Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T06:14:00.808Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

SHRIEKING SIRENS: SCHEMATA, SCRIPTS, AND SOCIAL NORMS. HOW CHANGE OCCURS

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 December 2018

Cristina Bicchieri
Affiliation:
Philosophy and Psychology, University of Pennsylvania
Peter McNally
Affiliation:
Philosophy, Politics, and Economics, University of Pennsylvania

Abstract:

This essay investigates the relationships among scripts, schemata, and social norms. The authors examine how social norms are triggered by particular schemata and are grounded in scripts. Just as schemata are embedded in a network, so too are social norms, and they can be primed through spreading activation. Moreover, the expectations that allow a social norm’s existence are inherently grounded in particular scripts and schemata. Using interventions that have targeted gender norms, open defecation, female genital cutting, and other collective issues as examples, the authors argue that ignoring the cognitive underpinnings of a social norm can hamper the effectiveness of behavioral interventions.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Social Philosophy and Policy Foundation 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

We are grateful to Barbara Mellers, Jon Baron, and Robert Seyfarth for their helpful suggestions and comments on this essay. We are also grateful to Jayson Dorsett for his helpful assistance in reviewing relevant literature.

References

1 Allport, Gordon, The Nature of Prejudice (New York: Basic Books, 1979), 179.Google Scholar

2 Bicchieri, Cristina, The Grammar of Society: The Nature and Dynamics of Social Norms (New York: Cambridge University Press, 2006).Google Scholar

3 Bicchieri, The Grammar of Society; Coleman, James Samuel, Foundations of Social Theory (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1994);Google Scholar Elster, Jon, “Social Norms and Economic Theory,” Journal of Economic Perspectives 3, no. 4 (1989): 99117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

4 Bicchieri, Grammar of Society.

5 Bicchieri, Cristina and Muldoon, Ryan, “Social Norms,” Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, ed. Zalta, Edward N. (Stanford, CA: Center for Study of Language and Information, 2011), para 1.Google Scholar

6 Bicchieri, Grammar of Society, chap. 1.

7 This will not be the case with a moral norm, where preferences for following such a norm are typically socially unconditional.

8 Ibid.

9 Cialdini, Robert B., Reno, Raymond R., and Kallgren, Carl A., “A Focus Theory of Normative Conduct: Recycling the Concept of Norms to Reduce Littering in Public Places,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58, no. 6 (1990): 1015–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

10 Most of what we are saying will also apply to descriptive norms.

11 World Bank, World Development Report 2015: Mind, Society, and Behavior (Washington, DC: World Bank, 2015) doi: 10.1596/978-1-4648-0342-0.Google Scholar License: Creative Commons Attribution CC BY 3.0 IGO

12 Fiske, Susan and Taylor, Shelley, Social Cognition (London: Sage, 1991);Google Scholar Piaget, Jean, “Piaget’s Theory,” in Piaget and His School, ed. Zwingmann, Charles, Inhelder, Bärbel, and Chapman, Harold (New York; Berlin: Springer, 1976), 1123;CrossRefGoogle Scholar Rumelhart, David, “Schemata: The Building Blocks of Cognition,” in Spiro, Rand, Bertram, Bruce, and Brewer, Willian, eds. Theoretical Issues in Reading Comprehension: Perspectives from Cognitive Psychology, Linguistics, Artificial Intelligence, and Education (Abingdon, UK: Routledge, 1980);Google Scholar Rumelhart, David, “The Architecture of Mind: A Connectionist Approach,” Mind Readings (1998): 207238;Google Scholar Rumelhart, David, McClelland, James, and the PDP Research Group, Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructures of Cognition, vol. 1 (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1986);Google Scholar Iran-Nejad, Asghar, and Winsler, Adam, “Bartlett’s Schema Theory and Modern Accounts of Learning and Remembering,” Journal of Mind and Behavior 21, nos. 1-2 (2000): 535.Google Scholar

13 Nadkarni, Sucheta and Narayanan, Vadake K., “Strategic Schemas, Strategic Flexibility, and Firm Performance: The Moderating Role of Industry Clockspeed,” Strategic Management Journal 28, no. 3 (2007): 243–70;CrossRefGoogle Scholar Rumelhart et al., Parallel Distributed Processing.

14 Rosch, Eleanor, “Natural Categories,” Cognitive Psychology 4, no. 3 (1973): 328–50;CrossRefGoogle Scholar “Principles of Categorization,” in Eleanor Rosch and Barbara Lloyd, eds., Cognition and Categorization (Hillsdale, NJ: Elbaum, 1978), 189–206.

15 Rosch, Eleanor, Simpson, Carol, and Miller, R. Scott, “Structural Bases of Typicality Effects,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2, no. 4 (1976): 491;Google Scholar Kinsella, Elaine L., Ritchie, Timothy D., and Igou, Eric R., “Zeroing in on Heroes: A Prototype Analysis of Hero Features,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 108, no. 1 (2015): 114–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

16 Piaget, “Piaget’s Theory”; Rumelhart et al., Parallel Distributed Processing.

17 Rosch, “Principles of Categorization.”

18 Ibid.; Crocker, Jennifer, Fiske, Susan T., and Taylor, Shelley E., “Schematic Bases of Belief Change,” in Attitudinal Judgment, ed. Eiser, J. Richard (New York: Springer, 1984), 197226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

19 Crocker, Fiske, and Taylor, “Schematic Bases of Belief Change”; Iran-Nejad, Ashgar and Winsler, Adam, “Bartlett’s Schema Theory and Modern Accounts of Learning and Remembering,” Journal of Mind and Behavior (2000): 535;Google Scholar Mori, Naohisa, “The Schema Approach: A Dynamic View on Remembering,” in Dynamic Process Methodology in the Social and Developmental Science (New York: Springer, 2009), 123–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

20 Xiao, Erte and Bicchieri, Cristina, “When Equality Trumps Reciprocity,” Journal of Economic Psychology 31, no. 3 (2010): 456–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

21 Rumelhart et al., Parallel Distributed Processing: Vol 1.

22 Collins, Allan M. and Loftus, Elizabeth F., “A Spreading-Activation Theory of Semantic Processing,” Psychological Review 82, no. 6 (1975): 407;CrossRefGoogle Scholar Collins, Allan M. and Quillian, M. Ross, “Retrieval Time from Semantic Memory,” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 8, no. 2 (1969): 240–47;CrossRefGoogle Scholar Woods, William, “What’s in a Link: Foundations for Semantic Networks,” Representation and Understanding: Studies in Cognitive Science (1975): 35–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

23 Lerner, Itamar, Bentin, Shlomo, and Shriki, Oren, “Spreading Activation in an Attractor Network with Latching Dynamics: Automatic Semantic Priming Revisited,” Cognitive Science 36, no. 8 (2012): 13391382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

24 Collins and Loftus, “Spreading-Activation Theory of Semantic Processing.”

25 Duncan, Ravit Golan, “The Role of Domain-Specific Knowledge in Generative Reasoning about Complicated Multileveled Phenomena,” Cognition and Instruction 25, no. 4 (2007): 271336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

26 Casson, Ronald W., “Schemata in Cognitive Anthropology,” Annual Review of Anthropology (1983): 429–62;Google Scholar D’Andrade, Roy Goodwin, “The Cultural Part of Cognition,” Cognitive Science 5, no. 3 (1981): 179–95;CrossRefGoogle Scholar Strauss, Claudia and Quinn, Naomi, A Cognitive Theory of Cultural Meaning, Vol. 9 (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1997).Google Scholar

27 Strauss and Quinn, A Cognitive Theory of Cultural Meaning.

28 Rosch, “Natural Categories.”

29 He was specifically referring to ethnic categories when he said this, but we think his quotation applies to social categories and social schemata in general.

30 Allport, The Nature of Prejudice, 179.

31 Abelson, Robert P., “Psychological Status of the Script Concept,” American Psychologist 36, no. 7 (1981): 715;CrossRefGoogle Scholar Schank, Roger C. and Abelson, Robert P., Scripts, Plans, Goals, and Understanding: An Inquiry into Human Knowledge Structures (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2013 [1977]).CrossRefGoogle Scholar

32 Bicchieri, Grammar of Society.

33 Bettenhausen, Kenneth and Murnighan, J. Keith, “The Emergence of Norms in Competitive Decision-Making Groups,” Administrative Science Quarterly 30, no. 3 (1985): 350–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

34 Liberman, Varda, Samuels, Steven M., and Ross, Lee, “The Name of the Game: Predictive Power of Reputations versus Situational Labels in Determining Prisoner’s Dilemma Game Moves,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 30, no. 9 (2004): 11751185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

35 Henrich, Joseph Patrick et al., Foundations of Human Sociality: Economic Experiments and Ethnographic Evidence from Fifteen Small-Scale Societies (New York: Oxford University Press, 2004).CrossRefGoogle Scholar

36 Cronk, Lee, “The Influence of Cultural Framing on Play in the Trust Game: A Maasai Example,” Evolution and Human Behavior 28, no. 5 (2007): 352–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

37 Yoder, Stanley, “Examining Ethnomedical Diagnoses and Treatment Choices for Diarrheal Disorders in Lubumbashi Swahili,” Medical Anthropology 16, nos. 1-4 (1994): 211–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

38 Cialdini et al., “A Focus Theory of Normative Conduct.”

39 Bicchieri, Grammar of Society; Harvey, Michael D. and Enzle, Michael E., “A Cognitive Model of Social Norms for Understanding the Transgression–Helping Effect,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 41, no. 5 (1981): 866–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

40 Macaulay, Jacqueline, “A Shill for Charity,” in Macaulay, Jacqueline and Berkowitz, Leonard, eds., Altruism and Helping Behavior (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press 1970), 4359.Google Scholar

41 Harvey and Enzle, “A Cognitive Model of Social Norms.”

42 Cialdini et al., “A Focus Theory of Normative Conduct.”

43 Aarts, Henk and Dijksterhuis, Ap, “The Silence of the Library: Environment, Situational Norm, and Social Behavior,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 84, no. 1 (2003): 18–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

44 See Frey, Bruno and Bohnet, Iris, “Institutions Affect Fairness: Experimental Investigations,” Journal of Institutional and Theoretical Economics (JITE)/Zeitschrift für die gesamte Staatswissenschaft 151, no. 2 (1995): 286303.Google Scholar See also Hoffman, Elizabeth et al., “Preferences, Property Rights, and Anonymity in Bargaining Games,” Games and Economic Behavior 7, no. 3 (1994): 346–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

45 Lippmann, W., Public Opinion (New York: Harcourt-Brace, 1922), 99.Google Scholar

46 Brown, Rupert, Prejudice: Its Social Psychology (Chichester, UK: John Wiley and Sons, 2011);Google Scholar Crocker et al., “Schematic Bases of Belief Change”; Hilton, James and Von Hippel, William, “Stereotypes,” Annual Review of Psychology 47, no. 1 (1996): 237–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

47 Piaget, “Piaget’s Theory.”

48 Piaget, Jean, “Part I: Cognitive Development in Children: Piaget Development and Learning,” Journal of Research in Science Teaching 3 (1964): 176–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

49 Mandler, Jean Matter, Stories, Scripts, and Scenes: Aspects of Schema Theory (New York: Psychology Press, 2014).CrossRefGoogle Scholar

50 Crocker et al., “Schematic Bases of Belief Change.”

51 This proves to be particularly problematic for social norms, as the stimuli that activate a norm are often highly social in nature

52 Devine, Patricia, Hirt, Edward R., and Gehrke, Elizabeth M., “Diagnostic and Confirmation Strategies in Trait Hypothesis Testing,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58, no. 6 (1990): 952;CrossRefGoogle Scholar Nickerson, Raymond S., “Confirmation Bias: A Ubiquitous Phenomenon in Many Guises,” Review of General Psychology 2, no. 2 (1998): 175;CrossRefGoogle Scholar Kunda, Ziva, “The Case for Motivated Reasoning,” Psychological Bulletin 108, no. 3 (1990): 480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

53 Brown, William J., “Prosocial Effects of Entertainment Television in India,” Asian Journal of Communication 1, no. 1 (1990): 113–35;CrossRefGoogle Scholar Crocker et al., “Schematic Bases of Belief Change”; Hewstone, Miles, Hopkins, Nicholas, and Routh, David A., “Cognitive Models of Stereotype Change: (1). Generalization and Subtyping in Young People’s Views of the Police,” European Journal of Social Psychology 22, no. 3 (1992): 219–34;CrossRefGoogle Scholar Hewstone, Miles, Johnston, Lucy, and Aird, Peter, “Cognitive Models of Stereotype Change: (2) Perceptions of Homogeneous and Heterogeneous Groups,” European Journal of Social Psychology 22, no. 3 (1992): 235–49;CrossRefGoogle Scholar Queller, Sarah and Smith, Eliot R., “Subtyping versus Bookkeeping in Stereotype Learning and Change: Connectionist Simulations and Empirical Findings,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 82, no. 3 (2002): 300;CrossRefGoogle Scholar Rothbart, Myron, “Memory Processes and Social Beliefs,” in Hamilton, David, ed., Cognitive Processes in Stereotyping and Intergroup Behavior (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum 1981), 145–81;Google Scholar Weber, Renee and Crocker, Jennifer, “Cognitive Processes in the Revision of Stereotypic Beliefs,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45, no. 5 (1983): 961.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

54 Crocker et al., “Schematic Bases of Belief Change.”

55 Rothbart, “Memory Processes and Social Beliefs.” See also: Rumelhart, David E. and Norman, Donald A., “Accretion, Tuning and Restructuring: Three Modes of Learning,” in Cotton, J. W. and Klatskey, R. L., eds., Schematic Factors in Cognition (Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum 1978).Google Scholar

56 Rothbart, “Memory Processes and Social Beliefs.”

57 Hewstone et al., “Generalization and Subtyping in Young People.”

58 Gurwitz, Sharon and Dodge, Kenneth A., “Effects of Confirmations and Disconfirmations on Stereotype-Based Attributions,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 35, no. 7 (1977): 495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

59 Weber and Crocker, “Revision of Stereotypic Beliefs.”

60 For similar results, see Hewstone et al., “Perceptions of Homogenous and Heterogeneous Groups.”

61 Johnston, Lucy and Hewstone, Miles, “Cognitive Models of Stereotype Change: 3. Subtyping and the Perceived Typicality of Disconfirming Group Members,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 28, no. 4 (1992): 360–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

62 Ibid.

63 Weber and Crocker, “Revision of Stereotypic Beliefs.”

64 We consider a “prototype analysis” to be the best way to measure a schema. Such measurement will be important when attempting to determine the structure of a schema for a population and the degree to which such a structure is shared. For example of work using prototype analysis, see: Fehr, Beverley, “Prototype Analysis of the Concepts of Love and Commitment,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 55, no. 4 (1988): 557–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

65 Crocker et al., “Schematic Bases of Belief Change.”

66 In fact, cognitive therapy largely relies on alternative schemata as replacements when attempting to reduce reliance on maladaptive ones. See Padesky, Christine, “Schema Change Processes in Cognitive Therapy,” Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy 1, no. 5 (1994): 267–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

67 Cialdini et al., “A Focus Theory of Normative Conduct.”

68 Anderson, Craig, “Imagination and Expectation: The Effect of Imagining Behavioral Scripts on Personal Influences,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45, no. 2 (1983): 293305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

69 Sanan, Deepak and Moulik, Soma Ghosh, “Community-Led Total Sanitation in Rural Areas: An Approach that Works,” (Washington, DC: Water and Sanitation Program, 2007);Google Scholar Toubia, Nahid F. and Sharief, E. H., “Female Genital Mutilation: Have We Made Progress?” International Journal of Gynecology and Obstetrics 82, no. 3 (2003): 251–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

70 Also known as female genital mutilation (FGM).

71 E.g., Althaus, Frances, “Female Circumcision: Rite of Passage or Violation of Rights?" International Family Planning Perspectives (1997): 130–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

72 Helmore, Kristin. “In Sudan: Changing Labels, Changing Lives” (2012). Retrieved from United Nations website: https://www.unfpa.org/public/home/news/pid/11223Google Scholar

73 Toubia and Sharief, “Female Genital Mutilation: Have We Made Progress?”

74 Helmore, “In Sudan: Changing Labels, Changing Lives,” para. 4.

75 Helmore, “In Sudan: Changing Labels, Changing Lives.”

76 Behrendt, Alice, “Female Genital Cutting in Moyamba and Bombali Districts of Sierra Leone: Perceptions, Attitudes and Practice” (Dakar, Senegal: Plan International, 2005).Google Scholar

77 Johnston and Hewstone, “Subtyping and Perceived Typicality.”

78 Joint evaluation of the UNFPA-UNICEF Joint Programme on Female Genital Mutilation / Cutting (FGM/C): Accelerating Change (2008–2012).

79 Evans, W. Douglas, “Saleema Evaluation Annual Report Year 1 (Phases 1-2)” submitted to UNICEF Sudan (2016).Google Scholar

80 Helmore, “In Sudan: Changing Labels, Changing Lives.”

81 Toubia and Sharief, “Female Genital Mutilation: Have We Made Progress?”

82 Ibid.

83 Bicchieri and Mercier, “Norms and Beliefs: How Change Occurs”; Kahan, Dan, “Gentle Nudges versus Hard Shoves: Solving the Sticky Norms Problem,” University of Chicago Law Review (2000): 607–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

84 Toubia and Sharief. “Female Genital Mutilation: Have We Made Progress?”

85 Galbraith, Colleen and Thomas, Ann, “Community Approaches to Total Sanitation: Based on Case Studies from India, Nepal, Sierra Leone, Zambia,” in Field Notes of UNICEF’s Division of Policy and Practice (New York: UNICEF, 2009);Google Scholar Kar, Kamal and Chambers, Robert, “Handbook on Community-Led Total Sanitation,” (London: Plan UK, 2008).Google Scholar

86 Galbraith and Thomas, “Community Approaches to Total Sanitation.”

87 Mukherjee, Nilanjana, Robiarto, Amin, Saputra, Efentrif, and Wartonio, D. Joko, “Achieving and Sustaining Open Defecation-Free Communities: Learning from East Java,” Report from WSP (Washington, DC: World Bank 2012);Google Scholar Wateraid, Comparison and Adaptation of Social Change Dynamics for the Collective Abandonment of Open Defecation (2008). Retrieved from Wateraid International website: http://www.wateraid.org/∼/media/Publications/open-defecation-social-change-dynamics-ghana-study.pdf

88 Kamal Kar and Robert Chambers, “Handbook on Community-Led Total Sanitation.”

89 Galbraith and Thomas, “Community Approaches to Total Sanitation”; Kamal Kar and Robert Chambers, “Handbook on Community-Led Total Sanitation.”

90 Kamal Kar and Robert Chambers, “Handbook on Community-Led Total Sanitation.”

91 Ibid, 27.

92 Ibid.

93 Ibid.

94 Bongartz, Petra, Musyoki, Samuel Musembi, Milligan, Angela, and Ashley, Holly, “Tales of Shit: Community-Led Total Sanitation in Africa–An Overview,” Participatory Learning and Action 61, no. 1 (2010): 2750.Google Scholar

95 Kamal Kar and Robert Chambers, “Handbook on Community-Led Total Sanitation.”

96 We say “script” here as we are specifically referring to private defecation as an event. Open defecation is also a loose script, without having highly ordered action elements. However, as we are more interested in open defecation as a general concept, we have referred to it as a schema—technically either term is applicable.

97 Bicchieri, Cristina, Norms in the Wild: How to Diagnose, Measure, and Change Social Norms (New York: Oxford University Press, 2016).Google Scholar

98 Kar, Kamal and Pasteur, Katherine, “Subsidy or Self-Respect? Community-Led Total Sanitation; An Update on Recent Developments,” Institute of Development Studies (2005).Google Scholar

99 Pickering, Amy J., Djebbari, Habiba, Lopez, Carolina, Coulibaly, Massa, and Alzua, Maria Laura, “Effect of a Community-Led Sanitation Intervention on Child Diarrhoea and Child Growth in Rural Mali: A Cluster-Randomised Controlled Trial,” Lancet Global Health 3, no. 11 (2015): e701e711.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

100 Mukherjee, Nilaniana, Robiarto, A., Saputra, E., and Wartono, D., “Achieving and Sustaining Open Defecation Free Communities: Learning From East Java,” Report from WSP, (Washington, DC: World Bank, 2012).Google Scholar

101 Galbraith, Colleen and Thomas, Ann, “Community Approaches to Total Sanitation: Based on case studies from India, Nepal, Sierra Leone, Zambia,” in Field notes of UNICEF’s Division of Policy and Practice (New York: UNICEF, 2009).Google Scholar

102 Sanan, Deepak and Moulik, Soma Ghosh, “Community-Led Total Sanitation in Rural Areas: An Approach that Works” (2007).Google Scholar

103 Kumar, Santosh and Vollmer, Sebastian, “Does Access to Improved Sanitation Reduce Childhood Diarrhea in Rural India?” Health Economics 22, no. 4 (2013): 410–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

104 Coffee, Diane, Gupta, Aashish, Hathi, Payal, Khurana, Nidhi, Spears, Dean, Srivastav, Nikhil, and Vyas, Sangita, “Revealed Preference for Open Defecation,” Economic and Political Weekly 49, no. 38 (2014): 4355Google Scholar

105 Ibid.

106 Complete abandonment is needed for any meaningful health improvements to take place.

107 Bicchieri, Norms in the Wild.

108 Mansbridge, Jane and Flaster, Katherine, “Male Chauvinist, Feminist, Sexist, and Sexual Harassment: Different Trajectories in Feminist Linguistic Innovation,” American Speech 80, no. 3 (2005): 261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

109 Ibid.

110 Ibid, 263.

111 Department of Environment and Conservation (DEC), Sustainability Programs Division, “Don’t Be a Tosser: Litter Prevention Campaign 2002” (2005). Retrieved: http://www.environment.nsw.gov.au/resources/warr/2005034_ed_dontbeatosser_cs.pdf

112 Campbell, Fiona, People Who Litter (Wigan, UK: ENCAMS, 2007).Google Scholar

113 Brown, “Prosocial Effects of Entertainment Television in India”; Brown, William, “Sociocultural Influences of Prodevelopment Soap Operas in the Third World,” Journal of Popular Film and Television 19, no. 4 (1992): 157–64;CrossRefGoogle Scholar Jensen, Robert and Oster, Emily, “The Power of TV: Cable Television and Women’s Status in India,” The Quarterly Journal of Economics 124, no. 3 (2009): 1057–94;CrossRefGoogle Scholar La Ferrara, Eliana, Chong, Alberto, and Duryea, Suzanne, “Soap Operas and Fertility: Evidence from Brazil,” American Economic Journal: Applied Economics 4, no. 4 (2012): 131;Google Scholar Paluck, Elizabeth Levy. “What’s in a Norm? Sources and Processes of Norm Change,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96, no. 3 (2009): 594600;CrossRefGoogle Scholar Paluck, Elizabeth Levy, “Reducing Intergroup Prejudice and Conflict Using the Media: A Field Experiment in Rwanda,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96, no. 3 (2009): 574–87;CrossRefGoogle Scholar Rogers, Everett, Vaughan, Peter W., Swalehe, Ramadhan M. A., Rao, Nagesh, Svenkerud, Peer; Sood, Suruchi, “Effects of an Entertainment-Education Radio Soap Opera on Family Planning Behavior in Tanzania,” Studies in Family Planning 30, no. 3 (1999): 193211;CrossRefGoogle Scholar Trujillo, Matthew and Paluck, Elizabeth Levy, “The Devil Knows Best: Experimental Effects of a Televised Soap Opera on Latino Attitudes Toward Government and Support for the 2010 US Census,” Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy 12, no. 1 (2012): 113–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

114 According to the narrative transportation theory, even though such characters and stories are obviously fictional, they can still feel real enough to the viewer to be persuasive. See: Green, Melanie and Brock, Timothy C., “The Role of Transportation in the Persuasiveness of Public Narratives,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 79, no. 5 (2000): 701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

115 Lam, David and Marteleto, Letícia, “Small Families and Large Cohorts: The Impact of the Demographic Transition on Schooling in Brazil,” The Changing Transitions to Adulthood in Developing Countries: Selected Studies (2005): 5683.Google Scholar

116 La Ferrara et al., “Soap Operas and Fertility: Evidence from Brazil.”

117 Ibid.

118 Ibid.

119 Ibid.

120 Ibid., 9.

121 Brown, “Sociocultural Influence of Predevelopment Soap Operas”; Tufte, Thomas, “Soap Operas and Sense-Making: Mediations and Audience Ethnography,” Entertainment–Education and Social Change: History, Research, and Practice (2004): 399415.Google Scholar

122 Brown, “Sociocultural Influence of Predevelopment Soap Operas.”

123 La Ferrara et al., “Soap Operas and Fertility: Evidence from Brazil.”

124 Brown, “Sociocultural Influence of Predevelopment Soap Operas”; Tufte, “Soap Operas and Sense Making.”

125 Trujillo and Paluck, “Experimental Effects of a Televised Soap Opera.”

126 Brown, “Sociocultural Influence of Predevelopment Soap Operas”; Arvind Singhal and Everett Rogers, “The Hum Log Story” (1989).

127 Jensen and Oster, “Cable Television and Women’s Status in India.”

128 Johnson, Kirk, “Media and Social Change: The Modernizing Influences of Television in Rural India,” Media, Culture and Society 23, no. 2 (2001): 147–69;CrossRefGoogle Scholar Scrase, Timothy, “Television, the Middle Classes and the Transformation of Cultural Identities in West Bengal, India,” International Communication Gazette 64, no. 4 (2002): 323–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

129 Jensen and Oster, “Cable Television and Women’s Status in India.”

130 Ibid., 1068.

131 Rogers et al., “Effects of an Entertainment-Education Radio Soap Opera.”

132 Ibid.; Slater, Michael, “Entertainment Education and the Persuasive Impact of Narratives,” in Green, Melanie, Strange, Jeffrey J., and Brock, Timothy C., eds. Narrative Impact: Social and Cognitive Foundations (New York: Taylor and Francis, 2003), 157–81.Google Scholar

133 Rogers et al., “Effects of an Entertainment-Education Radio Soap Opera.”

134 Ibid.

135 Bicchieri, Norms in the Wild.