Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T09:09:30.294Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

“Why are the Corrupt, Corrupt?”: The Multilevel Analytical Model of Corruption

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 May 2020

João Gabriel Modesto*
Affiliation:
Centro Universitário de Brasília (Brazil) Universidade Estadual de Goiás (Brazil)
Ronaldo Pilati
Affiliation:
Universidade de Brasília (Brazil)
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to João Gabriel Modesto. Centro Universitário de Brasília, Faculdade de Psicologia. 70790–075. Brasília, DF, Brazil. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Corruption is a global problem. Despite the importance of this theme, a shortage of theoretical models in both psychology and related areas that favor its understanding and investigation is noted. Due to this scarcity of theoretical models, in addition to the need to systematize studies on the topic, this theoretical article aims to describe the Analytical Model of Corruption (AMC) as an interdisciplinary and multilevel proposal aimed at corruption analysis. To achieve this goal, the concept of corruption was analyzed using related phenomena as reference. Similarities and differences in corruption have been identified with dishonest behavior and unethical behavior. Subsequently, theoretical models on corruption identified in the literature were presented, and their main characteristics and limitations were pointed out. After describing the models, the AMC was presented and its advantages over the previous models were discussed. Finally, it was concluded that the AMC could be configured as a theoretical model that guides interdisciplinary studies on corruption, allowing for a more complete analysis compared to previous theoretical models identified in the literature.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

The study was supported by the Grant 308268/2016-4 of the National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq) for the second author.

References

Alhassan-Alolo, N. (2007). Gender and corruption: Testing the new consensus. Public Administration and Development, 27(3), 227237. https://doi.org/10.1002/pad.455CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersson, S., & Heywood, P. M. (2009). The politics of perception: Use and abuse of Transparency International’s approach to measuring corruption. Political Studies, 57(4), 746767. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9248.2008.00758.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ashforth, B. E., Gioia, D. A., Robinson, S. L., & Treviño, L. K. (2008). Re-viewing organizational corruption. Academy of Management Review, 33(3), 670684. https://doi.org/10.5465/AMR.2008.32465714CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ayal, S., Gino, F., Barkan, R., & Ariely, D. (2015). Three principles to REVISE people’s unethical behavior. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(6), 738741. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615598512CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bai, B., Liu, X., & Kou, Y. (2016). Belief in a just world lowers bribery intention. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 19(1), 6675. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajsp.12108CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, T. D., Beaulieu, E., & Saxton, G. W. (2018). Restoring trust in the police: Why female officers reduce suspicions of corruption. Governance, 31(1), 143161. https://doi.org/10.1111/gove.12281CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, G. S. (1968). Crime and punishment: An economic approach. Journal of Political Economy, 76(2), 169217. https://doi.org/10.1086/259394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blickle, G., Schlegel, A., Fassbender, P., & Klein, U. (2006). Some personality correlates of business white-collar crime. Applied Psychology, 55(2), 220233. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2006.00226.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breen, M., Gillanders, R., Mcnulty, G., & Suzuki, A. (2017). Gender and corruption in business. The Journal of Development Studies, 53(9), 14861501. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220388.2016.1234036CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cislak, A., Cichocka, A., Wojcik, A. D., & Frankowska, N. (2018). Power corrupts, but control does not: What stands behind the effects of holding high positions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44(6), 944957. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167218757456CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collier, M. W. (2002). Explaining corruption: An institutional choice approach. Crime, Law and Social Change, 38(1), 132. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1019802614530CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, J. M., & Schmidt, F. L. (2006). Personality, integrity, and white collar crime: A construct validity study. Personnel Psychology, 46(2), 295311. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.1993.tb00875.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darley, J. M. (2005). The cognitive and social psychology of contagious organizational corruption. Brooklyn Law Review, 70(4), 11771194.Google Scholar
Davis, J. H., & Ruhe, J. A. (2003). Perceptions of country corruption: Antecedents and outcomes. Journal of Business Ethics, 43(4), 275288. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023038901080CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeCelles, K. A., DeRue, D. S., Margolis, J. D., & Ceranic, T. L. (2012). Does power corrupt or enable? When and why power facilitates self-interested behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97(3), 681689. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026811CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deephouse, D. L., Newburry, W., & Soleimani, A. (2016). The effects of institutional development and national culture on cross-national differences in corporate reputation. Journal of World Business, 51(3), 463473. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jwb.2015.12.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dimant, E., & Schulte, T. (2016). The nature of corruption: An interdisciplinary perspective. German Law Journal, 17(1), 5372. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2071832200019684CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doise, W. (1980). Levels of explanation in the European Journal of Social Psychology. European Journal of Social Psychology, 10(3), 213231. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420100302CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. S. B. T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223241. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612460685CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrett, N., Lazzaro, S. C., Ariely, D., & Sharot, T. (2016). The brain adapts to dishonesty. Nature Neuroscience, 19, 17271732. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4426CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gino, F., & Ariely, D. (2012). The dark side of creativity: Original thinkers can be more dishonest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 445459. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026406CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gino, F., Ayal, S., & Ariely, D. (2009). Contagion and differentiation in unethical behavior: The effect of one bad apple on the barrel. Psychological Science, 20(3), 393398. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02306.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gino, F., & Pierce, L. (2009). Dishonesty in the name of equity. Psychological Science, 20(9), 11531160. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02421.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gino, F., Ayal, S., & Ariely, D. (2013). Self-serving altruism? The lure of unethical actions that benefit others. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 93, 285292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2013.04.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallsson, B. G., Siebner, H. R., & Hulme, O. J. (2018). Fairness, fast and slow: A review of dual process models of fairness. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 89, 4960. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.02.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations. Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Jetter, M., Montoya Agudelo, A., & Ramírez Hassan, A. (2015). The effect of democracy on corruption: Income is key. World Development, 74, 286304. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.worlddev.2015.05.016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jha, C., & Panda, B. (2017). Individualism and corruption: A cross-country analysis. Economic Papers: A Journal of Applied Economics and Policy, 36(1), 6074. https://doi.org/10.1111/1759-3441.12163CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, J. F., & Connelly, S. (2016). Moral disengagement and ethical decision-making. Journal of Personnel Psychology, 15(4), 184189. https://doi.org/10.1027/1866-5888/a000166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Judge, W. Q., McNatt, D. B., & Xu, W. (2011). The antecedents and effects of national corruption: A meta-analysis. Journal of World Business, 46(1), 93103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jwb.2010.05.021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263292. https://doi.org/10.2307/1914185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kish-Gephart, J. J., Harrison, D. A., & Treviño, L. K. (2010). Bad apples, bad cases, and bad barrels: Meta-analytic evidence about sources of unethical decisions at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(1), 131. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017103CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knoll, M., Lord, R. G., Petersen, L.-E., & Weigelt, O. (2016). Examining the moral grey zone: The role of moral disengagement, authenticity, and situational strength in predicting unethical managerial behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 46(1), 6578. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12353CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehnert, K., Park, Y., & Singh, N. (2015). Research note and review of the empirical ethical decision-making literature: Boundary conditions and extensions. Journal of Business Ethics, 129(1), 195219. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-014-2147-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazar, N., Amir, O., & Ariely, D. (2008). The dishonesty of honest people: A theory of self-concept maintenance. Journal of Marketing Research, 45(6), 633644. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.45.6.633CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazar, N., & Ariely, D. (2006). Dishonesty in everyday life and its policy implications. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 25(1), 117126. https://doi.org/10.1509/jppm.25.1.117CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mead, N. L., Baumeister, R. F., Gino, F., Schweitzer, M. E., & Ariely, D. (2009). Too tired to tell the truth: Self-control resource depletion and dishonesty. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(3), 594597. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.02.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, H. (2003). Determinants of corruption: A cross‐national analysis. Multinational Business Review, 11(2), 2948. https://doi.org/10.1108/1525383X200300010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pellegrini, L. (2011). Corruption, development and the environment. Springer Netherlands.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pereira, M. E., & Araújo, S. de F. (2013). Social cognition. In Keith, K. D. (Ed.), The Encyclopedia of Cross-Cultural Psychology (1st Ed., pp. 11911197). Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, J. G., & Welch, S. (1978). Political corruption in America: A search for definitions and a theory, or if political corruption is in the mainstream of American politics why is it not in the mainstream of American politics research? American Political Science Review, 72(3), 974984. https://doi.org/10.2307/1955115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pettigrew, T. F. (2018). The emergence of Contextual Social Psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44(7), 963971. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167218756033CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rest, J. R. (1986). Moral development: Advances in research and theory. Praeger.Google Scholar
Reyna, V. F. (2004). How people make decisions that involve risk: A dual-processes approach. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(2), 6066. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00275.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rilling, J. K., Gutman, D. A., Zeh, T. R., Pagnoni, G., Berns, G. S., & Kilts, C. D. (2002). A neural basis for social cooperation. Neuron, 35(2), 395405. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00755-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saha, S., Gounder, R., & Su, J.-J. (2009). The interaction effect of economic freedom and democracy on corruption: A panel cross-country analysis. Economics Letters, 105(2), 173176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.econlet.2009.07.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schikora, J. T. (2011, March 1). Bringing the four-eyes-principle to the lab (Munich Discussion Paper 2011–3). Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München. http://epub.ub.uni-muenchen.de/12160/Google Scholar
Schindler, S., & Pfattheicher, S. (2017). The frame of the game: Loss-framing increases dishonest behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 69, 172177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2016.09.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, E. D., & Jehn, K. A. (1999). Ranking rank behaviors: A comprehensive situation-based definition of dishonesty. Business & Society, 38(3), 296325. https://doi.org/10.1177/000765039903800304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shenhav, A., & Greene, J. D. (2014). Integrative moral judgment: Dissociating the roles of the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(13), 47414749. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3390-13.2014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soeharto, I., & Nugroho, N. (2018). Are we culturally corrupt? Revisiting the relationship between cultural dimensions and Corruption Perception Index. Asia Pacific Fraud Journal, 2(2), 143149. http://doi.org/10.21532/apfj.001.17.02.02.02CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Søreide, T. (2009). Too risk averse to stay honest? International Review of Law and Economics, 29(4), 388395. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.irle.2009.03.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sung, H.-E. (2003). Fairer sex or fairer system? Gender and corruption revisited. Social Forces, 82(2), 703723. https://doi.org/10.1353/sof.2004.0028CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swamy, A., Knack, S., Lee, Y., & Azfar, O. (2001). Gender and corruption. Journal of Development Economics, 64(1), 2555. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3878(00)00123-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories: Studies in social psychology (1st Ed.). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tanzi, V. (1998). Corruption around the world: Causes, consequences, scope, and cures. IMF Economic Review, 45(4), 559594. https://doi.org/10.2307/3867585CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Transparency International (2018). How do you define corruption? https://www.transparency.org/what-is-corruptionGoogle Scholar
Treviño, L. K., den Nieuwenboer, N. A., & Kish-Gephart, J. J. (2014). (Un)Ethical behavior in organizations. Annual Review of Psychology, 65(1), 635660. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143745CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treviño, L. K., Weaver, G. R., & Reynolds, S. J. (2006). Behavioral ethics in organizations: A review. Journal of Management, 32(6), 951990. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206306294258CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Bank (2001). Engendering development: Through gender equality in rights, resources and voice. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Yeganeh, H. (2014). Culture and corruption. International Journal of Development Issues, 13(1), 224. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJDI-04-2013-0038CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zaloznaya, M. (2014). The social psychology of corruption: Why it does not exist and why it should. Sociology Compass, 8(2), 187202. https://doi.org/10.1111/soc4.12120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhao, H., Zhang, H., & Xu, Y. (2019). Effects of perceived descriptive norms on corrupt intention: The mediating role of moral disengagement. International Journal of Psychology, 54(1), 93101. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12401CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed