Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T06:12:03.783Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Youth adversity and trajectories of depression/anxiety symptoms in adolescence in the context of intersectionality in the United Kingdom

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 April 2024

Laura Havers*
Affiliation:
Centre for Psychiatry and Mental Health, Wolfson Institute of Population Health, Queen Mary, University of London, London, UK
Ruichong Shuai
Affiliation:
Centre for Psychiatry and Mental Health, Wolfson Institute of Population Health, Queen Mary, University of London, London, UK
Peter Fonagy
Affiliation:
Anna Freud National Centre for Children and Families, London, UK Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK
Mina Fazel
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Craig Morgan
Affiliation:
Health Service and Population Research, Institute of Psychology, Psychiatry & Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK ESRC Centre for Society and Mental Health, King's College London, London, UK
Daisy Fancourt
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioural Science and Health, University College London, London, UK
Paul McCrone
Affiliation:
Institute for Lifecourse Development, University of Greenwich, London, UK
Melanie Smuk
Affiliation:
Centre for Genomics and Child Health, Blizard Institute, Queen Mary, University of London, London, UK
Kamaldeep Bhui
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Nuffield Department of Primary Care Health Sciences, and Wadham College, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Health, Oxford and East London NHS Foundation Trusts, London, UK World Psychiatric Association Collaborating Centre, Oxford, UK
Sania Shakoor*
Affiliation:
Centre for Psychiatry and Mental Health, Wolfson Institute of Population Health, Queen Mary, University of London, London, UK
Georgina M. Hosang
Affiliation:
Centre for Psychiatry and Mental Health, Wolfson Institute of Population Health, Queen Mary, University of London, London, UK
*
Corresponding authors: Laura Havers; Email: [email protected]; Sania Shakoor; Email: [email protected]
Corresponding authors: Laura Havers; Email: [email protected]; Sania Shakoor; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Youth adversity is associated with persistence of depression and anxiety symptoms. This association may be greater for disadvantaged societal groups (such as females) compared with advantaged groups (e.g. males). Given that persistent symptoms are observed across a range of disadvantaged, minoritized, and neurodivergent groups (e.g. low compared with high socio-economic status [SES]), the intersection of individual characteristics may be an important moderator of inequality.

Methods

Data from HeadStart Cornwall (N = 4441) was used to assess the effect of youth adversity on combined symptoms of depression and anxiety (Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire emotional problems subscale) measured at three time-points in 11–14-year-olds. Latent trajectories and regressions were estimated for eight intersectionality profiles (based on gender, SES, and hyperactivity/inattention), and moderating effects of the individual characteristics and their intersections were estimated.

Results

Youth adversity was associated with higher average depression/anxiety symptoms at baseline (11–12-years) across all intersectionality profiles. The magnitude of effects differed across profiles, with suggestive evidence for a moderating effect of youth adversity on change over time in depression/anxiety symptoms attributable to the intersection between (i) gender and SES; and (ii) gender, SES, and hyperactivity/inattention.

Conclusions

The detrimental effects of youth adversity pervade across intersectionality profiles. The extent to which these effects are moderated by intersectionality is discussed in terms of operational factors. The current results provide a platform for further research, which is needed to determine the importance of intersectionality as a moderator of youth adversity on the development of depression and anxiety symptoms in adolescence.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Symptoms of depression (e.g. low mood and loss of enjoyment) and anxiety (e.g. nervousness and worry) are among the most common mental health problems in adolescence (Michaud & Fombonne, Reference Michaud and Fombonne2005). A global prevalence rate of ~35% has been estimated for elevated depressive symptoms (Shorey, Ng, & Wong, Reference Shorey, Ng and Wong2022) and ~10% for elevated anxiety symptoms (Biswas et al., Reference Biswas, Scott, Munir, Renzaho, Rawal, Baxter and Mamun2020). Experiencing persistently high or increasing symptoms of depression and/or anxiety across adolescence is associated with a range of negative correlates and outcomes, such as substance use and school dropout (Morin et al., Reference Morin, Maïano, Nagengast, Marsh, Morizot and Janosz2011; Schubert, Clark, Van, Collinson, & Baune, Reference Schubert, Clark, Van, Collinson and Baune2017). Importantly, some groups in society are more likely to experience a trajectory of elevated (i.e. high, or increasing) depression and/or anxiety symptoms (Musliner, Munk-Olsen, Eaton, & Zandi, Reference Musliner, Munk-Olsen, Eaton and Zandi2016). These include individuals with neurodivergent conditions (e.g. attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [ADHD]) compared with neurotypical individuals, those from low compared with high socio-economic status [SES] backgrounds, and females compared with males (Leban, Reference Leban2021; Schubert et al., Reference Schubert, Clark, Van, Collinson and Baune2017). The underlying mechanisms linking these characteristics with trajectories of elevated depression and/or anxiety are likely to be both complex, and diverse (e.g. Blakemore, Burnett, & Dahl, Reference Blakemore, Burnett and Dahl2010; Udry, Reference Udry2000). Importantly, however, in the context of the current study – these findings collectively suggest that minoritized, disadvantaged, and neurodivergent groups, are at heightened risk of experiencing trajectories of elevated depression and anxiety symptoms compared to their counterparts.

Youth adversity and adolescent depression/anxiety symptoms

In addition to risk conferred by individual-level and demographic characteristics, research consistently shows an association between youth adversity and trajectories of elevated depression and/or anxiety symptoms (Bevilacqua, Kelly, Heilmann, Priest, & Lacey, Reference Bevilacqua, Kelly, Heilmann, Priest and Lacey2021; Desch, Mansuri, Tran, Schwartz, & Bakour, Reference Desch, Mansuri, Tran, Schwartz and Bakour2023; Leban, Reference Leban2021). Youth adversity encompasses stressful and potentially traumatic experiences that occur in the home (e.g. abuse and parental separation), often referred to as adverse childhood experiences [ACEs] (Felitti et al., Reference Felitti, Anda, Nordenberg, Williamson, Spitz, Edwards and Marks1998; Kalmakis & Chandler, Reference Kalmakis and Chandler2014), as well as outside the home (e.g. bullying-victimization). Youth adversity is more prevalent in disadvantaged, minoritized, and neurodivergent groups – which may contribute to why trajectories of elevated depression/anxiety symptoms are more common in these groups (Assini-Meytin, Fix, Green, Nair, & Letourneau, Reference Assini-Meytin, Fix, Green, Nair and Letourneau2022; Craig, Bondi, O'Donnell, Pepler, & Weiss, Reference Craig, Bondi, O'Donnell, Pepler and Weiss2020; Walsh, McCartney, Smith, & Armour, Reference Walsh, McCartney, Smith and Armour2019).

While several studies have investigated the association between youth adversity and trajectories of emotional problems (encompassing depression/anxiety symptoms) in youth (Musliner et al., Reference Musliner, Munk-Olsen, Eaton and Zandi2016), few have examined whether the association is moderated by (dependent on) individual characteristics, such as gender. In these studies, moderation attributable to gender was not found, suggesting that the impact of youth adversity on the development of emotional problems is similar for females and males (Bevilacqua et al., Reference Bevilacqua, Kelly, Heilmann, Priest and Lacey2021; Leban, Reference Leban2021). However, an individual can hold multiple forms of minority or disadvantaged status (e.g. being a neurodivergent female from a low SES background), which may lead to greater vulnerability to mental health problems in the face of adversity (Ghavami, Katsiaficas, & Rogers, Reference Ghavami, Katsiaficas, Rogers, Horn, Ruck and Liben2016) – although this has yet to be empirically investigated.

Intersectionality

The broad analytic framework of intersectionality posits that the intersection (reflecting a multiplicative interaction) of an individual's characteristics (e.g. gender, ethnicity, and SES) has importance beyond their additive effects (Bowleg, Reference Bowleg2012; Crenshaw, Reference Crenshaw1990). Intersectional identities are considered to reflect social positions within a complex system of societal and social hierarchies, in the context of power, oppression, and privilege (Crenshaw, Reference Crenshaw1990). Investigating the extent to which the effects of youth adversity on the development of depression/anxiety symptoms are dependent on intersections of individual characteristics, reflective of exclusion and marginalization, may offer a more comprehensive account of the conditions under which adversity is associated with the development of poor mental health.

Current study

To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the association between youth adversity and trajectories of depression/anxiety symptoms in adolescence in the context of intersectionality. This study will address two main research questions:

  1. (1) Is youth adversity associated with baseline and change over time in depression/anxiety symptoms for all intersectionality profiles? It was hypothesized that youth adversity, compared to an absence of youth adversity, would be associated with higher baseline depression/anxiety symptoms that would remain higher, for all intersectionality profiles.

  2. (2) (i) Does the association between youth adversity and depression/anxiety symptoms (at baseline, and their change over time) differ across intersectionality profiles; and if so, (ii) to what extent is the association moderated by gender, SES, and hyperactivity/inattention, and their intersections? It was hypothesized that: (i) the association between youth adversity and depression/anxiety symptoms (at baseline and their change over time) would differ across intersectionality profiles; and (ii) these associations would be moderated by gender, SES, and hyperactivity/inattention, and their intersections. No predictions were made regarding the direction of these moderating effects.

Methods

Participants

A total of 5336 individuals aged 11- to 14-years-old were included in the initial sample of the current investigation (online Supplementary Table S1), drawn from the HeadStart Cornwall study (Deighton et al., Reference Deighton, Lereya, Casey, Patalay, Humphrey and Wolpert2019; Hosang et al., Reference Hosang, Havers, Shuai, Fonagy, Fazel, Morgan and Shakoor2023). In this study, pupils from all 31 state-maintained secondary schools in Cornwall, United Kingdom (UK), were invited to take part in 2017 when they were in school Year 7 (age 11–12-years, N = 4575) and were followed up annually in Year 8 (age 12–13-years, N = 4600) and Year 9 (13–14-years, N = 3604) (see Deighton et al., Reference Deighton, Lereya, Casey, Patalay, Humphrey and Wolpert2019 for a detailed description of HeadStart). School identification data and age data were not available for this sample (reported ages reflect UK averages). Parental consent was assumed unless parents opted their child out. Pupils assented prior to online participation at their school. Ethical approval for HeadStart was obtained from the University College London Ethics Committee (ref: 8097/003). The final sample for analysis was N = 4441 individuals with youth adversity and intersectionality profile data, detailed below.

Measures

Combined depression and anxiety symptoms were measured at each school year using the five-item emotional problems subscale of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) (Goodman, Reference Goodman1997). Items are worded to reflect usual experiences over the past six months (e.g. ‘I am often unhappy’) and are self-rated on a 3-point scale (‘Not true’, ‘Somewhat true’, and ‘Certainly true’). A prorated total score of combined depression/anxiety symptoms (0–10) was calculated where at least three items had response data, which was true for all observations at each school year.

Youth adversity was measured in two ways. First, using the bullying-victimization item from the SDQ peer problems subscale: ‘Other children or young people pick on me or bully me’ (Goodman, Reference Goodman1997). Second, using data collected from the local government Supporting Families program records (https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/supporting-families-programme-guidance-2022-to-2025). In this program, families were identified as ‘On Family List’ if they experienced any of the adversities listed in Supplementary Materials 1 (examples include homelessness and household domestic violence). Because data regarding the number (and type) of adversity was not available for this sample, a binary variable was created to indicate presence/absence of either bullying-victimization (in any school year) and/or local government recorded adversity (Supplementary Materials 1a). It was not possible to elucidate the timing of the local government-reported youth adversities because this information is updated yearly (overwriting any previous records), and only the most recent records are available.

Gender and SES data were drawn from School Census records. Gender was recorded as ‘female’ or ‘male’, and receipt/non-receipt of free school meals was used to index ‘lower SES’ and ‘higher SES’, respectively. Hyperactivity/inattention was measured at baseline using the five items of the self-rated SDQ hyperactivity/inattention subscale (Goodman, Reference Goodman1997). Individuals were classified as ‘low’ with scores of 0–6, and ‘high’ with scores above 6, in line with scoring recommendations (Goodman, Reference Goodman1997). Intersectionality profiles were created based on the combination of gender, SES, and hyperactivity/inattention. Individuals were assigned to one of eight intersectionality profiles (e.g. male from lower SES background and high hyperactivity/inattention).

Statistical analyses

To address research question one, trajectories of depression/anxiety symptoms were estimated by specifying a latent growth model within a structural equation modeling framework (Supplementary Materials 2). Depression/anxiety symptoms at each school year were modeled as observed variables, specified as indicators of a latent intercept factor and a latent slope factor. The latent intercept was positioned at school Year 7 (age 11–12-years), reflecting estimated initial/baseline levels. The latent slope reflects the annual rate of change in symptoms across school Years 7–9 (herein interchangeably referred to as change over time). School year-specific (residual) variances were freely estimated.

An unconditional model was first run, followed by a conditional model, with the latent growth factors regressed on youth adversity. A multiple group conditional model was then run, where parameters were freely estimated for each intersectionality profile (Supplementary Materials 3: Mplus script; online Supplementary Fig. S1: path diagram). A schematic diagram of the final model is shown in Fig. 1. Incremental better fit of these models would figuratively indicate that trajectories of depression/anxiety symptoms vary with the presence/absence of youth adversity, and that at an omnibus level, the relations between youth adversity and the latent growth factors vary across intersectionality profiles.

Figure 1. Schematic diagram of the multiple group conditional latent growth model.

Note. SES: socio-economic status. Intersectional profile (depicted at the center of the Venn diagram, above left) is used as the grouping variable in a multiple group model, where everything inside of the box (above right) is estimated for each intersectionality profile group. Compound parameters are further estimated, reflecting the extent to which the individual characteristics, as well as the intersections between them (i.e. the shaded areas of the Venn diagram), moderate the paths from youth adversity to the latent growth factors. The observed depression/anxiety symptoms scores are indicators of the latent growth factors. A non-schematic, labeled path diagram is shown in online Supplementary Fig. 1.

Model fit was assessed using the comparative fit index (CFI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). CFI >0.95, RMSEA <0.08, and SRMR <0.06 were broadly considered indicative of acceptable fit (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999; Marsh, Hau, & Wen, Reference Marsh, Hau and Wen2004). Bayesian information criterion (BIC) was also used, with lower values indicative of better relative fit. Depression/anxiety symptoms were treated as continuous data. Full information maximum likelihood estimation was used to accommodate missing data across school years under the assumption that data was missing at random. Robust estimation was used to accommodate multivariate nonnormality of residuals, with adjustment to s.e. and test statistics. As a sensitivity analysis, the final analysis model was rerun with multiply imputed youth adversity data from 10 datasets.

To address research question two, compound parameters of the maximum likelihood regression estimates from the final analysis model (above) were estimated; specified to approximate the extent to which the effect of youth adversity on the latent growth factors is moderated by the individual characteristics (gender, SES, and hyperactivity/inattention), and by interactions between these characteristics (reflecting intersectional effects, e.g. gender by SES). Using this approach, the derived compound parameters reflect the extent to which the regression estimates from the final analysis model are moderated by the individual characteristics and their intersections. Standard errors and confidence intervals of the compound parameters were estimated from 1000 bootstrapped draws.

Prior to the main analyses detailed above, the five depression/anxiety items were specified as indicators of a common latent factor in a confirmatory factor analysis, and measurement properties of the model were assessed. This provides model-based information regarding the extent to which modeling depression and anxiety as a unitary construct provides an adequate representation of the sample data, although testing models with more than one factor was beyond the scope of the current study. Measurement invariance of depression/anxiety across school years, and across intersectionality profile groups at each time-point, was assessed to provide model-based information regarding the extent to which the measurement of depression/anxiety is sufficiently equivalent across time, and across groups, respectively (van de Schoot, Lugtig, & Hox, Reference van de Schoot, Lugtig and Hox2012) (Supplementary Materials 4). Model specifications recommended for ordered categorical data were used (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Millsap, West, Tein, Tanaka and Grimm2017; Wu et al., Reference Svetina, Rutkowski and Rutkowski2020). For the models described in this paragraph, diagonally weighted least squares estimation was used, using pairwise present data.

Descriptive statistics were calculated using R (version 4.2). Structural equation modeling was conducted using Mplus (version 8.8).

Results

A description of the sample is presented in Table 1. Mean depression/anxiety symptoms were 3.82 (s.d. 2.54) at age 11–12-years, 3.89 (s.d. 2.64) at age 12–13-years, and 4.32 (s.d. 2.75) at age 13–14-years. A total of 52.94% of individuals experienced youth adversity across the study period (also see Supplementary Materials 1a). Descriptive results for youth adversity and depression/anxiety symptoms for each intersectionality profile are reported in Table 2. At each age, proportions experiencing youth adversity and mean levels of depression/anxiety symptoms were highest for the intersectionality profile of female, lower SES, and high hyperactivity/inattention. Depression/anxiety symptoms were lowest for the intersectionality profile of male, higher SES, and low hyperactivity/inattention. The rate of youth adversity was lowest for the intersectionality profile of female, higher SES, and low hyperactivity/inattention.

Table 1. Sample description

N, number of individuals; SES, socio-economic status; IQR, interquartile range.

Table 2. Youth adversity and depression/anxiety symptoms by intersectionality profiles

N, number of individuals; SES, socio-economic status.

a N = 4448 with intersectionality profile data.

b Percentage of individuals in intersectionality profile with youth adversity data across the study period (N = 4441).

c Individuals in intersectionality profile with depression/anxiety symptoms data at each time-point.

The depression and anxiety items were adequately represented by a common factor model at each school year. Model fit statistics and reliability estimates (ω = 0.80–0.85) derived from the models are reported in online Supplementary Table S2. Evidence in support of measurement invariance at the scalar level (a model with constrained thresholds and loadings) was observed longitudinally, and across groups at each time-point (online Supplementary Tables S3 and S3a, respectively).

Is youth adversity associated with baseline and change over time in depression/anxiety symptoms for all intersectionality profiles?

The fit of the conditional multiple group model was acceptable (online Supplementary Table S4). For all intersectionality profiles, the presence of youth adversity compared to an absence of such, was associated with higher mean depression/anxiety symptoms at baseline (age 11–12-years) (online Supplementary Table S5). Youth adversity was only negligibly associated with a different rate of change over time in these symptoms, except for in two profiles. Specifically in these profiles (detailed below), there was weak evidence to suggest that youth adversity, compared to an absence of youth adversity, was associated with a decrease in change over time for the male, lower SES, high hyperactivity/inattention profile, and with an increase in change over time for the female, lower SES, and high hyperactivity/inattention profile. Figure 2 shows the estimated average latent trajectories for each intersectionality profile in the presence of youth adversity, compared to an absence of youth adversity. To aid interpretation, visually – the effect of youth adversity on baseline symptoms can be considered as the distance between the dashed and solid lines at school Year 7, within each panel. The effect of youth adversity on the rate of change over time can be considered as the degree to which the dashed and solid lines are non-parallel, within each panel.

Figure 2. Effect of youth adversity on average depression/anxiety symptoms trajectories by intersectionality profiles.

Note. SES: socio-economic status. Y axis: depression/anxiety symptoms total observed score. School year corresponds to the following average ages: 11–12-years (Year 7), 12–13-years (Year 8), 13–14-years (Year 9).

Does the association between youth adversity and depression/anxiety symptoms (at baseline and their change over time) differ across intersectionality profiles?

The conditional multiple group model showed an improvement in terms of BIC compared to the single group models (online Supplementary Table S4). This provides model-based information to suggest that the association between youth adversity and the latent growth factors differs across intersectionality profiles at an omnibus level.

Parameter estimates of the conditional multiple group model are reported in online Supplementary Tables S5 and S6. The effect of youth adversity on baseline symptoms was highest for the male, lower SES, high hyperactivity/inattention intersectionality profile (b = 2.318, s.e. = 0.521), and was lowest for the female, low SES, low hyperactivity/inattention intersectionality profile (b = 1.106, s.e. = 0.329). As noted for research question one, although the evidence is weak, the results suggest that the effect of youth adversity on the rate of change over time may differ across intersectionality profiles and is not uniformly associated with an increase in the rate of change over time.

To what extent is the association between youth adversity and depression/anxiety symptoms (at baseline and their change over time) moderated by gender, SES, and hyperactivity/inattention, and their intersections?

Compound parameter estimates for assessing the moderating effects of the individual characteristics (weighted main effects) and their intersections (weighted interaction effects) are shown in Table 3. There was some, albeit notably weak, evidence for moderation effects in the association between youth adversity and change over time in depression/anxiety symptoms (discussed here for effect sizes greater than half a standard deviation, indicated in bold typeset in Table 3): The first was for a gender by SES interaction. The difference between the estimates for lower SES females (b = 0.169, s.e. = 0.196) and males (b = −0.307, s.e. = 0.200), was 0.543 s.d. greater (s.e. = 0.311, 95% CI −0.042, 1.153) compared to the difference between the estimates for higher SES females (b = −0.008, s.e. = 0.069) and males (b = 0.209, s.e. = 0.067). This reflects a greater moderating effect of gender at a lower level of SES than at a higher level of SES.

Table 3. Compound parameter estimates of youth adversity as a predictor of the latent growth factors of depression/anxiety symptoms

N, number of individuals; SES, socio-economic status.

Values in bold typeset reflect effect sizes greater than half a standard deviation, referred to in the main text.

Note: N = 4441 (with intersectionality profile and youth adversity data).

Compound parameter estimates specified using the maximum likelihood estimates derived from the multiple group model of youth adversity as a predictor of the latent growth factors of depression/anxiety symptoms (online Supplementary Table S5), weighted by intersectionality profile sample size. s.e. and bias-corrected bootstrapped CI from 1000 draws. Weighted pooled standard deviations used for calculation of standardized estimates. Results were substantively unchanged where missing youth adversity data was imputed (10 datasets, N = 4448).

a Average effect of youth adversity on depression/anxiety symptoms at baseline (age 11–12-years).

b Average effect of youth adversity on change over time in depression/anxiety symptoms.

The second was for a gender by SES by hyperactivity/inattention interaction. The difference between the estimates for lower SES females (b = 0.507, s.e. = 0.437) and males (b = −0.626, s.e. = 0.372) compared to higher SES females (b = −0.163, s.e. = 0.171) and males (b = 0.028, s.e. = 0.028) was 1.254 s.d. greater (s.e. = 0.737, 95% CI −2.189, 0.134) for high hyperactivity/inattention, than it was for the difference between the estimates for lower SES females (b = 0.036, s.e. = 0.198) and males (b = −0.104, s.e. = 0.029), compared to higher SES females (b = 0.029, s.e. = 0.077) and males (b = 0.029, s.e. = 0.077) for low hyperactivity/inattention. This reflects a greater moderating effect of gender and SES at a high compared to a low level of hyperactivity/inattention.

No other potentially notable moderating effects were observed for the effect of youth adversity on baseline depression/anxiety symptoms, or change over time in these symptoms, either for the individual characteristics as main effects or for their interactions (reflecting intersectional effects).

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate, (a) the effect of youth adversity on the trajectories of depression/anxiety symptoms in adolescence separately for different intersectionality profiles (based on gender, SES, and hyperactivity/inattention), and (b) the role of intersectionality in differentiating the effect of youth adversity on these trajectories. The findings from this investigation provide evidence to suggest that the detrimental effects of youth adversity on depression/anxiety symptoms at age 11–12-years transcend the intersections of society that were indexed in this study. The findings further provide evidence to suggest that the impact of youth adversity varies across intersectional groups, with preliminary evidence to suggest that this variation may be attributable to the intersections of (i) gender and SES, and (ii) gender, SES, and hyperactivity/inattention, albeit with notably wide confidence intervals around these effects. Collectively, this investigation provides a preliminary foundation to spur further research in this emerging area.

The results supported the hypothesis that youth adversity would be associated with trajectories of elevated depression/anxiety symptoms across intersectionality profiles. These findings are broadly in line with previous research that reported that youth adversity was associated with an increased risk of being in a high/increasing latent trajectory group for adolescent depression/anxiety symptoms compared to a low scoring latent trajectory group (Leban, Reference Leban2021). The current results extend these findings by demonstrating that the detrimental effects of youth adversity are evident across intersections of society indexed by gender, SES, and hyperactivity/inattention. In the current study, youth adversity did not alter the rate of change over time in depression/anxiety symptoms with any certainty. However, importantly – average depression/anxiety symptoms started higher and remained higher over the study period in the presence of youth adversity, compared to an absence of youth adversity, for all intersectionality profiles.

Findings related to the second hypothesis that gender, SES, and hyperactivity/inattention, and the intersections of these characteristics, would moderate the relation between youth adversity and the latent growth factors were less certain. Notably, however, effect sizes for two intersectional effects were above half a standard deviation and were therefore considered to warrant discussion. At a broad level, moderation attributable to between group differences in intersectionality profiles could be inferred through model comparison. More specifically, moderation attributable to the characteristics that were used to classify individuals into intersectionality profiles was suggested for the intersectional effects of (i) gender by SES, suggesting a greater moderating effect of gender at a lower compared to a higher level of SES; and (ii) gender by SES by hyperactivity/inattention interaction, suggesting a greater moderating effect of gender and SES at a high compared to a low level of hyperactivity/inattention. It is possible that the broad, group-level differences (inferred by model comparison) may be reflective of some unmodeled characteristics in the data between the groups. As a hypothetical example, differences between the intersectionality profile groups could reflect characteristics at the school level that correlate with the characteristics of the groups. For instance, neurotypical individuals from higher SES backgrounds are more likely to attend grammar schools compared to nonselective schools than neurodivergent individuals from lower SES backgrounds (Burgess, Crawford, & Macmillan, Reference Burgess, Crawford and Macmillan2018). While speculative, since school information was not available for this sample, this example serves to illustrate why there could be moderation at the group-level that is not explicitly due to the variables that were modeled.

In terms of moderation attributable to intersectionality, while the statistical uncertainty in the estimates prompts caution in over-interpretation, the non-negligible effect sizes are notable. The interaction (intersectional) effects in the current study were specified as compound parameters. These estimates cannot be used in the same way that maximum likelihood estimates could be utilized, for example, in a priori power analysis to estimate sample size requirements in future studies (Hancock & French, Reference Hancock and French2013). It was nonetheless of interest to investigate the effects of increasing the current sample size. The results of post-hoc analyses suggested that doubling the sample size would result in greater statistical certainty in the two specific interaction effects discussed above [i) b = 0.512, 95% CI 0.110, 0.916; ii) b = −1.183, 95% CI −2.251, −0.240]. These pseudo power analyses give a crude estimate of the extent to which sample sizes may need to be increased in future studies, in samples with similar characteristics, to detect the moderating effects of intersectionality as conceptualized in the current study. Further consideration related to this point is noted in the methodological considerations section.

Despite the lack of statistical certainty regarding moderation attributable to intersectionality, the results allude to an intersectional profile characterized by male gender, low SES, and high hyperactivity/inattention as being most vulnerable to experiencing heightened depression/anxiety symptoms at age 11–12-years in the face of youth adversity, compared to an absence of such. The results suggest that males with other vulnerability factors (low SES, high hyperactivity/inattention) may be more susceptible to depression/anxiety in the face of youth adversity compared to females. According to stress inoculation theory (Compton & Pfau, Reference Compton and Pfau2005), and through an intersectional analytic lens (Crenshaw, Reference Crenshaw1990), this could be due to females' development under specific conditions of structural oppression, leading to increased resilience. Clinically, this may be motivation to pay particular attention to adversity-exposed males with low SES and high hyperactivity/inattention. Importantly however, the results underscore the importance of a developmental approach, as they further suggest that youth adversity is associated with a decrease in the rate of change over time in depression/anxiety symptoms for this specific group, compared to an increase, for the counterpart female group (i.e. low SES, high hyperactivity/inattention). The current findings invite replication to probe the nuance of these findings further.

In addition to the effects of sample size, there are several other factors to consider when interpreting the findings of statistical uncertainty in terms of moderation due to intersectionality. The current results only pertain to specific societal/social identities reflected by the intersection of gender, SES, and hyperactivity/inattention, from many possible characteristics. Of note – trans, nonbinary, and gender-diverse identification was not measured in this sample but may be an important component of an individual's intersectional identity (e.g. Kidd et al., Reference Kidd, Sequeira, Douglas, Paglisotti, Inwards-Breland, Miller and Coulter2021). Other individual characteristics such as sexual orientation (Jonas et al., Reference Jonas, Salazar de Pablo, Shum, Nosarti, Abbott and Vaquerizo-Serrano2022), immigration status (Kern et al., Reference Kern, Duinhof, Walsh, Cosma, Moreno-Maldonado, Molcho and Stevens2020) and ethnicity (Mersky, Choi, Plummer Lee, & Janczewski, Reference Mersky, Choi, Plummer Lee and Janczewski2021) may further be important factors. These could not be considered in the current study due to an absence of, or insufficient data. Intersectionality theory originated in the context of the societal positioning of black women of color (Crenshaw, Reference Crenshaw1990). Thus ethnicity is important theoretically, and as well, preliminary empirical findings suggest that ethnicity may contribute to intersectional differences in the development of depression symptoms in young people (Chen & Tung, Reference Chen and Tung2023). Future work planned in our group will build on the current findings by investigating the effects of youth adversity including ethnicity as an intersectional characteristic (Hosang et al., Reference Hosang, Havers, Shuai, Fonagy, Fazel, Morgan and Shakoor2023).

Of note, other recent work in our group found evidence to suggest that the cross-sectional association between youth adversity and depression/anxiety at age 13–14-years in the current sample (pooled with two other Year 9 cohorts from the HeadStart Cornwall study) was moderated by gender, and SES, as individual (non-interacting) characteristics (Havers et al., Reference Havers, Bhui, Shuai, Fonagy, Fazel, Morgan and Hosang2024). This pattern of results was not observed at age 11–12-years (baseline) in the current study. Collectively, these results could suggest that age 13–14-years but not age 11–12-years (where the latent intercept factor was positioned in the current study) represents a specific developmental window of vulnerability for differentiating the effect of youth adversity on depression/anxiety symptoms in terms of gender and SES. However, in repositioning the latent intercept factor to 13–14-years in post-hoc analysis of the current data, moderating effects of gender and SES were not detected. Several factors could be contributing to this divergence in findings. For example, youth adversity in the current study included bullying-victimization across the study period, rather than solely at age 13–14-years as was the case in the cross-sectional study. More work in this area is required to facilitate an in-depth evaluation of and discussion around the source/s of these discrepant findings.

The current findings should spur future research in this area as a foundation for contributing towards a stratified approach to investigating mechanisms linking youth adversity and mental health problems. In turn, this can contribute to theory development, with the ultimate goal of informing clinical and/or community-level interventions that aim to reduce and mitigate the negative impacts of youth adversity.

Methodological considerations

The current study has a number of strengths, including the use of both self-report and objective (i.e. local government, and School Census) youth adversity data, from a large sample of adolescents. Utilizing repeated measures of depression/anxiety symptoms, this study was able to estimate trajectories of mental health in the face of youth adversity. However, several limitations need to be considered when interpreting the findings. First, despite the large overall sample size, some subgroup sample sizes were small. Oversampling the underrepresented intersectional groups in future data collection studies would aid greater precision in the point estimates. Second, the temporal impact of youth adversity could not be ascertained, since adversities were those reported across the study period, not prior to the reporting of symptoms at baseline. It is important for future studies to adopt prospective longitudinal data collection so that temporal relations can be delineated. Notwithstanding, our results show that individuals exposed to youth adversity experience higher levels of depression/anxiety symptoms that remain higher across ages 11–14-years, compared to those who have not experienced adversity.

Third, information about the number of adversities and the specific forms of adversity that were experienced was not available in this study. Instead, the presence or absence of a range of different adversities was provided. More granular detail, including timing-specificity of the experiences (Schalinski et al., Reference Schalinski, Teicher, Nischk, Hinderer, Müller and Rockstroh2016), would allow for investigation into specific adversities that may pose a greater risk for the progression of depression/anxiety symptoms, and for delineation of the cumulative effect of adversities on these symptoms (Petruccelli, Davis, & Berman, Reference Petruccelli, Davis and Berman2019). For example, neglect and emotional abuse have been found to be particularly strongly associated with mental health problems (Kisely et al., Reference Kisely, Abajobir, Mills, Strathearn, Clavarino and Najman2018; Mills et al., Reference Mills, Scott, Alati, O'Callaghan, Najman and Strathearn2013). Future research should expand on previous findings to explore the impact of specific types of youth adversity on trajectories of depression/anxiety symptoms in young people in the context of intersectionality.

Conclusion

In summary, our results indicate that youth adversity has detrimental effects on the development of depression/anxiety symptoms across ages 11–14-years, and that these effects pervade across intersectionality profiles. Our findings add to a growing body of literature that point to the negative impact of youth adversity on adolescent mental health, and futher underscore the pervasiveness of these effects across societal intersections in the United Kingdom. Our findings provide tentative evidence to suggest that the intersectionality profile characterized by male gender, lower SES, and high hyperactivity/inattention may be at heightened risk of experiencing elevated levels of depression/anxiety symptoms at age 11–12-years in the face of youth adversity. Nonetheless, they also provide preliminary evidence to suggest that youth adversity is associated with an attenuation in the development of these symptoms over time for this group, in contrast to females, where youth adversity continues to exert a detrimental effect over time. An accumulation of research in this area is fundamental for drawing conclusions regarding whether and to what extent intersectional identity is an important contextual condition for differentiating the effects of youth adversity on trajectories of depression/anxiety symptoms in adolescence.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724000795

Acknowledgements

The ATTUNE project is funded by a cross council UK Research and Innovation [UKRI] award (MR/W002183/1). Additionally, this research was funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Applied Research Collaboration Oxford and Thames Valley at Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust. KB is part supported by Oxford Health NIHR Biomedical Research Centre. The HeadStart Cornwall data was collected as part of the HeadStart learning program and supported by funding from The National Lottery Community Fund. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, NIHR, Department of Health, or The National Lottery Community Fund. LH is grateful to Gregory R. Hancock for guidance in the statistical aspects of this work. The authors are grateful to all the research teams and participants who have contributed to the data that was analyzed in this paper, and to all individuals involved in the ATTUNE project young people advisory groups.

Competing interests

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Footnotes

*

Joint last authors.

References

Assini-Meytin, L. C., Fix, R. L., Green, K. M., Nair, R., & Letourneau, E. J. (2022). Adverse childhood experiences, mental health, and risk behaviors in adulthood: Exploring sex, racial, and ethnic group differences in a nationally representative sample. Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma, 15(3), 833845. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40653-021-00424-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bevilacqua, L., Kelly, Y., Heilmann, A., Priest, N., & Lacey, R. E. (2021). Adverse childhood experiences and trajectories of internalizing, externalizing, and prosocial behaviors from childhood to adolescence. Child Abuse & Neglect, 112, 104890. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104890CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biswas, T., Scott, J. G., Munir, K., Renzaho, A. M. N., Rawal, L. B., Baxter, J., & Mamun, A. A. (2020). Global variation in the prevalence of suicidal ideation, anxiety and their correlates among adolescents: A population based study of 82 countries. eClinicalMedicine, 24, 100395. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eclinm.2020.100395CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S.-J., Burnett, S., & Dahl, R. E. (2010). The role of puberty in the developing adolescent brain. Human Brain Mapping, 31(6), 926933. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21052CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowleg, L. (2012). The problem with the phrase women and minorities: Intersectionality – an important theoretical framework for public health. American Journal of Public Health, 102(7), 12671273. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2012.300750CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgess, S., Crawford, C., & Macmillan, L. (2018). Access to grammar schools by socio-economic status. Environment and Planning A: Economy and Space, 50(7), 13811385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, E. Y.-J., & Tung, E. Y.-L. (2023). Similarities and differences in the longitudinal trajectories of depressive symptoms from mid-adolescence to young adulthood: The intersectionality of gender, race/ethnicity, and levels of depressive symptoms. Journal of Racial and Ethnic Health Disparities, 116. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40615-023-01630-5Google Scholar
Compton, J. A., & Pfau, M. (2005). Inoculation theory of resistance to influence at maturity: Recent progress in theory development and application and suggestions for future research. Annals of the International Communication Association, 29(1), 97146. https://doi.org/10.1080/23808985.2005.11679045CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craig, S. G., Bondi, B. C., O'Donnell, K. A., Pepler, D. J., & Weiss, M. D. (2020). ADHD and exposure to maltreatment in children and youth: A systematic review of the past 10 years. Current Psychiatry Reports, 22(12), 79. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-020-01193-wCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crenshaw, K. (1990). Mapping the margins: Intersectionality, identity politics, and violence against women of color. Stanford Law Review, 43(6), 12411300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deighton, J., Lereya, S. T., Casey, P., Patalay, P., Humphrey, N., & Wolpert, M. (2019). Prevalence of mental health problems in schools: Poverty and other risk factors among 28 000 adolescents in England. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 215(3), 565567. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2019.19CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desch, J., Mansuri, F., Tran, D., Schwartz, S. W., & Bakour, C. (2023). The association between adverse childhood experiences and depression trajectories in the add health study. Child Abuse & Neglect, 137, 106034. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2023.106034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., Williamson, D. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards, V., … Marks, J. S. (1998). Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: The adverse childhood experiences (ACE) study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 14(4), 245258. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-3797(98)00017-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghavami, N., Katsiaficas, D., & Rogers, L. O. (2016). Chapter two – toward an intersectional approach in developmental science: The role of race, gender, sexual orientation, and immigrant status. In Horn, S. S., Ruck, M. D., & Liben, L. S. (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 50, pp. 3173). London, UK: JAI. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.acdb.2015.12.001Google Scholar
Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 581586. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hancock, G. R., & French, B. F. (2013). Power analysis in structural equation modeling. In Structural equation modeling: A second course (2nd ed., pp. 117159). Charlotte, NC, USA: IAP Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Havers, L., Bhui, K., Shuai, R., Fonagy, P., Fazel, M., Morgan, C., … Hosang, G. (2024). The role of intersectionality in differentiating the impact of youth adversity on adolescent depression and anxiety symptoms in the community. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/6ct3xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hosang, G. M., Havers, L., Shuai, R., Fonagy, P., Fazel, M., Morgan, C., … Shakoor, S. (2023). Protocol for secondary data analysis of 4 UK cohorts examining youth adversity and mental health in the context of intersectionality. PLoS ONE, 18(8), e0289438. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0289438CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jonas, L., Salazar de Pablo, G., Shum, M., Nosarti, C., Abbott, C., & Vaquerizo-Serrano, J. (2022). A systematic review and meta-analysis investigating the impact of childhood adversities on the mental health of LGBT + youth. JCPP Advances, 2(2), e12079. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcv2.12079CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalmakis, K. A., & Chandler, G. E. (2014). Adverse childhood experiences: Towards a clear conceptual meaning. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 70(7), 14891501. https://doi.org/10.1111/jan.12329CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kern, M. R., Duinhof, E. L., Walsh, S. D., Cosma, A., Moreno-Maldonado, C., Molcho, M., … Stevens, G. W. J. M. (2020). Intersectionality and adolescent mental well-being: A cross-nationally comparative analysis of the interplay between immigration background, socioeconomic status and gender. Journal of Adolescent Health, 66(Suppl 6), S12S20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2020.02.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kidd, K. M., Sequeira, G. M., Douglas, C., Paglisotti, T., Inwards-Breland, D. J., Miller, E., & Coulter, R. W. S. (2021). Prevalence of gender-diverse youth in an urban school district. Pediatrics, 147(6), e2020049823. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2020-049823CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kisely, S., Abajobir, A. A., Mills, R., Strathearn, L., Clavarino, A., & Najman, J. M. (2018). Child maltreatment and mental health problems in adulthood: Birth cohort study. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 213(6), 698703. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2018.207CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leban, L. (2021). The effects of adverse childhood experiences and gender on developmental trajectories of internalizing and externalizing outcomes. Crime & Delinquency, 67(5), 631661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Y., Millsap, R. E., West, S. G., Tein, J.-Y., Tanaka, R., & Grimm, K. J. (2017). Testing measurement invariance in longitudinal data with ordered-categorical measures. Psychological Methods, 22(3), 486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, Yu, Millsap, Roger E, West, Stephen G., Tein, Jenn-Yun, Tanaka, Rika, & Grimm, Kevin J. (2017). Testing measurement invariance in longitudinal data with ordered-categorical measures. Psychological Methods, 22(3), 486506. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/met0000075CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, H. W., Hau, K.-T., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden rules: Comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler's (1999) findings. Structural Equation Modeling, 11(3), 320341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mersky, J. P., Choi, C., Plummer Lee, C., & Janczewski, C. E. (2021). Disparities in adverse childhood experiences by race/ethnicity, gender, and economic status: Intersectional analysis of a nationally representative sample. Child Abuse & Neglect, 117, 105066. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2021.105066CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michaud, P.-A., & Fombonne, E. (2005). Common mental health problems. BMJ, 330(7495), 835838. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.330.7495.835CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mills, R., Scott, J., Alati, R., O'Callaghan, M., Najman, J. M., & Strathearn, L. (2013). Child maltreatment and adolescent mental health problems in a large birth cohort. Child Abuse & Neglect, 37(5), 292302. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2012.11.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morin, A. J. S., Maïano, C., Nagengast, B., Marsh, H. W., Morizot, J., & Janosz, M. (2011). General growth mixture analysis of adolescents’ developmental trajectories of anxiety: The impact of untested invariance assumptions on substantive interpretations. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 18(4), 613648. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2011.607714CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Musliner, K. L., Munk-Olsen, T., Eaton, W. W., & Zandi, P. P. (2016). Heterogeneity in long-term trajectories of depressive symptoms: Patterns, predictors and outcomes. Journal of Affective Disorders, 192, 199211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.12.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petruccelli, K., Davis, J., & Berman, T. (2019). Adverse childhood experiences and associated health outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Child Abuse & Neglect, 97, 104127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.104127CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schalinski, I., Teicher, M. H., Nischk, D., Hinderer, E., Müller, O., & Rockstroh, B. (2016). Type and timing of adverse childhood experiences differentially affect severity of PTSD, dissociative and depressive symptoms in adult inpatients. BMC Psychiatry, 16(1), 295. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-016-1004-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schubert, K. O., Clark, S. R., Van, L. K., Collinson, J. L., & Baune, B. T. (2017). Depressive symptom trajectories in late adolescence and early adulthood: A systematic review. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 51(5), 477499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shorey, S., Ng, E. D., & Wong, C. H. J. (2022). Global prevalence of depression and elevated depressive symptoms among adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 61(2), 287305. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjc.12333CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Svetina, Dubravka, Rutkowski, Leslie, & Rutkowski, David. (2020). Multiple-Group Invariance with Categorical Outcomes Using Updated Guidelines: An Illustration Using M plus and the lavaan/semTools Packages. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 27(1), 111130. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2019.1602776CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Udry, J. R. (2000). Biological limits of gender construction. American Sociological Review, 65(3), 443457. https://doi.org/10.2307/2657466CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van de Schoot, R., Lugtig, P., & Hox, J. (2012). A checklist for testing measurement invariance. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9(4), 486492. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2012.686740CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walsh, D., McCartney, G., Smith, M., & Armour, G. (2019). Relationship between childhood socioeconomic position and adverse childhood experiences (ACEs): A systematic review. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health, 73(12), 10871093. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech-2019-212738CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Schematic diagram of the multiple group conditional latent growth model.Note. SES: socio-economic status. Intersectional profile (depicted at the center of the Venn diagram, above left) is used as the grouping variable in a multiple group model, where everything inside of the box (above right) is estimated for each intersectionality profile group. Compound parameters are further estimated, reflecting the extent to which the individual characteristics, as well as the intersections between them (i.e. the shaded areas of the Venn diagram), moderate the paths from youth adversity to the latent growth factors. The observed depression/anxiety symptoms scores are indicators of the latent growth factors. A non-schematic, labeled path diagram is shown in online Supplementary Fig. 1.

Figure 1

Table 1. Sample description

Figure 2

Table 2. Youth adversity and depression/anxiety symptoms by intersectionality profiles

Figure 3

Figure 2. Effect of youth adversity on average depression/anxiety symptoms trajectories by intersectionality profiles.Note. SES: socio-economic status. Y axis: depression/anxiety symptoms total observed score. School year corresponds to the following average ages: 11–12-years (Year 7), 12–13-years (Year 8), 13–14-years (Year 9).

Figure 4

Table 3. Compound parameter estimates of youth adversity as a predictor of the latent growth factors of depression/anxiety symptoms

Supplementary material: File

Havers et al. supplementary material

Havers et al. supplementary material
Download Havers et al. supplementary material(File)
File 218.6 KB