Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T23:32:22.308Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Anticipated Emotions and Personal Experience for Predicting Behavioral Intentions and Behavioral Expectations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2013

Pilar Carrera*
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (Spain)
Amparo Caballero
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (Spain)
Dolores Muñoz
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (Spain)
Luis Oceja
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (Spain)
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Pilar Carrera. Universidad Autónoma de Madrid. Departamento de Psicología Social, Facultad de Psicología, C/Ivan Pavlov, 6, Cantoblanco, 28049 Madrid. (Spain). E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

We tested how anticipated emotions interact with personal experience in risk behavior to improve predictions from TPB on behavioral intention (BI) and behavioral expectation (BE) for sex without condom (Study 1) and excessive drinking (Study 2). In the moderate-high experience group, anticipated emotional profiles (AEPs) improve TPB prediction from 28% to 45% in the case of BI and from 19% to 40% in that of BE in relation to sexual risk behavior (Study 1), and from 23% to 36% in the case of BI and from 17% to 31% in that of BE in relation to binge drinking (Study 2). However, in the low-experience group (Study 2) AEPs improve TPB predictions for BI (12% to 34%) but not for BE, showing that in less experienced people BI and BE are not equivalent: anticipated emotions have different relevance in their prediction. These results were replicated using a general negative anticipated emotion index (averaging emotional categories).

Hemos puesto a prueba como interactúan las emociones anticipadas y la experiencia personal en la conducta de riesgo para mejorar las predicciones hechas desde la Teoría de la Conducta Planeada (TPB) sobre las intenciones (BI) y expectativas conductuales (BE), tanto para el sexo sin preservativo (Estudio 1), como para la ingesta puntual de alcohol en exceso (Estudio 2). En el grupo con experiencia personal moderada-alta, los perfiles de emociones anticipadas (AEPs) mejoraron las predicciones hechas desde la TPB del 28% al 45% en el caso de intenciones conductuales (BI) y del 19% al 40% sobre expectativas conductuales (BE) en relación con la conducta sexual de riesgo (Estudio 1); y del 23% al 36% en el caso de intención conductual y del 17% al 31% sobre las expectativas conductuales de beber alcohol en exceso (Estudio 2). Sin embargo, en el grupo de baja experiencia personal (Estudio 2) los perfiles de emociones anticipadas (AEPs) mejoraron las predicciones hechas sobre intenciones conductuales (del 12% al 34%) pero no sobre las expectativas conductuales, mostrando que en las personas con baja experiencia personal en la conducta, las intenciones conductuales no son equivalentes a las expectativas conductuales: las emociones anticipadas tienen diferente relevancia en su predicción. Estos resultados fueron replicados usando un índice general de emociones negativas anticipadas (calculado a partir de la media de las categorías emocionales informadas).

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abraham, Ch., & Sheeran, P. (2004). Deciding to exercise: The role of anticipated regret. British journal of Health Psychology, 9, 269278. doi:10.1348/135910704773891096CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179211. doi:10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-TCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (2005). The influence of attitudes on behavior. In Albarracín, D., Johnson, B. T., & Zanna, M. P. (Eds.), The Handbook of Attitudes (pp. 173221). New Jersey, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behavior: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 471499. doi:10.1348/014466601164939CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bagozzi, R. P., Baumgartner, H., & Pieters, R. (1998). Goal-directed emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 126. doi:10.1080/026999398379754CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A., & Ferguson, M. J. (2000). Beyond behaviorism: on the automaticity of higher mental processes. Psychology Bulletin, 126, 925945. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.6.925CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumgartner, H., Pieters, R., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2008). Future-oriented emotions: conceptualization and behavioral effects. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 685696. doi:10.1002/ejsp.467CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, L., & Ajzen, I. (1991). Predicting dishonest actions using the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 25, 285301. doi:10.1016/0092-6566(91)90021-HCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becoña, E. (2000). Los adolescentes y el consumo de drogas [Teens and drugs]. Papeles del Psicólogo, 77, 2532.Google Scholar
Bermúdez, M. P., Castro, A., Madrid, J., & Buela Casal, G. (2010). Análisis de la conducta sexual de adolescentes autóctonos e inmigrantes latinoamericanos en España [Analysis of sexual behavior of indigenous and Latin American immigrants adolescents in Spain]. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 10(1), 89103.Google Scholar
Bermúdez, M. P., & Teva, I. (2004). Situación actual del sida en España e inmigración: análisis epidemiológico [Current situation of AIDS in Spain and immigration: an epidemiological analysis]. Universitas Psychologica, 8, 3748.Google Scholar
Brehm, J. W. (1999). The intensity of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 222. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0301_1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caballero, A., Carrera, P., Muñoz, D., & Sánchez, F. (2007). Emotional ambivalence in risk behaviors: The case of occasional excessive use of alcohol. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 10, 151158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caballero, A., Carrera, P., Sánchez, F., Shih, P. C., Muñoz, D., Toro, I.,… Muñoz, A. (2004). Conductas de riesgo en adolescentes [risk behaviors in adolescents.] Madrid, Spain: Concejalía de Juventud Ayuntamiento de Tres Cantos.Google Scholar
Carrera, P., Muñoz, D., & Caballero, A. (2010). Mixed emotional appeals in emotional and danger control processes. Health Communication, 25, 726736. doi:10.1080/10410236.2010.521914CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Centro Nacional de Epidemiología, ISCIII (2011). Vigilancia epidemiológica de las infecciones de transmisión sexual 1995-2009 [Epidemiological surveillance of sexually transmitted infections 1995-2009]. Madrid, Spain: Centro Nacional de Epidemiología.Google Scholar
Conner, M. T., & Sparks, P. (2005). Ambivalence and attitudes. In Stroebe, W. & Hewstone, M. (Eds). European Review of Social Psychology, (Vol 12, pp. 3770). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Dons, Ltd. doi: 10.1002/0470013478.ch2Google Scholar
Conner, M. T., Sparks, P., Povey, R., James, R., & Shepherd, R. (1998). Interpretations of healthy and unhealthy eating, and implications for dietary change, Health Education Research, 13(2), 171183. doi:10.1093/her/13.2.171Google Scholar
Cook, R., & Sheeran, P. (2004). Moderation of cognition-intention and cognition-behavior relations: A meta-analysis of properties of variables from the theory of planned behavior. British Journal of Social Psychology. 43(2), 159186. doi:10.1348/0144666041501688CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, F. D., & Warshaw, P. R. (1992). What do intention scales measure? The Journal of General Psychology, 119(4), 391407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dillard, J. P., Plotnick, C. A., Godbold, L. C., Freimuth, V. S., & Edgar, T. (1996). The multiple affective outcomes of AIDS PSAs: Fear appeals do more than scare people. Communication Research, 23, 4472. doi:10.1177/009365096023001002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. (1992). Are there basic emotions? Psychological Review, 99, 550553. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.99.3.550CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Forgas, J. P. (2001). Feeling and thinking. The role of affect in social cognition. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fox, E. (2008). Emotion Science. Basingstoke, NY: Palgrave MacMillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijda, N. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbons, F. X., Gerrard, M., & Lane, D. J. (2003). A social-reaction model of adolescent health risk. In Suls, J. J. & Wallston, K. A. (Eds.), Social Psychological Foundations of Health and Illness. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. doi:10.1002/9780470753552.ch5Google Scholar
Gibbons, F. X. (2006). Behavioral intentions, expectations, and willingness. In Gerrard, M. & McCaul, K. D. (Eds.), Constructs and measures web resource. National Cancer Institute Internet Web Site. retrieved from: http//cancercontrol.cancer.gov/brp/constructs/intent-expect-willingness/index.html.Google Scholar
Giner-Sorolla, R. (1999). Affect in attitude. Immediate and deliberative perspectives. In Chaiken, S. & Trope, Y. (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 441461). New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
Gordon, R. A. (1990). Informational bases of behavioral intentions and behavioral expectations or self-predictions. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 11(4), 433442. doi:10.1207/s15324834basp1104_6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haddock, G., & zanna, M. (1999). Cognition, affect, and the prediction of social attitudes. In Stroebe, W. & Hewstone, M. (Eds.), European Review of Social Psychology, (Vol 10, pp. 7599). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Dons, Ltd. doi:10.1080/14792779943000026Google Scholar
Hurtado, A., Fernández-Dols, J. M., Parrot, G., & Carrera, P. (2010). Emotion terms, category structure, and the problem of translation: the case of shame and vergüenza. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 661680. doi:10.1080/02699930902958255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inglés, C. J., Delgado, B., Bautista, R., Torregrosa, M. S., Espada, J. P., García-Fernández, J. M.,… García-López, L. J. (2007). Factores psicosociales relacionados con el consumo de alcohol y tabaco en adolescentes españoles. [Psychosocial factors related with alcohol and tobacco use in Spanish adolescents.] International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 7(2), 403420.Google Scholar
Parker, D., Manstead, A. S. R., & Stradling, S. G. (1995). Extending the theory of planned behavior: The role of personal norm. British Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 127137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perugini, M., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2001). The role of desires and anticipated emotions in goal-directed behaviors: Broadening and deepening the theory of planned behavior. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 7998. doi:10.1348/014466601164704CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pomery, E. A., Gibbons, F. X., Reis-Bergan, M., & Gerrard, M. (2009). From willingness to intention: experience moderates the shift from reactive to reasoned behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(7), 894908. doi:10.1177/0146167209335166CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rhodes, R. E., & Matheson, H. (2005). Discrepancies in exercise intention and expectation: theoretical and applied issues. Psychology and Health, 20, 6378. doi:10.1080/08870440412331296071CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richard, R., van der Pligt, J., & de Vries, N. (1995). Anticipated affective reactions and prevention of AIDS. British Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 14111428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richard, R., van der Pligt, J., & de Vries, N. (1996). Anticipated affect and behavioral choice. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 18, 111129. doi:10.1207/s15324834basp1802_1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: Evidence for an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 934960. doi:10.1037/00332909.128.6.934CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. (1991). Essentials of behavioral research: Methods and data analysis (2nd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Sheeran, P. (2002). Intention-behavior relations: A conceptual and empirical review. In Stroebe, W. & Hewstone, M. (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 136). Chichester, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
Sheppard, B. H., Hartwick, J., & Warshaw, P. R. (1988). The theory of reasoned action: A meta-analysis of past research with recommendations for modifications and future research. Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 325343. doi:10.1086/209170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teva, M. P., Bermúdez, M. P., & Buela-Casal, G. (2009). Characteristics of sexual behavior in Spanish adolescents. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 12(2), 471484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trafimow, D. (2004). Problems with change in R 2 as applied to theory of reasoned action research. British Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 515530. doi:10.1348/0144666042565344CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warshaw, P. R., & Davis, F. D. (1984). Self-Understanding and the accuracy of behavioral expectations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10(1), 111118. doi:10.1177/0146167284101013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeelenberg, M. (1999). Anticipated regret, expected feedback and behavioral decision-making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12, 93106. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0771(199906)12:2 <93::AID-BDM311>3 .0.CO;2-S3.0.CO;2-S>CrossRefGoogle Scholar