Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T02:35:48.051Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Unsparing self-critique strengthens the field, but Bailey et al. overstate the ‘problems with delay discounting’

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 February 2022

Jeffrey S. Stein*
Affiliation:
Fralin Biomedical Research Institute at VTC, Virginia Tech, Roanoke, VA, USA
James MacKillop
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural Neurosciences, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada
Samuel M. McClure
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA
Warren K. Bickel
Affiliation:
Fralin Biomedical Research Institute at VTC, Virginia Tech, Roanoke, VA, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Jeffrey S. Stein, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Correspondence
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

We agree with a fundamental premise of Bailey, Romeu, and Finn's (Reference Bailey, Romeu and Finn2021) recent paper; that is, understanding the limitations of the delay discounting (DD) paradigm will increase the value of research on intertemporal choice (ITC) in clinical disorders. To this agreement, however, we add two provisos. First, the significance of those limitations should be determined by systematic review and meta-analysis, not selective review or personal opinion. Bailey et al.'s narrative review reflects a bias for their position that DD measures have not been adequately validated and lack generalizability, without regard for evidence that may mitigate these concerns. Second, the strengths of the paradigm should be acknowledged in order to avoid, in the pursuit of progress, weakening the measures we seek to refine. In what follows, we highlight our agreements and disagreements with Bailey et al.'s three major concerns: convergent validity, divergent validity, and generalizability of DD measures.

Convergent validity

Bailey et al. acknowledge the well-known observation that DD is largely uncorrelated with measures of impulsivity. While accurate, this view fails to characterize the state of the field. Impulsivity itself is a multifactorial construct showing little correlation among its putative forms (e.g. acting without forethought, response disinhibition, and DD). Decades of research have failed to identify common neurobiological mechanisms among these various ‘impulsivities’, and the effects of behavioral and pharmacological interventions vary across forms. For these and other reasons, some have called for rejecting the superordinate construct of impulsivity in favor of continued, rigorous inquiry of its various component measures (Strickland & Johnson, Reference Strickland and Johnson2021). We agree with this perspective.

Bailey et al. outline additional concerns over convergent validity, arguing that DD has yet to show strong and replicable associations with other cognitive and personality measures. These authors also suggest that, in order to be considered an ‘important’ individual difference, DD should show incremental validity over these other measures when examining associations with substance use disorder (SUD) and other clinical disorders. We agree that DD has shown relatively modest associations with executive function and personality measures, but these findings are replicable at the behavioral and neurobiological levels. Such consistent, but limited, covariance with a variety of measures suggests not necessarily, as Bailey et al. argue, that DD is an unimportant individual difference, but rather that its determinants are multivariate.

Moreover, Bailey et al. disregard emerging evidence of incremental validity. Specifically, using machine-learning approaches to predict SUD, measures of DD outperform the Stroop task, Iowa Gambling task, Continuous Performance task, and other measures of attention, planning, and inhibitory control (e.g. Bickel, Moody, Eddy, & Franck, Reference Bickel, Moody, Eddy and Franck2017). Nonetheless, we agree that the literature would benefit from additional studies examining incremental validity, particularly in predicting health behaviors beyond substance use.

Divergent validity

Bailey et al. note concerns over divergent validity of the DD paradigm, citing evidence that elevated DD is robustly associated with not only SUD but various other psychiatric disorders, including depression, bipolar disorder, and schizophrenia (for a meta-analysis, see Amlung et al., Reference Amlung, Marsden, Holshausen, Morris, Patel, Vedelago and McCabe2019). Thus, as Bailey et al. argue, measures of DD may not differentially predict diagnostic status among patients with varied disorders. This remains an empirical question and more research should examine the specificity of such associations. However, some research provides emerging evidence of divergent validity. For example, SUD and comorbid mood and personality disorders are interactively associated with higher DD compared to SUD alone (e.g. Moody, Franck, & Bickel, Reference Moody, Franck and Bickel2016). Conversely, anorexia nervosa is associated with lower DD compared to healthy controls (for a meta-analysis, see Amlung et al., Reference Amlung, Marsden, Holshausen, Morris, Patel, Vedelago and McCabe2019). Thus, DD rates at opposing ends of a continuum are associated with divergent pathology.

More generally, we note that the well-documented comorbidity between SUD and other psychiatric disorders suggests transdiagnostic risk factors and etiologies. That DD is associated with a variety of clinical endpoints is not necessarily a cause for concern, but may instead be a strength of the paradigm and provides the opportunity to investigate a single decision-making process relevant to a broad range of disorders. However, further research is needed to understand the circumstances under which genetic, neurobiological, or environmental predispositions manifest in divergent phenotypes. Indeed, this pursuit is a focus of the NIH's Research Domain Criteria (RDoC), an area in which DD holds some promise (e.g. Levitt et al., Reference Levitt, Oshri, Amlung, Ray, Sanchez-Roige, Palmer and MacKillop2022).

Generalizability

Bailey et al. argue that little evidence suggests that DD is a generalizable measure of decision-making. In the process, these authors disregard evidence of reliable correlations in measures of discounting across different commodities (e.g. money, food, health, and drugs of abuse; for a systematic review, see Odum et al., Reference Odum, Becker, Haynes, Galizio, Frye, Downey and Perez2020). Thus, contrary to the limited findings reviewed by Bailey et al., knowledge of how an individual discounts one delayed commodity provides information about how they are likely to discount other delayed commodities.

Given this correspondence, the majority of studies on DD preferentially examine discounting of monetary outcomes because, unlike non-monetary outcomes, money is universally valued, easily quantifiable, highly fungible, and its utility curve is approximately linear across a broad range of values. We recognize, however, that discounting of non-monetary, pathology-relevant commodities in some circumstances may provide incremental validity over the study of monetary outcomes (e.g. discounting of food in the study of obesity; for a meta-analysis, see Amlung, Petker, Jackson, Balodis, & MacKillop, Reference Amlung, Petker, Jackson, Balodis and MacKillop2016). As such, future research should continue to examine the relationship between non-monetary discounting, SUD, and other disorders. Moreover, choice arrangements that may more closely model real-world ITC should also be explored (Green & Myerson, Reference Green and Myerson2019). For example, ‘cross-commodity’ discounting tasks arrange choices between qualitatively different immediate and delayed outcomes, which may characterize intertemporal tradeoffs evident in health-related decision-making (e.g. immediate food v. delayed weight loss in the study of obesity).

Conclusions

In the era of the ‘replication crisis’, the links between DD and clinical disorders are notably robust. At the heart of Bailey et al.'s concerns is the gap between theoretical concepts (in this case, ITC) and their operational definitions (in this case, DD). Critically, ITC is multifaceted, including multiple dimensions of time orientation (past, present, and future), gains and losses, and same- or cross-commodity choice. Bailey et al. take issue with the fact the literature operationalizes ITC in only one way (DD), contending this provides an incomplete perspective. We do not disagree and likewise recommend a more fulsome study of the published varieties of ITC. Indeed, we have conducted empirical research mapping these less studied varieties. Fundamentally, however, promoting this more expansive perspective should not justify overstating the limitations of the robust and trenchant literature on DD in SUD and other disorders.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of interest

Although the following activities/relationships do not create conflicts of interest pertaining to this manuscript, in the interest of full disclosure, the authors would like to report the following: JSS works on a project supported by Indivior, Inc. and has received subcontract funding through an NIH grant awarded to BEAM Diagnostics, Inc. JM is a principal in BEAM Diagnostics, Inc. and is a consultant for Clairvoyant Therapeutics, Inc. WKB is a principal in HealthSim, LLC; BEAM Diagnostics, Inc.; and Red 5 Group, LLC. WKB also serves on the scientific advisory board for Sober Grid, Inc. and Ria Health; and serves as a consultant for Boehringer Ingelheim International; and works on a project supported by Indivior, Inc. SMM has no disclosures to report.

References

Amlung, M., Marsden, E., Holshausen, K., Morris, V., Patel, H., Vedelago, L., … McCabe, R. E. (2019). Delay discounting as a transdiagnostic process in psychiatric disorders: A meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(11), 11761186. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.2102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amlung, M., Petker, T., Jackson, J., Balodis, I., & MacKillop, J. (2016). Steep discounting of delayed monetary and food rewards in obesity: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 46(11), 24232434. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716000866.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bailey, A. J., Romeu, R. J., & Finn, P. R. (2021). The problems with delay discounting: A critical review of current practices and clinical applications. Psychological Medicine, 51, 17991806. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721002282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bickel, W. K., Moody, L. N., Eddy, C. R., & Franck, C. T. (2017). Neurocognitive dysfunction in addiction: Testing hypotheses of diffuse versus selective phenotypic dysfunction with a classification-based approach. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 25(4), 322332. doi: 10.1037/pha0000115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2019). On the complexity of discounting, choice situations, and people. Perspectives on Behavior Science, 42(3), 433443. doi: 10.1007/s40614–019-00209-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levitt, E. E., Oshri, A., Amlung, M., Ray, L. A., Sanchez-Roige, S., Palmer, A. A., … MacKillop, J. (2022). Evaluation of delay discounting as a transdiagnostic Research Domain Criteria indicator in 1388 general community adults. Psychological Medicine. Advance Online Publication. doi:10.1017/S0033291721005110.Google Scholar
Moody, L., Franck, C., & Bickel, W. K. (2016). Comorbid depression, antisocial personality, and substance dependence: Relationship with delay discounting. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 160, 190196. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.01.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Odum, A. L., Becker, R. J., Haynes, J. M., Galizio, A., Frye, C. C. J., Downey, H., … Perez, D. M. (2020). Delay discounting of different outcomes: Review and theory. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 113(3), 657679. doi: 10.1002/jeab.589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strickland, J. C., & Johnson, M. W. (2021). Rejecting impulsivity as a psychological construct: A theoretical, empirical, and sociocultural argument. Psychological Review, 128(2), 336361. doi: 10.1037/rev0000263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed