Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T10:24:20.977Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Age and Gender Differences in Emotion Regulation Strategies: Autobiographical Memory, Rumination, Problem Solving and Distraction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 July 2016

Jorge Javier Ricarte Trives
Affiliation:
Universidad de Castilla-La Mancha (Spain)
Beatriz Navarro Bravo*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Castilla-La Mancha (Spain)
José Miguel Latorre Postigo
Affiliation:
Universidad de Castilla-La Mancha (Spain)
Laura Ros Segura
Affiliation:
Universidad de Castilla-La Mancha (Spain)
Ed Watkins
Affiliation:
University of Exeter (UK)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed Beatriz Navarro Bravo. Departamento de Psicología. Facultad de Medicina. Calle Almansa 14. 02006. Albacete (Spain). E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Our study tested the hypothesis that older adults and men use more adaptive emotion regulatory strategies but fewer negative emotion regulatory strategies than younger adults and women. In addition, we tested the hypothesis that rumination acts as a mediator variable for the effect of age and gender on depression scores. Differences in rumination, problem solving, distraction, autobiographical recall and depression were assessed in a group of young adults (18–29 years) compared to a group of older adults (50–76 years). The older group used more problem solving and distraction strategies when in a depressed state than their younger counterparts (ps < .05). The younger participants reported more rumination (p < .01). Women scored higher in depression scores and lower in distraction than men (ps < .05). There were no significant effects of age, gender, or interaction of age by gender on the recall of specific autobiographical memories (ps > .06). Ordinary least squares regression analyses with bootstrapping showed that rumination mediated the association between age, gender and depression scores. These results suggest that older adults and men select more adaptive strategies to regulate emotions than young adults and women with rumination acting as a significant mediator variable in the association between age, gender, and depression.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004 Google Scholar
Alea, N., Bluck, S., & Semegon, A. B. (2004). Young and adults’ expression of emotional experience. Do autobiographical narratives tell a different story? Journal of Adult Development, 11, 235250. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/B:JADE.0000044527.52470.5d Google Scholar
Artistico, D., Orom, H., Cervone, D., Krauss, S., & Houston, E. (2010). Everyday challenges in context: The influence of contextual factors on everyday problem solving among young, middle-aged, and older adults. Experimental Aging Research, 36, 230247. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03610731003613938 Google Scholar
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 11731182. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173 Google Scholar
Beaman, A., Pushkar, D., Etezadi, S., Bye, D., & Conway, M. (2007). Autobiographical memory specificity predicts social problem-solving ability in old and young adults. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 12751288. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470210600943450 Google Scholar
Blanchard-Fields, F., Mienaltowski, A., & Seay, R. B. (2007). Age differences in everyday problem-solving effectiveness: Older adults select more effective strategies for interpersonal problems. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 62(1), 6164. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/62.1.P61 Google Scholar
Blanchard-Fields, F., Stein, R., & Watson, T. L. (2004). Age differences in emotion-regulation strategies in handling everyday problems. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 59, 261269. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/59.6.P261 Google Scholar
Butler, L. D., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1994). Gender differences in responses to depressed mood in a college sample. Sex Roles, 30, 331346. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01420597 Google Scholar
Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously. A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54, 165181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.165 Google Scholar
Coats, A. B. (2007). Age-related effects of online emotion regulation strategies on mood and memory. Atlanta, GA: Georgia Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Donaldson, C., Lam, D., & Mathews, A. (2007). Rumination and attention in major depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 26642678. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.07.002 Google Scholar
Erskine, J. A. K., Kvavilashvili, L., Conway, M. A., & Myers, L. (2007). The effects of age on psychopathology, well-being and repressive coping. Aging & Mental Health, 11, 394404. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607860600963737 Google Scholar
Extremera, N., & Fernandez-Berrocal, P. (2006). Validity and reliability of Spanish versions of the Ruminative Responses Scale-Short Form and the Distraction Responses Scale in a sample of Spanish high school and college students. Psychological Reports, 98(1), 141150.Google Scholar
Gallo, D. A., Korthauer, L. E., McDonough, I. M., Teshale, S., & Johnson, E. L. (2011). Age-related positivity effects and autobiographical memory detail: Evidence from a past/future source memory task. Memory, 19, 641652. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2011.595723 Google Scholar
Goddard, L., Dritschel, B., & Burton, A. (1996). Role of autobiographical memory in social problem solving and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 609616. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.105.4.609 Google Scholar
Gross, J. J., Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Tsai, J., Skorpen, C. G., & Hsu, A. Y. C. (1997). Emotion and aging: Experience, expression, and control. Psychology & Aging, 12, 590599. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.12.4.590 Google Scholar
Haringsma, R., Spinhoven, P., Engels, G. I., & van der Leeden, R. (2010). Effects of sad mood on autobiographical memory in older adults with and without lifetime depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49, 343357.Google Scholar
Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76, 408420. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03637750903310360 Google Scholar
Heron, J., Crane, C., Gunnell, D., Lewis, G., Evans, J., & Williams, J. M. G. (2012). 40,000 memories in young teenagers: Psychometric properties of the Autobiographical Memory Test in a UK cohort study. Memory, 20, 300320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2012.656846 Google Scholar
Hilt, L. M., & Pollak, S. D. (2012). Getting out of rumination: Comparison of three brief interventions in a sample of youth. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 11571165. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9638-3 Google Scholar
Huffziger, S., Reinhard, I., & Kuehner, C. (2009). A longitudinal study of rumination and distraction in formerly depressed inpatients and community controls. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 746756. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016946 Google Scholar
John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. Journal of Personality, 72, 13011334. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00298.x Google Scholar
Kryla-Lingthall, N., & Mather, M. (2009). The role of cognitive control in older adults’s emotional well being. In Bengtson, V., Silverstein, M., Putney, N., & Gans, D. (Eds.), Handbook on Theories of Aging (pp. 323344). New York, NY: Springer Publishing.Google Scholar
Latorre, J. M., & Montañés, J. (1997). Depression in the elderly: Assessment, implicated variables and relation to cognitive impairment. Revista de Psicopatología y Psicología Clínica, 2, 243264.Google Scholar
Levine, B., Svoboda, E., Hay, J. F., Winocur, G., & Moscovitch, M. (2002). Aging and autobiographical memory: Dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychology & Aging, 17, 677689. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.677 Google Scholar
Lima, M. M., Fernández-Berrocal, P., Extremera, N., & Queirós, P. S. (2006). Physical activity in the elderly: Analysis of relations to perceived emotional intelligence and styles of response to depression. Ansiedad y Estrés, 12, 293303.Google Scholar
Lopez, C. M., Driscoll, K. A., & Kistner, J. A. (2009). Sex differences and response styles: Subtypes of rumination and associations with depressive symptoms. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 38(1), 2735. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15374410802575412 Google Scholar
Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Aging and motivated cognition: The positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 496502. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.005 Google Scholar
Mather, M., & Knight, M. (2005). Goal-directed memory: The role of cognitive control in older adults’ emotional memory. Psychology & Aging, 20, 554570. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.554 Google Scholar
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1990). Sex differences in depression. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569582 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.569 Google Scholar
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Larson, J., & Grayson, C. (1999). Explaining the gender difference in depressive symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 10611072. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.5.1061 Google Scholar
OECD. (2013). Education at Glance 2013 . OECD Indicators. Paris, France: OECD Publishing.Google Scholar
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 36, 717731. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03206553 Google Scholar
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879891. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.3.879 Google Scholar
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385401. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014662167700100306 Google Scholar
Radloff, L. S., & Teri, L. (1986). Use of the center for epidemiological studies depression scale with older adults. Clinical Gerontologist, 5, 119136.Google Scholar
Raes, F., Hermans, D., Williams, J. M. G., & Eelen, P. (2007). A sentence completion procedure as an alternative to the Autobiographical Memory Test for assessing overgeneral memory in non-clinical populations. Memory, 15, 495507. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210701390982 Google Scholar
Ricarte, J., Latorre, J. M., Ros, L., Navarro, B., Aguilar, M. J., & Serrano, J. P. (2011). Overgeneral autobiographical memory effect in older depressed adults. Aging & Mental Health, 15, 10281037. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2011.573468 Google Scholar
Ricarte, J., Ros, L., Serrano, J. P., Martinez-Lorca, M., & Latorre, J. M. (2015). Age differences in rumination and autobiographical retrieval. Aging & Mental Health, 17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2015.1060944 Google ScholarPubMed
Ros, L., & Latorre, J. M. (2010). Gender and age differences in the recall of affective autobiographical memories using the autobiographical memory test. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 950954. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.08.002 Google Scholar
Ros, L., Ricarte, J. J., Serrano, J. P., Nieto, M., Aguilar, M. J., & Latorre, J. M. (2014). Overgeneral autobiographical memories: Gender differences in depression. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 472480. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.3013 Google Scholar
Scheibe, S., & Blanchard-Fields, F. (2009). Effects of regulating emotions on cognitive performance: What is costly for young adults is not so costly for older adults. Psychology & Aging, 24, 217223. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013807 Google Scholar
Scheibe, S., & Carstensen, L. L. (2010). Emotional aging: Recent findings and future trends. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 65B, 135144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbp132 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Segerstrom, S. C., Roach, A. R., Evans, D. R., Schipper, L. J., & Darville, A. K. (2010). The structure and health correlates of trait repetitive thought in older adults. Psychology & Aging, 25, 505515. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019456 Google Scholar
Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (2001). Getting older, getting better? Personal strivings and psychological maturity across the life span. Developmental Psychology, 37, 491501. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.37.4.491 Google Scholar
Shiota, M. N., & Levenson, R. W. (2009). Effects of aging on experimentally instructed detached reappraisal, positive reappraisal, and emotional behavior suppression. Psychology & Aging, 24, 890900. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017896 Google Scholar
St Jacques, P. L., & Levine, B. (2007). Ageing and autobiographical memory for emotional and neutral events. Memory, 15, 129144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210601119762 Google Scholar
Sumner, J. A. (2012). The mechanisms underlying overgeneral autobiographical memory: An evaluative review of evidence for the CaR-FA-X model. Clinical Psychology Review, 32(1), 3448. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.10.003 Google Scholar
Sumner, J. A., Griffith, J. W., & Mineka, S. (2010). Overgeneral autobiographical memory as a predictor of the course of depression: A meta-analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 614625. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.03.013 Google Scholar
Thomsen, D. K., Mehlsen, M. Y., Viidik, A., Sommerlund, B., & Zachariae, R. (2005). Age and gender differences in negative affect – Is there a role for emotion regulation? Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 19351946. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.12.001 Google Scholar
Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 163206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163 Google Scholar
Watkins, E., & Moulds, M. (2005). Distinct modes of ruminative self-focus: Impact of abstract versus concrete rumination on problem solving in depression. Emotion, 5, 319328. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.3.319 Google Scholar
Watkins, E. R., Mullan, E., Wingrove, J., Rimes, K., Steiner, H., Bathurst, N., … Scott, J. (2011). Rumination-focused cognitive-behavioural therapy for residual depression: Phase II randomized controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry, 199, 317322. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.110.090282 Google Scholar
Watkins, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2001). Rumination and overgeneral memory in depression: Effects of self-focus and analytic thinking. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 353357. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.110.2.333 Google Scholar
Wilkinson, P. O., Croudace, T. J., & Goodyer, I. M. (2013). Rumination, anxiety, depressive symptoms and subsequent depression in adolescents at risk for psychopathology: A longitudinal cohort study. BMC Psychiatry, 13, 250. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-13-250 Google Scholar
Williams, J. M. G., Barnhofer, T., Crane, C., Herman, D., Raes, F., Watkins, E., & Dalgleish, T. (2007). Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 122148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.122 Google Scholar
Williams, J. M., & Broadbent, K. (1986). Autobiographical memory in suicide attempters. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 144149. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.95.2.144 Google Scholar