Rosa Rodríguez-Bailón
Affiliation:
University of Granada (Spain)
Miguel Moya
Affiliation:
University of Granada (Spain)
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Rocío Martínez. Departamento de Psicología Social, Facultad de Psicología. Campus de Cartuja s/n. 18011 - Granada (Spain). E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The present research deals with two forms of dehumanization: 1) denying uniquely human attributes to others (seeing them as animals); 2) denying human nature to others (seeing them as machines or automata). Studies 1 and 2 explored these two forms of dehumanization, analyzing whether people associated their ingroup more with human-related words (vs. animal- vs. machine-related words) than two different outgroups. A paper and pencil procedure was used to find out which words were associated with the surnames of the ingroup (Spaniards) or the outgroup (Germans, Gypsies). Results showed that participants were more ready to link ingroup than outgroup surnames to human words. They also linked more Gypsy surnames to animal-related words and German surnames with machine-related words. Studies 3 and 4 used the Implicit Association Test to analyze the same ideas and replicated the results of Studies 1 and 2.

La investigación que se presenta trata sobre dos formas de deshumanización: 1) Negar a los demás cualidades exclusivamente humanas (viéndolos como animales); 2) negarles la naturaleza humana (viéndolos como máquinas o autómatas). En los estudios 1 y 2 se examinaron ambas formas de deshumanización analizando si la gente asociaba más al endogrupo con palabras relativas a humanos (vs. animales o máquinas) en comparación con dos exogrupos diferentes. Se utilizó un procedimiento de papel y lápiz para saber qué palabras se asociaban más con los apellidos del endogrupo (españoles) o con los apellidos del exogrupo (alemanes, gitanos). Los resultados pusieron de manifiesto que los participantes relacionaron en mayor medida los apellidos del endogrupo con las palabras relativas a humanos. Además también vincularon los apellidos gitanos con palabras vinculadas a animales y los apellidos alemanes con palabras de máquinas. En los estudios 3 y 4 se utilizó el Test de Asociación Implícita (IAT) para probar las mismas ideas y los resultados corroboraron los encontrados en los estudios 1 y 2.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allport, G. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison Wesley.Google Scholar
Bain, P., Park, J., Kwok, C., & Haslam, N. (2009). Attributing human uniqueness and human nature to cultural groups: Distinct forms of subtle dehumanization. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 12, 789805. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1368430209340415CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boccato, G., Cortes, B., Demoulin, S., & Leyens, J. Ph. (2007). The automaticity of infrahumanization. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 987999. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.412CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chulvi, B., & Pérez, J. A. (2005). El prejuicio genealógico [The genealogical prejudice]. Psicología Política, 30, 5770.Google Scholar
Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., Kwan, V. S. Y., Glick, P., Demoulin, S., Leyens, J. Ph., … Ziegler, R. (2009). Stereotype content model across cultures: Towards universal similarities and some differences. British Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 133. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/014466608X314935CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demoulin, S., Leyens, J. Ph., Paladino, M. P., Rodriguez, R. T., Rodriguez, A. P., & Dovidio, J. F. (2004). Dimensions of “uniquely” and “non-uniquely” emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 7196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930244000444CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demoulin, S., Leyens, J. Ph., Rodriguez, R. T., Rodriguez, A. P., Paladino, M. P., & Fiske, S. T. (2005). Motivation to support a desired conclusion versus motivation to avoid an undesirable conclusion: The case of infra-humanization. International Journal of Psychology, 40, 416428. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207590500184495CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demoulin, S., Saroglou, V., & Van Pachterbeke, M. (2008). Infrahumanizing others, supra-humanizing gods: The emotional hierarchy. Social Cognition, 26, 235247. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.2008.26.2.235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 169200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699939208411068CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gómez-Berrocal, C., & Ruiz, J. (2001). Los valores: Una construcción cultural asociada al prejuicio hacia los gitanos, inmigrantes y minusválidos. Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada, 54, 313329.Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 427. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.102.1.4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. K. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 14641480. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.74.6.1464CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test I. An improved scoring algorithm Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197216. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haslam, N. (2006). Dehumanization: An integrative review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 252264. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haslam, N., Bain, P., Douge, L., Lee, M., & Bastian, B. (2005). More human than you: Attributing humanness to self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 937950. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.937CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haslam, N., Kashima, Y., Loughnan, S., Shi, J., & Suitner, C. (2008). Subhuman, inhuman, and superhuman: Contrasting humans and nonhumans in three cultures. Social Cognition, 26, 248258. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.2008.26.2.248CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Innes-Ker, A., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2002). Emotion concepts and emotional states in social judgment and categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 804816. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.83.4.804CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loughnan, S., & Haslam, N. (2007). Animals and androids: Implicit associations between social categories and nonhumans. Psychological Science, 18, 116121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01858.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leyens, J., Demoulin, S., Vaes, J., Gaunt, R., & Paladino, M. P. (2007). Infra-humanization: The wall of group differences. Social Issues and Policy Review, 1, 139172. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-2409.2007.00006.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leyens, J. Ph., Paladino, P. M., Rodríguez, R. T., Vaes, J., Demoulin, S., Rodríguez, A. P., & Gaunt, R. (2000). The emotional side of prejudice: The role of secondary emotions. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 186197. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0402_06CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leyens, J-Ph., Rodriguez, A. P., Rodriguez, R. T., Gaunt, R., Paladino, M. P., & Vaes, J. (2001). Psychological essentialism and the differential attribution of uniquely human emotions to ingroups and outgroups. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 395411. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morera, D., Marichal, F., Quiles, M., Betancor, V., Rodríguez, R., Rodríguez, A., … Vargas, I. (2004). La percepción de la semejanza intragrupal y la identificación con el ingroup: ¿Incrementa o disminuye el prejuicio? [Perception of intragroup similarity and identification with the ingroup: Does it increase or decrease prejudice?] Psicothema, 16, 7075.Google Scholar
Paladino, P. M., Leyens, J. Ph., Rodriguez-Torres, R., Rodriguez-Perez, A., Gaunt, R., & Demoulin, S. (2002). Differential association of uniquely and non uniquely human emotions to the ingroup and the outgroups. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 5, 105117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1368430202005002539CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pettigrew, T. F., & Meertens, R. W. (1995). Subtle and blatant prejudice in Western Europe. European Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 5775. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420250106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodríguez-Bailón, R., Barranco, J., & Casado, P. (2000). Tipos motivacionales de valores dominantes y prejuicio hacia grupos minoritarios [Types of dominant motivational values and prejudice towards minority groups]. In Caballero, F., Méndez, Mª., & Pastor, J. (Eds.), La mirada psicosociológica (pp. 273279). Madrid, Spain: Biblioteca Nueva.Google Scholar
Rodríguez-Bailón, R., & Puertas, S. (2000, April). ¿Seguimossiendo prejuiciosos? Prejuicio clásico y prejuicio simbólico hacia gitanos, magrebíes y latinoamericanos [Are we still prejudiced? Classic and symbolic prejudice toward Roma people, North Africans, and South Americans]. Paper presented at Congreso Nacional de Estudiantes, UNI'2000. Granada, Spain.Google Scholar
Rodríguez-Bailón, R., Ruiz, J., Moya, M. (2009). The impact of music on automatically activated attitudes: Flamenco and gypsy people. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 12, 381396. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1368430209102849CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rohmann, A., Niedenthal, P. M., Brauer, M., Castano, E., & Leyens, J-P. (2009). The attribution of primary and secondary emotions to the in-group and to the out- group. The case of equal status countries. Journal of Social Psychology, 149, 709730. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224540903348253CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rueda, J. F., & Navas, M. (1996). Hacia una evaluación de las nuevas formas del prejuicio racial: Las actitudes sutiles del racismo [Evaluation of new forms of racial prejudice: Subtle attitudes of racism]. Revista de Psicología Social, 11, 131149. http://dx.doi.org/10.1174/02134749660569314CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaes, J., Paladino, M. P., Castelli, L., Leyens, J. Ph., & Giovanazzi, A. (2003). On the behavioural consequences of infrahumanization: The implicit role of uniquely human emotions in intergroup relations. JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology, 85, 10161034. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1016Google ScholarPubMed
Viki, G. T., Winchester, L., Titshall, L., Chisango, T., Pina, A., & Russell, R. (2006). Beyond secondary emotions: The infrahumanization of outgroups using human-related and animal-related words. Social Cognition, 24, 753775. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.2006.24.6.753CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Viki, G. T., & Calitri, R. (2008). Infrahuman outgroup or suprahuman ingroup: The role of nationalism and patriotism in the infrahumanization of outgroups. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 10541061. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.495CrossRefGoogle Scholar