Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-mzp66 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-08T09:20:23.908Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Irrelevance of Distributional Assumptions on Reaction Times in Multidimensional Scaling of Same/Different Judgment Tasks

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2025

Gert Storms*
Affiliation:
Katholieke University Leuven, Belgium
Luc Delbeke
Affiliation:
Katholieke University Leuven, Belgium
*
Requests for reprints should be sent to Gert Storms, University of Leuven, Tiensestraat 102, B-3000 Leuven, BELGIUM.

Abstract

Takane and Sergent developed a model (MAXRT) for scaling same/different judgments and response times (RTs) simultaneously. The model assumes that RTs are distributed lognormally. Our experiment showed that the RT distribution of the judgments might be task dependent. It is shown that lognormal RTs provide a far better fit than exponential, normal, and Pareto distributed RTs (with the same means and variances), but that the final parameter estimates from the data set with lognormal RTs hardly differ from the alternatively distributed RTs. Finally, despite the robustness of the distributional assumption of the RTs with respect to the parameter estimates, it is shown that RTs have an informational value that is not contained in the same/different judgments alone.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 1992 The Psychometric Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

The authors thank Jan Beirlant, Geert De Soete, Yoshio Takane, the three reviewers, and the Editor, for their useful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript. We are also indebted to Noël Bovens for his help in programming the experiments.

References

Akaike, H. (1974). A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control, 19, 716723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. R., Bower, G. H. (1973). Human associative memory, Washington: Winston & Sons.Google Scholar
Behrman, B. W., Brown, D. R. (1968). Multidimensional scaling of form: A psychophysical analysis. Perception & Psychophysics, 4, 1925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, D. R., Andrews, M. H. (1968). Visual form discrimination: Multidimensional analysis. Perception & Psychophysics, 3, 401406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chocholle, R. (1940). Variations des temps de réaction auditifs en fonction de l'intensité à diverses fréquences [Variations of auditory reaction times as a function of intensity at varying frequences]. L'Année Psychologique, 41, 65124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D'Agostino, R., Stephens, M. A. (1986). Goodness-of-fit techniques, New-York: Dekker.Google Scholar
De Soete, G. (1986). A perceptual study of the Flury-Riedwyl faces for graphically displaying multivariate data. International Journal for Man-Machine Studies, 25, 549555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Soete, G., Carroll, J. D. (1983). A maximum likelihood method for fitting the wandering vector model. Psychometrika, 48, 553566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flury, B., Riedwyl, H. (1981). Graphical representation of multivariate data by means of asymmetrical faces. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 76, 757765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flury, B., & Riedwyl, H. (1983). Some applications of asymmetrical faces. Unpublished manuscript, Institut für Mathematische Statistik und Versicherungslehre, Universitaet Bern.Google Scholar
Green, D. M., Luce, R. D. (1967). Detection of auditory signals presented at random times. Perception & Psychophysics, 2, 441449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. M., Luce, R. D. (1971). Detection of auditory signals presented at random times III. Perception & Psychophysics, 9, 257268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrington, L. F., Brown, D. R. (1972). Visual pattern perception: Multidimensional analysis of discrimination difficulty for children. Psychonomic Science, 29, 219221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ida, M. (1980). The application of the Weibull distribution to the analysis of the reaction time data. Japanese Psychological Review, 22, 207212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, N. L., Kotz, S. (1970). Distributions in statistics: Continuous univariate distributions-2, Boston: Houghton Miffin.Google Scholar
Kak, A. V., Brown, D. R. (1979). Visual pattern perception: Multidimensional analysis of development of children's reading skills. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 49, 819830.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirk, R. E. (1982). Experimental design. Procedures for the behavioral sciences 2nd ed.,, Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
Kohfeld, D. L., Santee, J. L., Wallace, N. D. (1981). Loudness and reaction time: II. Identification of detection components at different intensities and frequencies. Perception & Psychophysics, 2, 550562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruskal, J. B. (1964). Multidimensional scaling by optimizing goodness of fit to a nonmetric hypothesis. Psychometrika, 29, 127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruskal, J. B. (1964). Multidimensional scaling: A numerical method. Psychometrika, 29, 115129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luce, R. D. (1986). Response times. Their role in inferring elementary mental organization, New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Luce, R. D., Green, D. M. (1970). Detection of auditory signals presented at random times II. Perception & Psychophysics, 7, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGill, W. J., Gibbon, J. (1965). The general gamma distribution and reaction times. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 2, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Medin, D. L., Schaffer, M. M. (1978). Context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review, 85, 207238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melara, R. D. (1989). Similarity relations among synesthetic stimuli and their attributes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 212231.Google ScholarPubMed
Murdock, B. B. (1985). An analysis of the strength-latency relationship. Memory and Cognition, 13, 511521.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearson, E. S., D'Agostino, R. B., Bowman, K. O. (1977). Tests for departure from normality: Comparison of powers. Biometrika, 64, 231246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Podgorny, P., Garner, W. R. (1979). Reaction time as a measure of inter and intra-object visual similarity. Perception & Psychophysics, 26, 3752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ratcliff, R. (1978). Theory of memory retrieval. Psychological Review, 85, 59108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ratcliff, R., Murdock, B. B. (1976). Retrieval processes in recognition memory. Psychological Review, 86, 190214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Royston, J. P. (1982). An extension of Shapiro and Wilk's W test for normality to large samples. Applied Statistics, 31, 115124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheiblechner, H. (1979). Specifically objective stochastic latency mechanisms. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 19, 1838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sergent, J., Takane, Y. (1987). Structures in two-choice reaction-time data. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 300315.Google Scholar
Shapiro, S. S., Wilk, M. B. (1965). An analysis of variance test for normality (complete samples). Biometrika, 52, 591611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shrout, P. E., Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 420428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snodgrass, J. G., Luce, R. D., Galanter, E. (1967). Some experiments on simple and choice reaction times. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75, 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takane, Y., Sergent, J. (1983). Multidimensional scaling models for reaction times and same-different judgments. Psychometrika, 48, 393423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Townsend, J. T., Ashby, F. G. (1983). The stochastic modeling of elementary psychological processes, Cambridge: University Press.Google Scholar
Tversky, A. (1977). Features of similarity. Psychological Review, 84, 327352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, F. W. (1970). Nonmetric scaling for line length using latencies, similarity, and same-different judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 8, 363369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar