Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T07:06:51.311Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Temporal Relation between Regression and Transition Periods in Early Infancy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2013

Marta Sadurní*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Girona (Spain)
Marc Pérez Burriel
Affiliation:
Universidad de Girona (Spain)
Frans X. Plooij
Affiliation:
International Research Institute on Infant Studies (Netherlands)
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Marta Sadurní. Departmento de Psicología. Facultad de Educación y Psicología. Universidad de Girona. Plaça San Domènec, n°3. 17007 Girona. (Spain). E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

According to the literature about developmental changes, periods of instability and disorganization in the social and emotional behavior in both human and non-human primate, infancy precedes major developmental achievements or transitions (Heimann, 2003; Sparrow & Brazelton, 2006). Developmental investigators have observed a more frequent and prolonged crying, clinging and bids for physical contact with mother during these periods of instability and disorganization. Some authors, according to Horwich (1974), called these periods regression periods. Rijt-Plooij and Plooij (1992) claimed that 10 regression periods could be identified during the first 20 months of human life. In an early study, Sadurní and Rostan (2002) confirmed the presence of 8 such regression periods during the first year of life of 18 Catalan babies. Their 8 regression periods were comparable to the first 8 of the 10 regression periods found by Van de Rijt Plooij and Plooij. The aim of the present study is to see whether the regression periods that we found are temporally related to some transition. We define a transition as the occurrence of a new developmental change in a child. In the present study we have used non-analyzed data from the same 18 Catalan babies (10 boys and 8 girls) as mentioned in our earlier published study on regression periods. The age of these babies was between 3 weeks and 14 months. Using a microgenetic methodology we have found 8 transitions periods in the first year of life. We have also observed a temporal relation between the regressions periods found earlier and the transition periods reported here.

La literatura científica acerca de los cambios en el desarrollo, sostiene que existen períodos de desorganización e inestabilidad en el comportamiento emocional y social de las crías de primate tanto humanas como no humanas que preceden a los cambios evolutivos o transiciones (Heimann, 2003; Sparrow & Brazelton, 2006). Las investigaciones revelan un llanto más prolongado y un aumento de la necesidad de aferramiento y contacto físico con la madre durante estos períodos de inestabilidad. Algunos autores, siguiendo a Horwich (1974) han denominado a esos períodos, periodos de regresión. Van de Rijt-Plooij & Plooij (1992) afirman que 10 períodos de regresión pueden ser identificados durante los primeros 20 meses de vida humana. En un estudio anterior, Sadurní and Rostan (2002) confirmaron la presencia de 8 de estos períodos durante el primer año de vida en 18 bebés pertenecientes a la Comunidad Autónoma de Cataluña, que coincidieron con los 8 primeros encontrados por Van de Rijt Plooij and Plooij. El objetivo del presente estudio es comprobar si estos períodos de regresión hallados están temporalmente relacionados con alguna transición. Definimos una transición como la emergencia de un nuevo cambio en el desarrollo de un niño/a. En el presente estudio hemos utilizado datos no analizados de los mismos 18 bebés (10 niños y 8 niñas) que formaron parte del estudio anterior. Los bebés tenían entre 3 semanas y 14 meses. Utilizando un análisis microgenético hemos hallado 8 períodos de transición en el primer año de vida. Asimismo hemos observado una relación temporal entre los períodos de regresión hallados previamente y los períodos de transición presentados en este estudio.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bever, T. G. (Ed.). (1982). Regression in mental development: Basic phenomena and theoretical alternatives. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. volt 1: Attachment. London: Hogart.Google Scholar
Brazelton, T.B. (1992). Touchpoints: your child's emotional and behavioral development. Reading, Massachussets: Merloyd Lawrence Book.Google Scholar
Brazelton, T. B., & Sparrow, J. D. (2006). Touchpoints-birth to three (2nd ed.). New York: Da Capo Press.Google Scholar
Camioni, L; Aureli, T; Bellagamba, F& Fogel, A (2003) A Longitudinal Examination of the Transition to Symbolic Communication in the Second Year of Life. Infant and Child Development, 12, 126.Google Scholar
Fischer, K. W. (Ed.). (1983). Levels and transitions in children's development (new directions for child and adolescent development. London: Jossey-Bass Inc.Google Scholar
Fischer, K. W., & Pipp, S. L. (1984). Detecting developmental discontinuities. methods and measurement. In Emde, R. N., & Harmon, R. J. (Eds.), Continuities and discontinuities in development (pp. 95121). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Heimann, M. (Ed.). (2003). Regression periods in human infancy. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hernández-Blasi, C., Bering, J. M., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2003). Psicología evolucionista del desarrollo: Contemplando la ontogénesis humana desde los ojos del evolucionismo. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 26(3), 276285.Google Scholar
Horwich, R. H. (1974). Regressive periods in primate behavioral development with reference to other mammals. Primates, 15, 141149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J. (1984). Continuity and change in the opening years of life. In Emde, R. N., & Harmon, R. J. (Eds.), Continuities and discontinuities in development. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1994). Précis of beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Science, 17, 693745.Google Scholar
Lavelli, M., Pantoja, A. P. F., Hsu, H., Messinger, D., & Fogel, A. (2005). Using microgenietic designs to study change processes. In Teti, D. M. (Ed.), Handbook of research methods in developmental science (pp. 4065). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Lewis, M., & Ash, A. J. (1992). Evidence for a neo-piagetian stage transition in early cognitive development. International Journal of Cognitive Development, 15, 337358.Google Scholar
Lindahl, B. L., Heimann, M., & Ullstadius, E. (2003). Occurrence of regressive periods in the normal development of Swedish infants. In Heimann, M. (Ed.), Regression periods in early infancy (pp. 4156). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Maratos, O. (1982). Trends in development of imitation in early infancy. In Bever, T. G. (Ed.), Regression in mental development: Basic phenomena and theoretical alternatives (pp. 81101). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
McCall, R. B., Eichorn, D. H., & Hogarty, P. S. (1977). Transitions in early development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 42(3), Serial No 171Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., & Gopnik, A. (1989). Some relationships between imitation, cognitive development and early language development in the first two years of life. In Speidel, G., & Nelson, K. (Eds.), The many faces of imitation in language learning. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Monoud, P. (1982). Revolutionary periods in early development. In Bever, T. G. (Ed.), Regression in mental development: Basic phenomena and theoretical alternatives (pp. 119131). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Oyama, S. (1993) The ontogeny of information: Developmental systems and evolution. In Johnson, M. H. (Ed.), Brain development and cognition: A reader (pp. 1930). Oxford and Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Perinat, A.& Sadurní, M. (1999). The ontogenesis of meaning: an interactional approach. Mind, Culture and Activity: An interactional Journal, 6, 5376.Google Scholar
Perner, J., Leekam, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds' difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 125137.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1950). Introduction à l'Épistémologie génétique. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Plooij, F. X. (2003). The trilogy of mind. In Heimann, M. (Ed.), Regression periods in human infancy (pp. 185205). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Plooij, F. X., & Rijt-Plooij, H. H. C. v. d. (1989a). Evolution of human parenting. canalization, new types of learning, and mother-infant conflict. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 4, 177192.Google Scholar
Plooij, F. X., & Rijt-Plooij, H. H. C. v. d. (1989b). Vulnerable periods during infancy: Hierarchically reorganized systems control, stress and disease. Ethnology and Sociobiology, 10, 279296.Google Scholar
Plooij, F. X., Rijt-Plooij, H. H. C. v. d., & Helmers, R. (2003). Multimodal distribution of SIDS and regression periods. In Heimann, M. (Ed.), Regression periods in human infancy (pp. 97106). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Plooij, F. X., Rijt-Plooij, H. H. C. v. d., Stelt, J. M. v. d., Es, B. v., & Helmers, R. (2003). Illness peaks during infancy and regression periods. In Heimann, M. (Ed.), Regression periods in human infancy (pp. 8195). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Rijt-Plooij, H. H. C. v. d., & Plooij, F. X. (1987). Growing independence, conflict and learning in mother-infant relations in free-ranging chimpanzees. Behavior, 101, 186.Google Scholar
Rijt-Plooij, H. H. C. v. d., & Plooij, F. X. (1992). Infantile regressions: Disorganization and onset of transition periods. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 10, 129149.Google Scholar
Rijt-Plooij, H. H. C. v. d., & Plooij, F. X. (1993). Distinct periods of mother-infant conflict in normal development: Sources of progress and germs of pathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 229246.Google Scholar
Rosenblith, J. (1992). In the beginning. Development from conception to age two (2nd ed.). Newbury Park: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Rostan, R. (1998). Análisis de los periodos de regresión y transición durante el primer año de vida. (Doctor, University of Girona. Spain.). Retrieved from http://www.tesisenxarxa.net/TDX-1127101-191348/Google Scholar
Ruff, H. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (1996). Attention in early development: Themes and variations. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sadurní, M., & Rostan, R. (2002). Regression periods in infancy: A case study from catalonia. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 1, 3643.Google Scholar
Sadurní, M., & Rostan, R. (2003a). La importancia de las emociones en los períodos sensibles del desarrollo. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 27(1), 105114.Google Scholar
Sadurní, M., & Rostan, R. (2003b). Reflections on regression periods in the development of catalan infants. In Heimann, M. (Ed.), Regression periods in early infancy (pp. 722). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Sadurní, M., Rostan, R., & Perez Burriel, M. (2006). The link between regression periods and transition periods. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Siegel, S. (1956). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Sonnander, K. (1987). Parental developmental assessment of 18-month-old children: Reliability and predictive value. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 29, 351362.Google Scholar
Sparrow, J. D., & Brazelton, T. B. (2006). A developmental approach for the prevention of common behavioural problems. Retrieved April 2nd, 2008, from http://www.touchpoints.org/nwsltrs_flyers_forms/A_Developmental_Approach_to_Behavioral_Problems_2006.pdfGoogle Scholar
Spitz, R. A. (1958). La première année de la vie de l'enfant. Paris: P.U.F.Google Scholar
Strauss, S. (Ed.). (1982). U-shaped behavioral growth. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Strauss, S., & Stavey, R. (1982). U-shaped behavioral growth: Implications for theories of development. In Hartup, W. W. (Ed.), Review of child development research (pp. 547599). Chicago: University odd Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1982). The primary motives for cooperative understanding. In Butterworth, G., & Light, P. (Eds.), Social cognition: Studies of development of understanding (pp. 77109). Brighton, UK: Hasvester Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. J. (2003). Regulation of brain development and age-related changes in infants' motives: The developmental function of “regressive” periods. In Heimann, M. (Ed.), Regression periods in human infancy (pp. 107184). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
van der Veer, R. (1986). Vygotsky's developmental psychology. Psychological Reports, 59, 527536.Google Scholar
Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental development. Chicago: Follett.Google Scholar
Woolmore, A., & Richer, J. (2003). Detecting infant regression periods: Weak signals in a noisy environment. In Heimann, M. (Ed.), Regression periods in early infancy (pp. 2340). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Zelazzo, P. R. (1982). The year-old infant. A period of major cognitive development. In Bever, T. G. (Ed.), Regressions in mental development: Basic phenomena and theories. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar