Article contents
Human Nature and Grammar
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 12 April 2012
Extract
Seeing human nature through the prism of grammar may seem rather unusual. I will argue that this is a symptom for a problem – in both discussions of human nature and grammar: Neither the theory of grammar has properly placed its subject matter within the context of an inquiry into human nature and speciation, nor have discussions of human nature properly assessed the significance of grammar.
- Type
- Papers
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Royal Institute of Philosophy and the contributors 2012
References
1 Though see Roeper, T., The Prism of Grammar (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2009)Google Scholar.
2 For exceptions to this generalization, apart from Roeper ibid., see Hinzen, W., Mind Design and Minimal Syntax, (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar and Mukherji, N., The Primacy of Grammar (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
3 Sterelny, K. and Griffiths, P. E., 1999. Sex and Death (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1999)Google Scholar.
4 Hull, D., ‘On Human Nature’. In Hull, and Ruse, (eds.) The Philosophy of Biology (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1998)Google ScholarPubMed.
5 Ibid. 385.
6 Pinker, S., The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature (Harmondsworth: Penguin, 2003)Google Scholar.
7 Skinner, B. F., Verbal Behavior (New York: Appletonn-Century-Crofts, 1957)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.
8 Rorty, R., ‘The Priority of Democracy Over Philosophy. In Malachowski, A. R., (ed.) Reading Rorty (Oxford: Blackwell, 1990)Google Scholar.
9 K. Sterelny and P. E. Griffiths, 1999. Sex and Death, op. cit.
10 Griffiths, P. E. and Machery, E., ‘Innateness, Canalisation and “Biologizing the Mind’, Philosophical Psychology 21 (2008), 395–412CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
11 Evans, N., and Levinson, J., ‘The Myth of Language Universals: Language Diversity and its Importance for Cognitive Science’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (2009), 429–492CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed; Christiansen, M., and Chater, N. ‘Language as Shaped by the Brain’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (2008), 489–558CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Tomasello, M., Constructing a Language. (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 2005)Google Scholar.
12 Pinker, S., How the Mind Works. (New York: Norton, 1997)Google ScholarPubMed.
13 Pinker, S., The Language Instinct (Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1994)CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
14 For example, Christiansen and Chater, op. cit.
15 Op. cit.
16 Hull, D. and Ruse, M., (eds.) The Philosophy of Biology (Oxford: Oxford University Press,1998)Google ScholarPubMed.
17 Rosenberg, A., and Arp, R., Philosophy of Biology (Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell, 2010)Google Scholar.
18 Chomsky, N., Aspects of the Theory of Syntax (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1965)Google Scholar; Chomsky, N., Cartesian Linguistics, Third edition, ed. McGilvray, J. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1966/2009)Google Scholar; Lenneberg, E., Biological Foundations of Language, (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1967)Google ScholarPubMed; Piattelli-Palmarini, M., Uriagereka, J., and Salaburu, P. 2009. Of minds and language. Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar.
19 Chomsky, N., New Horizons in the Study of Language and Mind. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
20 Mayr, E., ‘Darwin's Influence on Modern Thought’, Scientific American (July 2000)Google Scholar, 80.
21 W. Hinzen, Mind Design and Minimalist Syntax, op. cit.
22 Gould, S. J., The Structure of Evolutionary Theory (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 2002)Google Scholar, ch. 4.
23 Newman, S. A., and Müller, G. B., G. B. 1999. ‘Morphological Evolution: Epigenetic Mechanisms’. In Embryonic Encyclopedia of Life Sciences. (London: Nature Publishing Group, 1999)Google Scholar. Available at: http://www.els.net; Webster, G. and Goodwin, B., Form and Transformation: Generative and Relational Principles in Biology (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1996)Google Scholar.
24 Darwin, C., On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection (Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1859/1968)Google Scholar, 206.
25 Gould, op. cit, ch. 4.
26 Amundson, R., ‘Two Concepts of Constraint’, Philosophy of Science 61 (1994), 556–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Amundson, R., ‘Typology reconsidered’, Biology and Philosophy 13 (1998) 153–77CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
27 Op. cit., 434.
28 S. A. Newman, and G. B. Müller, G. B. 1999. ‘Morphological Evolution: Epigenetic Mechanisms’, op. cit., note 23; Arthur, W., ‘The Emerging Conceptual Framework of Evolutionary Development Biology’, Nature 415 (14 Feb. 2002), 757–64CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
29 Denton, M. J., Dearden, P. K., and Sowerby, S. J., ‘Physical Law Not Natural Selection as the Major Determinant of Biological Complexity in the Subcellular Realm: New Support for the pre-Darwinian Conception of Evolution by Natural Law, BioSystems 71 (2003), 297–303CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed. See also Denton, M. J., ‘Laws of Form Revisited’, Nature 410 (22 March 2001)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed, 417.
30 Pennisi, E., ‘Evo-Devo Enthusiasts Get Down to Details’, Science 298 (2002), 953–5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.
31 W. Hinzen, Mind Design and Minimal Syntax, op.cit., ch. 3. Also see: Hinzen, W., ‘Spencerism and the Causal Theory of Reference’ Biology and Philosophy 21 (2006), 71–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar; and Hinzen, W., ‘The Philosophical Significance of Universal Grammar’, (University of Durham, Ms., 2011)Google Scholar.
32 See, for example: Pigliucci, M., and Müller, G.B. (eds.) 2010. Evolution: The Extended Synthesis (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Wagner, A., The Origins of Evolutionary Innovations (Oxford: Oxford University Press 2011)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed; Davidson, E. H., and Erwin, D. H. 2006. ‘Gene Regulatory Networks and the Evolution of Animal Body Plans’, Science 311 (10 February 2006), 796–800CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed; Sherman, M. Y., ‘Universal Genome in the Origin of Metazoa: Thoughts About Evolution’, Cell Cycle 6 (2007), 1873–1877CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed; and Bejan, A. and Marden, J. H.. ‘Unifying Constructal Theory for Scale Effects in Running, Swimming and Flying’, The Journal of Experimental Biology 209 (2006), 238–248CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.
33 Chomsky, N., Aspects of the Theory of Syntax (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1965)Google Scholar.
34 Chomsky, N., The Minimalist Program (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1995)Google Scholar, 11.
35 Tomaselli, S., S. 1995. ‘Human Nature’. In Yolton, J. et al. (eds.), The Blackwell Companion to the Enlightenment. (Oxford: Blackwell, 1995)Google Scholar, 229.
36 Hume, D., An Enquiry Concerning Human Understanding, ed. Selby-Bigge, L. A., 3rd edn. rev. by Nidditch, P. H. (Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1975)Google Scholar, section XI:144).
37 Quoted in Leibniz, G., Neue Abhandlungen über den menschlichen Verstand. (Hamburg: Meiner, 1704/1996)Google Scholar, 18, 23.
38 Yolton, J. W., Thinking Matter: Materialism in 18th-Century Britain (Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1983)Google Scholar.
39 Op.cit, 20.
40 N. Chomsky, New Horizons in the Study of Language and Mind, op. cit., 112–3.
41 Hume, D., An Enquiry Concerning Human Understanding, ed. Selby-Bigge, L. A., 3rd edn. rev. by Nidditch, P. H. (Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1975)Google Scholar, section V, part II, §44, 54.
42 Ibid., section VII, part I, §57, 72.
43 Hume, D., A Treatise of Human Nature, ed. Selby-Bigge, L. A., 2nd edn. (Oxford: Clarendon, 1978)Google Scholar, xvii.
44 See W. Hinzen, Mind Design and Minimalist Syntqx, op. cit., part 1.
45 See, for example, Carey, S., The Origins of Concepts (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2009)CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
46 Bejan, A. and Marden, J. H.. ‘Unifying Constructal Theory for Scale Effects in Running, Swimming and Flying’, The Journal of Experimental Biology 209 (2006), 238–248CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.
47 S. Tomaselli, ‘Human Nature’, op. cit, 232.
48 W. Hinzen, Minimalist Syntax, op.cit., 10.
49 Mikhail, J., Elements of Moral Cognition: Rawls' Linguistic Analogy and the Cognitive Science of Moral and Legal Judgment (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2010)Google Scholar.
50 S. Pinker, The Language Instinct, op. cit.
51 This generalization is crucially consistent with claims such as Everett's, D.L. (‘Cultural Constraints on Grammar and Cognition in Pirahã: Another Look at the Design Features of Human Language’, Current Anthropology 46 (2005) 621–46)CrossRefGoogle Scholar, to the effect that some languages have some grammatical constructions that do not exhibit formal properties that the same constructions exhibit in other languages.
52 Crow, T. J. (ed.), The Speciation of Modern Homo Sapiens (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
53 Dediu, D. and Ladd, D.L. (‘Linguistic Tone is Related to the Population Frequency of the Adaptive Haplogroups of Two Brain Size Genes, ASPM and Microcephalin’, PNAS 104 (2007), 10944–10949)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed present interesting evidence for a causal connection between genetic and linguistic diversity in regards to the acquisition of tonal contrasts in a language, suggesting some on-going gene-language co-evolution. Insofar as this co-evolution affects the phonological externalization of language rather than its syntax or semantics, it is perhaps quite expected, and it certainly does not disturb what has been called the ‘psychic unity of mankind’.
54 Tomasello, M., The Origins of Human Communication (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2008)Google Scholar, 249.
55 See Zuberbühler, K. (‘Linguistic Prerequisites in the Primate Lineage. In Tallerman, M. (ed.), Language Origins (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2005))Google Scholar for a discussion and careful qualifications of the claim that Campbell monkeys can process a combinatorial rule. Given relevant doubts even about the analogy between the relevant call and human words (Bickerton, D., Adam's Tongue (New York: Hill and Wang, 2009))Google Scholar, talk of a rule of ‘modification’ is tenuous. Semantic compositionality seems a long a way off.
56 Interestingly, not even in the case of so-called ‘indexicals’ such as ‘here’, ‘I’, or ‘now’. ‘I’, in particular, need not refer to the speaker, and ‘here’ need not refer to the place of utterance (Schlenker, P., ‘A Plea for Monsters’, Linguistics and Philosophy 26 (2003), 29–120)CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
57 Moro, A., The Boundaries of Babel (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2010)Google Scholar; W. Hinzen and D. Poeppel, ‘Semantics Between Cognitive Neuroscience and Linguistic Theory’, Journal of Cognitive Processes (in press).
58 Matilal, B. K., The Word and the World: India's Contribution to the Study of Language (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1991)Google Scholar.
59 Covington, M., Syntactic Theory in the High Middle Ages: Modistic Models of Sentence Structure (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2009)Google Scholar.
60 Arnauld, A. and Lancelot, E., Grammaire Générale et Raisonnée de Port-Royal. (Paris, 1660/1966)Google Scholar.
61 N. Chomsky, Aspects of the Theory of Syntax, op. cit.
62 Larson, R. K., Grammar as Science. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2010)Google Scholar.
63 Brock, W.H., The Fontana History of Chemistry (London: Fontana, 1992)Google Scholar.
64 Mellars, P., ‘The Impossible Coincidence: A Single-Species Model for the Origins of Modern Human Behavior in Europe’, Evol. Anthropo 14 (2005), 12–276CrossRefGoogle Scholar; Klein, R. and Edgar, B., The Dawn of Human Culture, (New York: John Wiley and Sons, 2002)Google Scholar.
65 Henshilwood, C. S., ‘Modern Humans and Symbolic Behaviour: Evidence from Blombos Cave, South Africa’. In Blundell, G. (ed.) Origins (Cape Town: Double Storey, 2006)Google ScholarPubMed.
66 T. J. Crow (ed.), The Speciation of Modern Homo Sapiens, op. cit.
67 This is not to say that Neanderthal did not vocalize, which is a different issue (for some evidence and discussion in regards to the FOXP2 gene see Green, R. E., et al. (‘A Draft Sequence of the Neandertal Genome’, Science 328 (2010), 710–722)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed; Mithen, S., The Singing Neanderthals (London:Phoenix, 2006)Google Scholar.
68 Tattersall, I., ‘A Putative Role for Language in the Origin of Human Consciousness’. In Larson, R. et al. (eds.), The Evolution of Human Language (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2010)Google Scholar.
69 M. Tomasello, The Origins of Human Communication, op. cit., 321.
70 Fitch, W. T., Language Evolution (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2010)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed, section 4.11.
71 Marler, P., 2000. ‘Origins of Music and Speech’. In Wallin, N., Merker, B., and Brown, S., (eds.), The Origins of Music, (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2000)Google Scholar.
72 S. Mithen, The Singing Neanderthals, op. cit.
73 N. Mukherji, The Primacy of Grammar,op.cit.
74 D. Bickerton, Adam's Tongue, op. cit.
75 N. Mukherji, The Primacy of Grammar, op. cit., 189.
76 N. Chomsky, The Minimalist Program,op.cit.
77 J. Katz, and D. Pesetsky ‘The Recursive Syntax and Prosody of Tonal Music’ (Ms., MIT, 2009).
78 W.H. Brock, The Fontana History of Chemistry, op. cit.
79 Gallistel, C. R., ‘The Foundational Abstractions’. In Piattelli-Palmarini, M., Uriagereka, J., and Salaburu, P. (eds.), Of Minds and Language. (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2009)Google Scholar.
80 N. Mukherji, The Primacy of Grammar, op. cit., ch. 7.
81 Descartes, R., Discours de la Méthode (Paris: Vrin, 1637/1984)Google Scholar.
82 Crow, T. J., ‘The “Big Bang” Theory of the Origin of Psychosis and the Faculty of Language’, Schizophrenia Research 102 (2008), 31–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
83 T. J. Crow (ed.), The Speciation of Modern Homo Sapiens, op.cit.
84 N. Chomsky, New Horizons in the Study of Language and Mind,op. cit.
85 Cf. Ibid., ch. 5.
86 N. Chomsky, Cartesian Lingusitics, op. cit.
87 A. Arnauld and E. Lancelot, Grammaire Générale et Raisonnée de Port-Royal, op.cit.
88 N. Chomsky, The Minimalist Program, op.cit.
89 As re-stated in a recent editorial of the journal Lingua, the autonomy of grammar consists in the following claim: ‘phonological and semantic rules can refer to syntactic information, but syntactic rules cannot refer to phonological or semantic information’ (J. Rooryck, N. Smith, A. Liptak, and D. Blakemore (eds.). ‘Editorial Introduction to Special Issue on Evans and Levinson's “The Myth of Language Universals”’, Lingua 120, 2655).
90 Haegeman, L., Introduction to Government and Binding Theory (Oxford: Blackwell, 1994)Google Scholar.
91 N. Chomsky, New Horizons in the Study of Language and Mind, op. cit.
92 N. Chomsky, The Minimalist Program, op. cit.
93 Boeckx, C., Bare Syntax (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2008)Google Scholar.
94 Chomsky, N., ‘Approaching UG from Below’. In Sauerland, U. and Gärtner, H.-M., (eds.) Interfaces + Recursion = Language? Chomsky's Minimalism and the view from syntax-semantics, (Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter, 2008)Google Scholar.
95 W. Hinzen, ‘Minimalism’. In T. Fernando and R. Kempson, (eds.) Handbook of Philosophy of Linguistics (Elsevier, in press).
96 Davidson, D., ‘Rational Animals’, Dialectica 36 (1982) 317–327CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
97 D. Bickerton, Adam's Tongue, op.cit.
98 Terrace, H., ‘Metacognition and the Evolution of Language’, in Terrace, H. and Metcalfe, P. (eds.), The Missing Link in Cognition (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2005)Google Scholar; Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., and Povinelli, D. J., ‘Darwin's Mistake: Explaining the Discontinuity Between Human and Nonhuman Minds’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (2008), 109–130CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed.
99 Leiss, E., Sprachphilosophie. (Berlin:De Gruyter, 2009)CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
100 R. Jackendoff, Foundations of Language, op. cit.; M. Tomasello, The Origins of Human Communication, op. cit.
101 M. Christiansen, and N. Chater 2008 ‘Language as Shaped by the Brain’, op. cit., 501.
102 D. Davidson, ‘Rational Animals’,op. cit.
103 W.Hinzen, Mind Design and Minimal Syntax, op. cit.; Hinzen, W., An Essay on Naming and Truth (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2007)Google Scholar.
104 Pietroski, P., ‘Meaning Before Truth’. In Peter, G. and Preyer, G. (eds.), Contextualism in Philosophy, (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2005)Google Scholar.
105 P. Pietroski, P. 2011.‘Minimal Semantic Instructions’. In Boeckx, C. (ed.). The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Minimalism (Oxford:Oxford University Press, 2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
106 Graffi, G., 200 years of syntax. (Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 2001)CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
107 S. Pinker, The Language Instinct, op. cit.
108 R. Jackendoff, Foundations of language. op. cit.; N. Evans, and J. Levinson, ‘The Myth of Language Universals: Language Diversity and its Importance for Cognitive Science’, op. cit.; M. Christiansen, and N. Chater ‘Language as Shaped by the Brain’, op. cit.; M. Tomasello, The Origins of Human Communication, op. cit.
109 W. Hinzen, Mind Design and Minimal Syntax, op. cit.
110 N. Chomsky, New Horizons in the Study of Language and Mind, op. cit., 27.
111 Hinzen, W., ‘The Philosophical Significance of Universal Grammar’, (Ms., University of Durham, 2011)Google Scholar.
112 Hinzen, W. and Sheehan, M., ‘Moving Towards the Edge’, Linguistic Analysis, 2011(in press).Google Scholar
113 Longobardi, G., G. 1994. ‘Reference and Proper Names’, Linguistic Inquiry 25 (1994), 609–665Google Scholar; Longobardi, G., ‘Towards a Unified Grammar of Reference’, Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft 24 (2005), 5–44CrossRefGoogle Scholar.
114 The connection between the fragmentation of grammar and the breakdown of the deictic frame (and hence human reason) in Schizophrenia argued for by Crow (T. J. Crow, ‘The “Big Bang” Theory of the Origin of Psychosis and the Faculty of Language’, op. cit.) is a powerful further support for this conclusion, which came up during the Human Nature conference and which unfortunately I cannot go into here for reasons of space.
115 This research was made possible by the grant ‘Un-Cartesian Linguistics’ (AHRC/DFG, AH/H50009X/1). I thank my co-workers Uli Reichard and Michelle Sheehan for numerous conversations on the issues of this paper, and the organizers of the Human Nature conference for stimulating such useful discussions.
- 1
- Cited by