Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-5r2nc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-08T10:14:13.649Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

R. Duncan Luce, Robert R. Bush, and Eugene Galanter (Eds.) Handbook of Mathematical Psychology. Vol. I, pp. 491 + xiii; Vol. II, pp. 666 + vii. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1963.

Review products

R. Duncan Luce, Robert R. Bush, and Eugene Galanter (Eds.) Handbook of Mathematical Psychology. Vol. I, pp. 491 + xiii; Vol. II, pp. 666 + vii. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1963.

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2025

E. C. Carterette*
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Book Review
Copyright
Copyright © 1965 Psychometric Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bartlett, M.S. Stochastic processes, Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press, 1955.Google Scholar
Berge, C. The theory of graphs and its applications, New York: Wiley, 1962.Google Scholar
Bush, R. R. and Estes, W. K. Studies in mathematical learning theory, Stanford: Stanford Univ. Press, 1959.Google Scholar
Bush, R. R. and Mosteller, F. Stochastic models for learning, New York: Wiley, 1955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. Syntactic structures, The Hague: Mouton, 1957.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, F. R. Constant-ratio rule for confusion matrices in speech communication. J. acoust. Soc. Amer., 1959, 31, 629630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coombs, C. H. A theory of data, New York: Wiley, 1964.Google Scholar
Estes, W. K. Toward a statistical theory of learning. Psychol. Rev., 1950, 57, 94107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamming, R. W. Numerical methods for scientists and engineers, New York: McGraw-Hill, 1962.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R., Fant, G. C. M., and Halle, M. Preliminaries to speech analysis, Cambridge: MIT Press, 1951.Google Scholar
Lenneberg, E. H. Speech development: its anatomical and physiological concomitants. In Carterette, E. C. (Ed.), Speech, language and communication. Berkeley: Univ. California Press. (In press).Google Scholar
Luce, R. D. Individual choice behavior, New York: Wiley, 1959.Google Scholar
McGill, W. J. and Gibbon, J. The general-gamma distribution and reaction times, Stanford: Stanford Institute for Mathematical Studies in the Social Sciences, 1963.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. Mathematics and psychology, New York: Wiley, 1964.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. The psycholinguists. Encounter, 1964, 22, 2937.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. Some psychological studies of grammar. Amer. Psychologist, 1962, 17, 748762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E. H., and Pribram, K. Plans and the structure of behavior, New York: Holt, 1960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfanzagl, J. A general theory of measurement: Applications to utility. Naval Res. Logistics Quart., 1959, 6, 283294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reitman, W. Information-processing models in psychology. Science, 1964, 144, 11921198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, S. S. Mathematics, measurement and psychophysics. In Stevens, S. S. (Eds.), Handbook of experimental psychology. New York: Wiley, 1951, 149.Google Scholar
Stone, M. Models for choice reaction time. Psychometrika, 1960, 25, 251260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swets, J. A. Signal detection and recognition by human observers, New York: Wiley, 1964.Google Scholar
Thrall, R. M., Coombs, C. H., and Davis, R. L. Decision processes, New York: Wiley, 1954.Google Scholar
Wald, A. Sequential analysis, New York: Wiley, 1947.Google Scholar