Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T23:06:03.335Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The temporal dynamics of sleep disturbance and psychopathology in psychosis: a digital sampling study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 January 2021

Nicholas Meyer*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychology, Psychiatry and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King's College London, London, UK
Dan W. Joyce
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, National Institute of Health Research, Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre, Warneford Hospital, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Chris Karr
Affiliation:
Audacious Technologies, Chicago, IL, USA
Maarten de Vos
Affiliation:
Institute of Biomedical Engineering, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK ESAT, Department of Engineering & Department of Development and Regeneration, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Derk-Jan Dijk
Affiliation:
Sleep Research Centre, University of Surrey, Surrey, UK UK Dementia Research Institute, London, UK
Nicholas C. Jacobson
Affiliation:
Center for Technology and Behavioral Health, Geisel School of Medicine, Dartmouth College, Lebanon, NH, USA
James H. MacCabe
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychology, Psychiatry and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King's College London, London, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Nicholas Meyer, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Sleep disruption is a common precursor to deterioration and relapse in people living with psychotic disorders. Understanding the temporal relationship between sleep and psychopathology is important for identifying and developing interventions which target key variables that contribute to relapse.

Methods

We used a purpose-built digital platform to sample self-reported sleep and psychopathology variables over 1 year, in 36 individuals with schizophrenia. Once-daily measures of sleep duration and sleep quality, and fluctuations in psychopathology (positive and negative affect, cognition and psychotic symptoms) were captured. We examined the temporal relationship between these variables using the Differential Time-Varying Effect (DTVEM) hybrid exploratory-confirmatory model.

Results

Poorer sleep quality and shorter sleep duration maximally predicted deterioration in psychosis symptoms over the subsequent 1–8 and 1–12 days, respectively. These relationships were also mediated by negative affect and cognitive symptoms. Psychopathology variables also predicted sleep quality, but not sleep duration, and the effect sizes were smaller and of shorter lag duration.

Conclusions

Reduced sleep duration and poorer sleep quality anticipate the exacerbation of psychotic symptoms by approximately 1–2 weeks, and negative affect and cognitive symptoms mediate this relationship. We also observed a reciprocal relationship that was of shorter duration and smaller magnitude. Sleep disturbance may play a causal role in symptom exacerbation and relapse, and represents an important and tractable target for intervention. It warrants greater attention as an early warning sign of deterioration, and low-burden, user-friendly digital tools may play a role in its early detection.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Sleep disturbance is a common feature of the relapse prodrome of schizophrenia, reported in between 43% and 70% of patients (Birchwood et al., Reference Birchwood, Smith, Macmillan, Hogg, Prasad, Harvey and Bering1989; Heinrichs & Carpenter, Reference Heinrichs and Carpenter1985; Herz & Melville, Reference Herz and Melville1980; Jorgensen, Reference Jorgensen1998), and appearing in the weeks prior to the onset of significant deterioration in mental state. These observations are clinically relevant, as they raise the possibility that dysregulated sleep is an important early warning sign of deterioration. If this were to be the case, sleep dysfunction may also play a causal role in psychotic deterioration and relapse, and present a tractable target for preventative interventions which avert progression to relapse. However, no studies have specifically examined the time-course of sleep disturbance in relation to the deterioration of psychosis.

Recent clinical studies have begun to examine the hypothesised association between sleep and psychopathology. In a 7-day experience sampling method (ESM) study of self-reported and actigraphic sleep variables on next-day symptoms in schizophrenia, Mulligan, Haddock, Emsley, Neil, and Kyle (Reference Mulligan, Haddock, Emsley, Neil and Kyle2016) found that poorer sleep predicted greater next-day negative affect and psychosis symptoms, with negative affect mediating a proportion of this relationship. Similarly, in a 6-day ESM study in individuals on the psychosis-spectrum, Kasanova, Hajdúk, Thewissen, and Myin-Germeys (Reference Kasanova, Hajdúk, Thewissen and Myin-Germeys2020) reported a significant effect of poor sleep quality on paranoia the next morning, with negative affect fully mediating the relationship. Here, no evidence of an inverse relationship – greater paranoia on evening sleep quality – was found. A third study by Reeve, Nickless, Sheaves, and Freeman (Reference Reeve, Nickless, Sheaves and Freeman2018) assessed subjective insomnia, psychosis and negative affect symptoms at monthly intervals over 3 months, in 29 patients with non-affective psychosis. Insomnia predicted later psychosis symptoms, and this relationship was again mediated by negative affect. This study also found a reciprocal effect between psychotic symptoms and insomnia, implying a bidirectional relationship between these variables.

Although canonical ESM offers high temporal resolution by intensively sampling mental state at multiple time-points throughout the day, the approach has some limitations. It is demanding of the participant, and lends itself to short periods of observation (Palmier-Claus et al., Reference Palmier-Claus, Myin-Germeys, Barkus, Bentley, Udachina, Delespaul and Dunn2011). Analytic approaches have typically used multilevel models to examine short-term associations between variables from one day to the next, and are not optimised for exploring lag structures over longer timescales. Studies are required that are of sufficient duration to capture longitudinal fluctuations in sleep and psychopathology, while of adequate temporal resolution to sample variation over a range of timescales. Methods of analysis that allow the temporal structure between longitudinal variables to be examined are also necessary.

Designed collaboratively with service users, the Sleepsight platform (Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Kerz, Folarin, Joyce, Jackson, Karr and Maccabe2018) capitalises on the ubiquity and usability of smartphone technologies to allow long-term sampling of self-reported sleep (sleep quality and duration) and psychopathology variables (positive affect, negative affect, cognition and psychosis symptom) in individuals living with psychosis, with minimal burden to the user. We captured these variables once a day over the course of a year, in 36 individuals with schizophrenia, collecting over 12 000 data-points.

We employed an analytic tool developed specifically for investigating the temporal structure of intensive longitudinal data, the Differential Time-Varying Effect Model (DTVEM; Jacobson, Chow, & Newman, Reference Jacobson, Chow and Newman2019). DTVEM examines the lag structure between one or more time-series, using a two-stage, exploratory–confirmatory model. This approach examines the lags at which one construct maximally predicts itself, or another construct, at a later time, and here allows the strength and timing of association between sleep and psychopathology to be studied (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1. (a ) Simulated time-series for one participant. An increase in the severity of symptom A appears to precede that of symptom B, and the temporal relationship is suggestive of A driving B. DTVEM looks for the same pattern across the entire time-series, for all participants. (b) Lagged regression between the two symptom time-series produces two coefficients, A to B and B to A. Here, symptom A maximally predicts symptom B over a threshold of statistical significance, with a lag of 3–17 days. However, symptom B does not significantly predict symptom A.

We predicted that shorter sleep duration and poorer sleep quality would be associated with greater intensity of psychotic, negative affect and cognitive symptoms. In the context of findings supporting a bidirectional relationship between sleep disturbances and psychopathology from the mood disorder literature (Talbot et al., Reference Talbot, Stone, Gruber, Hairston, Eidelman and Harvey2012), we also expected to observe a reciprocal relationship between psychopathology and sleep, but predicted that this association would be of shorter duration and lower magnitude. In line with previous research, we also hypothesised that the relationship between sleep and psychosis symptoms would be mediated by negative affect but also cognitive symptoms, as evidenced by a weaker and less durable direct effect of sleep on psychosis when these variables are included as mediators.

Methods

Sleepsight app and platform

Developed specifically to optimise acceptability and feasibility in people with psychosis, the Sleepsight system consisted of an Android smartphone application and secure back-end infrastructure, for the real-time collection of self-reported sleep and psychopathology variables. Further details of the development and testing of the system (Biagianti, Hidalgo-Mazzei, & Meyer, Reference Biagianti, Hidalgo-Mazzei and Meyer2017; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Kerz, Folarin, Joyce, Jackson, Karr and Maccabe2018) and source code for the application have been published elsewhere (Karr, Reference Karr2017). Although Sleepsight also gathered continuous objective rest-activity variables from smartphone sensors and wearable devices, in the present paper, we focus uniquely on the self-reported, subjective variables.

A fixed sampling schedule prompted users to complete a brief once-daily sleep and symptom diary, between 11:00 and 14:00. This window was chosen to be early enough to minimise forgetting the previous night's sleep times, while also avoiding diurnal extremes of mood (Peeters, Berkhof, Delespaul, Rottenberg, & Nicolson, Reference Peeters, Berkhof, Delespaul, Rottenberg and Nicolson2006). If not completed after the first notification, participants received two further prompts at 30 min intervals. The sleep diary (Fig. 2b) consisted of four items: time into bed, time out of bed, sleep duration and sleep quality, rated from 1 (‘very poor’) to 5 (‘very good’). All times were entered using a 12 h clock, with AM and PM specifiers, to the nearest 15 min. The symptom diary (Fig. 2c, d) consisted of items validated and widely used in previous ESM studies of psychosis (e.g. Myin-Germeys, Van Os, Schwartz, Stone, & Delespaul, Reference Myin-Germeys, Van Os, Schwartz, Stone and Delespaul2001; Palmier-Claus et al. Reference Palmier-Claus, Ainsworth, Machin, Barrowclough, Dunn, Barkus and Lewis2012). Rated on a seven-point Likert scale anchored at ‘not at all’ to ‘very much’, they consisted of seven mood items (‘over the past 24 h, I have been feeling: cheerful, anxious, relaxed, irritable, sad, in control, stressed’) and seven items relating to psychosis and cognition (‘over the past 24 h, I have been experiencing/feeling: suspicious, trouble concentrating, preoccupied by thoughts, others dislike me, confused, others influence my thoughts, unusual sights and sounds’). Positive and negative mood items were counterbalanced, to reduce skew and minimise stereotyped responses. The diary took approximately 1–3 min to complete, and all sections had to be completed in order for the app to allow submission. Participants received a motivational message on submitting a diary, but did not receive feedback based on their entries.

Fig. 2. Screenshots of Sleepsight user-facing app: (a) splash page, (b) sleep diary, (c) mood symptoms, (d) psychosis symptoms. Note that only a section of screens (bd) are visible.

Participants and study procedures

Adults aged 18–65, living in the community with a diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, and under the care of secondary care mental health services were invited to take part. Participants were required to be able to provide informed consent and interact comfortably with the smartphone interface, but were otherwise not excluded on the severity of their psychosis symptoms or medication status.

On study entry, the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS), Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI; Buysse, Reynolds, Monk, Berman, & Kupfer, Reference Buysse, Reynolds, Monk, Berman and Kupfer1989) and Insomnia Severity Index (ISI; Bastien, Vallières, & Morin, Reference Bastien, Vallières and Morin2001) were administered. Participants without their own device were provided with an Android smartphone, which they were asked to use as their sole device throughout the study period. For eligible participants already in possession of an Android device, the Sleepsight app was installed on their own device. Therefore, the risk of non-response by having multiple devices was minimised, by ensuring all participants only made use of one device. All participants received equal compensation for their time, regardless of device ownership status, and received a face-to-face training session on enrolment.

Participants were asked to respond to the once-daily smartphone notification by completing the smartphone diary, and use their mobile devices as normal, for 12 months. Participants received a weekly motivational SMS message, and were contacted if they were having technical difficulties, or if the engagement was low. Otherwise, no change was made to the delivery of psychiatric care. Screening for eligibility and all clinical assessments were undertaken by an experienced psychiatrist (NM), and the study received ethical approval from the local Research Ethics Committee.

Data analysis

Data were organised with the unit of time as one day. Four summary psychopathology variables were computed from diary items: positive affect (mean of ‘cheerful, ‘relaxed’ and ‘in control’); negative affect (mean of ‘anxious’, ‘irritable’, ‘sad’, ‘stressed’), cognitive symptoms (mean of ‘trouble concentrating’, ‘confused’) and psychosis symptoms (mean of ‘suspicious’, ‘preoccupied by thoughts’, ‘others dislike me’, ‘others influence my thoughts’ and ‘unusual sights and sounds’). In order to examine associations between sleep and individual psychotic symptoms in more detail, two additional symptoms were included in a secondary analysis: hallucinations (i.e. the ‘unusual sights and sounds’ item) and paranoia (mean of ‘suspicious’ and ‘others dislike me’ items). Duplicate entries within a 24 h period were removed, and summary variables visualised (Fig. 3). Participants whose data showed minimal variation, suggestive of stereotyped responses, were excluded, and the rate of diary completion over the 12-month period was calculated.

Fig. 3. Example responses over 12 months from one participant, fitted with a loess smoother with degree 1 polynomial. Higher scores indicate better sleep quality and positive affect, longer sleep duration, and worse negative affect and psychosis symptoms.

The predictive lag structure between longitudinal variables was then estimated using DTVEM (Jacobson et al., Reference Jacobson, Chow and Newman2019), which estimates the lags at which one variable predicts another, over a threshold of statistical significance from a multi-stage modelling procedure (Fig. 1). In the exploratory stage, DTVEM uses generalised additive mixed models [GAMMs, as part of R package mgcv (Wood, Reference Wood2011)] to identify lags that have substantial effects on the outcome variable, thereby narrowing down the search space and improving the computational efficiency of the confirmatory step. Next, DTVEM passes these to a vector autoregressive of order p [VAR(p)] model [using the OpenMx (Neale et al., Reference Neale, Hunter, Pritikin, Zahery, Brick, Kirkpatrick and Boker2016) R package], which uses a state-space modelling framework to confirm when one variable maximally predicts another. DTVEM cycles iteratively between stages until an optimal solution is found. We only report lag relationships between variables which DTVEM has identified as statistically significant, and also report the combined total effect size of all significant lags between two variables, rather than the effect sizes of each individual lag. DTVEM has been shown to be robust to missing data. This two-stage approach has been shown to be conservative in dramatically reducing type I error, while retaining high power.

In the bivariate, unmediated analyses, a single predictor (sleep quality or duration) and outcome (psychopathology variable) were modelled, to establish the total unadjusted lag relationship (i.e. the total effect between the two variables). Next, the variables were reversed, to examine the reciprocal lag relationship between psychopathology and sleep. An auto-regression model was also fitted to examine whether sleep variables predicted themselves over time.

The mediation analyses followed the approach of Baron and Kenny (Reference Baron and Kenny1986) and used the joint significance test, which has been shown to perform better than many of the alternative methods (Leth-Steensen & Gallitto, Reference Leth-Steensen and Gallitto2016). First, the putative mediator variable (negative affect or cognitive symptoms) was fitted to the predictor (sleep variable): the absence of a predictive relationship would suggest that the intermediate variable could not act as a mediator. Next, the mediator and predictor were entered together in the same model, with psychosis symptoms as the outcome variable. This allows estimation of the direct effect between predictor and outcome in the presence of the mediator, and the indirect effect between predictor and outcome via the mediator. A reduction in the size of the direct effect in comparison with the total effect would support the presence of the mediator, and favour the mediated model. Also, model fit for the unmediated and mediated analyses was compared using the Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC), which penalises for the complexity of the model and sample size. A lower BIC indicates a closer fit of model to the data. Finally, as negative affect and cognitive symptoms are hypothesised to have a reciprocal relationship on sleep, an oppositional model was examined, where the above steps were repeated with the sleep variable as the mediator and negative affect/cognitive symptoms as the predictor. The effect sizes and model fit of both models were compared, and the model which provided the most parsimonious explanation for the data was favoured.

For all analyses, a lag window of 1–20 days was chosen. Given that each analysis is computationally intensive, this strikes a balance between reducing analysis time, while allowing a realistic window of time within which early signs of relapse have been reported in previous studies (e.g. Birchwood et al., Reference Birchwood, Smith, Macmillan, Hogg, Prasad, Harvey and Bering1989; Heinrichs & Carpenter, Reference Heinrichs and Carpenter1985; Herz & Melville, Reference Herz and Melville1980; Jorgensen, Reference Jorgensen1998). All variables were standardised to obtain person-centred z-scores, and the number of k selection points was set at 19 (i.e. lag window −1). Analyses were undertaken in R (R Core Team, 2020), using the Rosalind High Performance Computing Infrastructure, King's College London.

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics

Characteristics of the 36 individuals who took part in the study are presented in Table 1. Six participants were within the first 5 years of their first episode of psychosis and therefore under the care of early intervention mental health services, while the remaining participants had a more established illness. Thirty-five out of 36 participants were prescribed regular antipsychotic medication, of which nine (25%) were taking clozapine for treatment-resistant psychosis, nine (25%) were prescribed more than one antipsychotic and seven (19%) were receiving depot medication. The ISI score for 11 (31%) participants was within the range of clinically significant insomnia (score of 15 or above), and the PSQI score indicated poor sleep quality (score of 5 or greater) for 26 (72%) participants.

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics on study entry (n = 36), and summary statistics of self-reported sleep variables (n = 33)

The mean (s.d.) duration of participation was 323 (88) days, number of submitted diaries per participant was 246 (136), and the overall response rate was 69%. Data from two participants who were in the study for <1 month, and one participant whose responses showed no variability, were excluded; therefore, data from 33 participants were entered into the final analysis.

Unmediated associations between sleep and psychopathology

Sleep quality negatively predicted psychosis symptoms over lags of 1–8 days (i.e. better sleep quality was associated with lower psychosis symptoms over an 8-day horizon, and vice versa). It also negatively predicted negative affect over lags of 1–8 and 10–11 days, and cognitive symptoms over lags of 1–8 days. Conversely, sleep quality positively predicted positive affect over 1–5 days (i.e. better sleep quality was associated with greater positive affect, over a 5-day window). Effect sizes were in the medium range.

Sleep duration negatively predicted psychosis symptoms over lags of 1–12 days, negative affect over lags of 1–8 days, and cognitive symptoms over lags of 1, 3–12 and 14–15 days. No association between sleep duration and positive affect was found (Table 2, top).

Table 2. Self-reported sleep variables: lag relationships between single predictor–outcome pairs

n.s., no significant lag identified.

a Sum of β coefficients over all lags.

Reciprocal associations between psychopathology and sleep variables

Examining the opposite causal direction, statistically significant negative associations of smaller effect size were found between psychosis symptoms and sleep quality (over lags of 1–3 days), negative affect and sleep quality (over lags of 1–3 and 5–6 days), and cognitive symptoms and sleep quality (with lags of 1–2 and 4–5 days). Positive affect predicted better sleep quality over lags of 1–3 and 6–7 days.

No associations were found between psychosis symptoms and sleep duration, positive or negative affect and sleep duration, or cognitive symptoms and sleep duration (Table 2, middle).

Unmediated associations between sleep and individual psychotic symptoms

Sleep quality had a negative relationship with both paranoia and hallucinations, over a lag of 1–5 days, in the low–medium effect size range. In the reciprocal direction, only paranoia, and not hallucinations, predicted sleep quality.

Sleep duration predicted hallucinations, but not paranoia, with lags of 2–3 and 5–9 days. No reciprocal association between individual psychotic symptoms and sleep duration was found.

Auto-regression analyses in sleep and psychopathology variables

Sleep quality predicted itself over lags of 1–5 days (i.e. better sleep quality was associated with better sleep quality over the next five nights), and sleep duration predicted itself over lags of 1–4, 6–8, 10, 14 and 19 days. Negative affect, positive affect, cognitive and psychosis symptoms predicted themselves with lags of 1–4, 1–3, 1–5 and 1–6 days, respectively, with effect sizes in the medium–large range (Table 2, bottom).

Mediated associations between sleep and psychopathology

Results of mediation analyses are set out in Table 3 and explained in greater detail in online Supplementary data and Figs S1–S4. In all cases, models where negative affect and cognitive symptoms mediated the relationship between sleep quality/sleep duration and psychosis symptoms were favoured over unmediated relationships. Negative affect was a full mediator of the effect between sleep duration and psychosis symptoms, and a partial mediator of the effect between sleep quality and psychosis symptoms. Cognitive symptoms were a partial mediator of the effect between sleep quality and sleep duration, and psychosis symptoms.

Table 3. Mediated lag relationships and effect sizes

n.s., no significant lag identified.

Total, direct and indirect effects are calculated from the sum of β coefficients over all lags. Refer to online Supplementary Figs S1–S4 for path diagrams.

Oppositional models where sleep variables mediated the relationship between negative affect/cognitive symptoms and psychosis symptoms either showed no evidence of an indirect pathway or showed poorer model fit, than the non-mediated total effect, arguing against an indirect relationship mediated by sleep duration or sleep quality.

Discussion

Main findings

Sleep disruption has traditionally been considered a consequence of psychosis, arising as the product of mental state disturbance. The possibility that it may play a causal role in the emergence of psychotic symptoms has received support only more recently, through studies in clinical (Kasanova et al., Reference Kasanova, Hajdúk, Thewissen and Myin-Germeys2020; Mulligan et al., Reference Mulligan, Haddock, Emsley, Neil and Kyle2016; Reeve et al., Reference Reeve, Nickless, Sheaves and Freeman2018; Waters et al., Reference Waters, Sinclair, Rock, Jablensky, Foster and Wulff2011) and non-clinical populations (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Sheaves, Goodwin, Yu, Nickless, Harrison and Espie2017; Hennig, Schlier, & Lincoln, Reference Hennig, Schlier and Lincoln2019; Reeve, Emsley, Sheaves, & Freeman, Reference Reeve, Emsley, Sheaves and Freeman2018; Scott, Rowse, & Webb, Reference Scott, Rowse and Webb2017). Using a purpose-built digital platform and novel analytic approach, our findings extend those of recent studies, by determining the temporal relationship over which sleep and psychopathology variables influence and sustain one another.

First, these results support the hypothesis that poorer sleep quality and sleep duration are an antecedent to worsening psychotic symptoms, occurring up to 8 and 12 days prior to the deterioration in mental state, respectively. Poorer sleep quality and duration also anticipate worsening in negative affect and cognitive symptoms, from 1 to 15 days before the deterioration in mood and cognition. Similarly, better sleep quality (but not sleep duration) predicted greater positive affect, but with a shorter lag duration of up to 5 days.

Second, we also found evidence of a reciprocal relationship, where greater psychopathology predicted poorer sleep quality (but interestingly, not sleep duration), and greater positive affect predicted better sleep quality. As advanced in previous reports (Harvey, Murray, Chandler, & Soehner, Reference Harvey, Murray, Chandler and Soehner2011; Talbot et al., Reference Talbot, Stone, Gruber, Hairston, Eidelman and Harvey2012), this supports the concept of a bidirectional, mutually reinforcing association between sleep quality and psychopathology. Importantly, however, with the exception of positive affect, where the reciprocal association was of longer duration, the lags of reciprocal associations for all other variables were only between 1 and 6 days and of smaller effect size, suggesting that sleep has a more durable and pronounced effect on psychopathology than the opposite direction of the association. This finding is compatible with other studies, which found unidirectional (Kasanova et al., Reference Kasanova, Hajdúk, Thewissen and Myin-Germeys2020) or only partially reciprocal (Hennig et al., Reference Hennig, Schlier and Lincoln2019; Reeve et al., Reference Reeve, Nickless, Sheaves and Freeman2018) relationships between psychopathology and sleep variables.

It is notable that bidirectional associations were found for sleep quality, but not duration. This provides support to the argument that changes in the quantity and continuity of sleep drive the development of mental symptoms, but are in themselves relatively unaffected by mental state fluctuations. One factor that may contribute to this finding is the use of sedative antipsychotic medications in all but one of the participants, which have been shown to increase sleep duration and reduce sleep latency (Cohrs, Reference Cohrs2008; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Faulkner, McCutcheon, Pillinger, Dijk and MacCabe2020), thereby dampening the impact of changes in psychopathology on sleep duration. Another explanation for this finding is that the subjective evaluation of sleep duration is less affected by past mental state, whereas sleep quality is more contingent on an individual's sense of well-being. That is, sleep quality is more closely ‘tied’ to psychopathology than is sleep duration.

Third, the mediation analyses provided stronger support for models where negative affect and cognitive symptoms mediated the relationship between sleep quality/sleep duration and psychosis symptoms, than unmediated and oppositional models, where sleep variables were the mediator. This replicates previous findings (Kasanova et al., Reference Kasanova, Hajdúk, Thewissen and Myin-Germeys2020; Reeve et al., Reference Reeve, Nickless, Sheaves and Freeman2018; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Rowse and Webb2017), and reinforces the view that affective and cognitive processes lie on the pathway to psychotic symptoms. In exploring the relationship with individual psychotic symptoms, a bidirectional relationship between sleep quality and paranoia, and unidirectional association between sleep quality and hallucinations, was found. Also, changes in sleep duration influenced hallucinations, but not paranoia. This also accords with previous findings, and supports the hypothesis that the pathway between sleep disturbances and paranoia may differ from those linking sleep disturbances and hallucinations (Hennig et al., Reference Hennig, Schlier and Lincoln2019; Reeve et al., Reference Reeve, Nickless, Sheaves and Freeman2018; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Rowse and Webb2017).

Clinical implications and mechanisms

As conceived in the criteria of Bradford Hill (Bradford, Reference Bradford1965), temporality and strength of association are key tenets for inferring a causal relationship between a predictor and outcome. In this study, the longer lag duration and larger effect size when sleep variables are predictors, rather than outcome variables, suggest that sleep disturbance emerges earlier in the pathway to deterioration of psychosis, and supports a causal role for sleep disturbance in the development of psychopathology. A corollary of this is that subjective sleep disturbances may serve as a clinically useful early warning sign of impending deterioration, and allow the implementation of timely, preventative interventions. Our findings suggest that a reduction in sleep duration precedes psychotic deterioration by up to 12 days, suggesting that this is a particularly important symptom in this regard. A further implication is that psychological and pharmacological interventions which specifically target sleep dysfunction may diminish psychopathology and risk of relapse – a strategy which has gained recent experimental support (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Sheaves, Goodwin, Yu, Nickless, Harrison and Espie2017).

Another essential criterion for inferring causality is the presence of plausible mechanisms that link predictor and outcome variables. Experimental sleep deprivation in healthy volunteers induces depressed mood and anxiety, executive function and working memory impairment, perceptual distortions and hallucinations, and paranoia, with particularly strong associations with mood and cognitive variables (Kahn-Greene, Killgore, Kamimori, Balkin, & Killgore, Reference Kahn-Greene, Killgore, Kamimori, Balkin and Killgore2007; Petrovsky et al., Reference Petrovsky, Ettinger, Hill, Frenzel, Meyhofer, Wagner and Kumari2014; Reeve et al., Reference Reeve, Emsley, Sheaves and Freeman2018; Waters, Chiu, Atkinson, & Blom, Reference Waters, Chiu, Atkinson and Blom2018). These effects may be mediated by multiple biological mechanisms, including increased dopamine release and sensitivity (Yates, Reference Yates2016), impaired sleep-dependent plasticity (Pocivavsek & Rowland, Reference Pocivavsek and Rowland2018) and disrupted synaptic homeostasis (Cirelli & Tononi, Reference Cirelli and Tononi2015). These findings also align with theoretical frameworks which implicate negative affect, worry and rumination (Hartley, Haddock, Vasconcelos e Sa, Emsley, & Barrowclough, Reference Hartley, Haddock, Vasconcelos e Sa, Emsley and Barrowclough2014), and cognitive impairment (Garety et al., Reference Garety, Joyce, Jolley, Emsley, Waller, Kuipers and Freeman2013) in driving psychotic symptoms – constructs which were also identified as mediators in this study. There is also growing interest in the role of immune dysregulation in the pathoaetiology of psychosis (Pillinger et al., Reference Pillinger, Osimo, Brugger, Mondelli, McCutcheon and Howes2018), which converges with evidence suggesting that impaired sleep plays a causal role in the development of metabolic and cardiovascular disease (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Salo, Lange, Jennum, Virtanen, Pentti and Vahtera2016), mediated by inflammatory mechanisms (Irwin, Olmstead, & Carroll, Reference Irwin, Olmstead and Carroll2016; Mullington, Simpson, Meier-Ewert, & Haack, Reference Mullington, Simpson, Meier-Ewert and Haack2010).

Strengths and limitations

A key feature of this study is its use of novel approaches for sampling and analysing intensive longitudinal data that goes beyond single-day lags, and allows the evaluation of longer lag structures between dynamic variables over ecologically valid timescales. There were high levels of engagement with the technology for such a study, yielding a high response rate and an unparalleled dataset.

However, it is important to note that DTVEM operates by examining relationships between multiple concurrent processes in relative time, rather than absolute time, and assumes that the relationship between covariates does not change as a function of study time. Related to this, deterioration in sleep and mental state may occur contemporaneously; however, it is possible the current study design lacks the sensitivity to detect more subtle, early shifts in psychopathology. Also, DTVEM remains a novel method, which requires further validation and replication. The study was promoted as one which examined sleep, meaning that individuals with sleep disorders may have been over-represented in our sample. However, rates of insomnia in this study were comparable to those published elsewhere (Palmese et al., Reference Palmese, DeGeorge, Ratliff, Srihari, Wexler, Krystal and Tek2011; Xiang et al., Reference Xiang, Weng, Leung, Tang, Lai and Ungvari2009). We cannot exclude the possibility that the response rate to the daily diary varies with symptom severity at baseline, and also with longitudinal changes in mental state, leading to non-response biases. Finally, given the limitations inherent to self-report measures, including noise and non-response, further examination of the nature of sleep disturbance using objective measures and a better understanding of how these measures relate to sleep quality (Della Monica, Johnsen, Atzori, Groeger, & Dijk, Reference Della Monica, Johnsen, Atzori, Groeger and Dijk2018) are warranted.

Conclusions

Deterioration in sleep quality and sleep duration predicts worsening of psychosis symptoms from up to 8 and 12 days in advance, respectively, and is mediated by negative affect and cognitive symptoms. Psychopathology variables have a reciprocal predictive effect on sleep quality, supporting a bidirectional relationship, but is generally weaker and of shorter duration.

These findings support the conclusion that sleep disturbance plays a causal role in symptom exacerbation and relapse in psychosis, and suggests it warrants greater attention as an early warning sign of deterioration.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720004857

Acknowledgements

The valuable contributions of the participants and their clinical teams are gratefully acknowledged. The authors also thank the Clintouch study investigators (University of Manchester) for sharing their validated questionnaire items. The authors are grateful to Dr Ian Grant, Department of Biostatistics and Health Informatics, King's College London, for support with the High Performance Computing Cluster. The authors acknowledge the use of the research computing facility at King's College London, Rosalind (https://rosalind.kcl.ac.uk), which is delivered in partnership with the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centres at South London & Maudsley and Guy's & St. Thomas' NHS Foundation Trusts, and part-funded by capital equipment grants from the Maudsley Charity (award 980) and Guy's & St. Thomas' Charity (TR130505).

Author contributions

NM designed and conducted the study, analysed data and wrote the manuscript; DWJ and NCJ contributed to the analysis and presentation of data; CK developed the data collection platform; DJD, MdV and JHM provided oversight in study design and conduct. All authors contributed to drafting and editing the manuscript.

Financial support

NM is supported by the UK Medical Research Council (grant no. MR/P 001378/1). DWJ is funded by the NIHR Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre (grant no. BRC-1215-20005). DJD is supported by the UK Dementia Research Institute. This study represents an independent research part funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King's College London. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research. Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 11731182. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bastien, C. H., Vallières, A., & Morin, C. M. (2001). Validation of the insomnia severity index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Medicine, 2, 297307. doi: 10.1016/S1389-9457(00)00065-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biagianti, B., Hidalgo-Mazzei, D., & Meyer, N. (2017). Developing digital interventions for people living with serious mental illness: Perspectives from three mHealth studies. Evidence-Based Mental Health, 20, 98101. doi: 10.1136/eb-2017-102765CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birchwood, M., Smith, J., Macmillan, F., Hogg, B., Prasad, R., Harvey, C., & Bering, S. (1989). Predicting relapse in schizophrenia the development and implementation of an early signs monitoring system using patients and families as observers, a preliminary investigation*. Psychological Medicine, 19, 649656. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700024247CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradford, A. (1965). The environment and disease: Association or causation? Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine, 58(5), 295300.Google Scholar
Buysse, D. J., Reynolds, C. F., Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R., & Kupfer, D. J. (1989). The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Research, 28, 193213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cirelli, C., & Tononi, G. (2015). Sleep and synaptic homeostasis. Sleep, 38, 161162. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4348CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. J., Salo, P., Lange, T., Jennum, P., Virtanen, M., Pentti, J., … Vahtera, J. (2016). Onset of impaired sleep and cardiovascular disease risk factors: A longitudinal study. Sleep, 39, 17091718. doi: 10.5665/sleep.6098CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohrs, S. (2008). Sleep disturbances in patients with schizophrenia: Impact and effect of antipsychotics. CNS Drugs, 22, 939962. doi: 10.2165/00023210-200822110-00004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Della Monica, C., Johnsen, S., Atzori, G., Groeger, J. A., & Dijk, D. J. (2018). Rapid eye movement sleep, sleep continuity and slow wave sleep as predictors of cognition, mood, and subjective sleep quality in healthy men and women, aged 20-84 years. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, 117. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, D., Sheaves, B., Goodwin, G. M., Yu, L. M., Nickless, A., Harrison, P. J., … Espie, C. A. (2017). The effects of improving sleep on mental health (OASIS): A randomised controlled trial with mediation analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 4, 749758. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30328-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garety, P., Joyce, E., Jolley, S., Emsley, R., Waller, H., Kuipers, E., … Freeman, D. (2013). Neuropsychological functioning and jumping to conclusions in delusions. Schizophrenia Research, 150, 570574. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.08.035CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartley, S., Haddock, G., Vasconcelos e Sa, D., Emsley, R., & Barrowclough, C. (2014). An experience sampling study of worry and rumination in psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 44, 16051614. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002080CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, A. G., Murray, G., Chandler, R. A., & Soehner, A. (2011). Sleep disturbance as transdiagnostic: Consideration of neurobiological mechanisms. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 225235. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heinrichs, D. W., & Carpenter, W. T. (1985). Prospective study of prodromal symptoms in schizophrenic relapse. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 371373.Google ScholarPubMed
Hennig, T., Schlier, B., & Lincoln, T. M. (2019). Sleep and psychotic symptoms: An actigraphy and diary study with young adults with low and elevated psychosis proneness. Schizophrenia Research, 221, 1219. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.09.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herz, M. I., & Melville, C. (1980). Relapse in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 801805. doi: 10.1176/ajp.137.7.801Google Scholar
Irwin, M. R., Olmstead, R., & Carroll, J. E. (2016). Sleep disturbance, sleep duration, and inflammation: A systematic review and meta-analysis of cohort studies and experimental sleep deprivation. Biological Psychiatry, 80, 4052. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.05.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobson, N. C., Chow, S. M., & Newman, M. G. (2019). The Differential Time-Varying Effect Model (DTVEM): A tool for diagnosing and modeling time lags in intensive longitudinal data. Behavior Research Methods, 51, 295315. doi: 10.3758/s13428-018-1101-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorgensen, P. (1998). Early signs of psychotic relapse in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 327330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahn-Greene, E. T., Killgore, D. B., Kamimori, G. H., Balkin, T. J., & Killgore, W. D. (2007). The effects of sleep deprivation on symptoms of psychopathology in healthy adults. Sleep Medicine, 8, 215221. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2006.08.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karr, C. (2017). URL: Audacious technologies code for sleepsight app. Retrieved from https://github.com/audaciouscode/sleepsight.Google Scholar
Kasanova, Z., Hajdúk, M., Thewissen, V., & Myin-Germeys, I. (2020). Temporal associations between sleep quality and paranoia across the paranoia continuum: An experience sampling study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 129, 122130. doi: 10.1037/abn0000453CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leth-Steensen, C., & Gallitto, E. (2016). Testing mediation in structural equation modeling: The effectiveness of the test of joint significance. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 76, 339351. doi: 10.1177/0013164415593777CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, N., Faulkner, S. M., McCutcheon, R. A., Pillinger, T., Dijk, D.-J., & MacCabe, J. H. (2020). Sleep and circadian rhythm disturbance in remitted schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 46(5), 11261143. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbaa024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, N., Kerz, M., Folarin, A., Joyce, D. W., Jackson, R., Karr, C., … Maccabe, J. (2018). Capturing rest-activity profiles in schizophrenia using wearable and mobile technologies: Development, implementation, feasibility, and acceptability of a remote monitoring platform. JMIR mHealth and uHealth, 6(10), e188. doi: 10.2196/mhealth.8292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulligan, L. D., Haddock, G., Emsley, R., Neil, S. T., & Kyle, S. D. (2016). High resolution examination of the role of sleep disturbance in predicting functioning and psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia: A novel experience sampling study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125, 788797. doi: 10.1037/abn0000180CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mullington, J. M., Simpson, N. S., Meier-Ewert, H. K., & Haack, M. (2010). Sleep loss and inflammation. Best Practice and Research: Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 24, 775784. doi: 10.1016/j.beem.2010.08.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myin-Germeys, I., Van Os, J., Schwartz, J. E., Stone, A. A., & Delespaul, P. A. (2001). Emotional reactivity to daily life stress in psychosis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 11371144. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.12.1137CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neale, M. C., Hunter, M. D., Pritikin, J. N., Zahery, M., Brick, T. R., Kirkpatrick, R. M., … Boker, S. M. (2016). Openmx 2.0: Extended structural equation and statistical modeling. Psychometrika, 81, 535549. doi: 10.1007/s11336-014-9435-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmese, L. B., DeGeorge, P. C., Ratliff, J. C., Srihari, V. H., Wexler, B. E., Krystal, A. D., & Tek, C. (2011). Insomnia is frequent in schizophrenia and associated with night eating and obesity. Schizophrenia Research, 133, 238243. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.07.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmier-Claus, J. E., Ainsworth, J., Machin, M., Barrowclough, C., Dunn, G., Barkus, E., … Lewis, S. W. (2012). The feasibility and validity of ambulatory self-report of psychotic symptoms using a smartphone software application. BMC Psychiatry, 12, 10. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-12-172CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmier-Claus, J. E., Myin-Germeys, I., Barkus, E., Bentley, L., Udachina, A., Delespaul, P. A., … Dunn, G. (2011). Experience sampling research in individuals with mental illness: Reflections and guidance. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 123, 1220. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01596.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peeters, F., Berkhof, J., Delespaul, P., Rottenberg, J., & Nicolson, N. A. (2006). Diurnal mood variation in major depressive disorder. Emotion, 6, 383391. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.383CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrovsky, N., Ettinger, U., Hill, A., Frenzel, L., Meyhofer, I., Wagner, M., … Kumari, V. (2014). Sleep deprivation disrupts prepulse inhibition and induces psychosis-like symptoms in healthy humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 91349140. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0904-14.2014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pillinger, T., Osimo, E. F., Brugger, S., Mondelli, V., McCutcheon, R. A., & Howes, O. D. (2018). A meta-analysis of immune parameters, variability, and assessment of modal distribution in psychosis and test of the immune subgroup hypothesis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 45, 114. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sby160Google Scholar
Pocivavsek, A., & Rowland, L. M. (2018). Basic neuroscience illuminates causal relationship between sleep and memory: Translating to schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 44, 714. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx151CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Reeve, S., Emsley, R., Sheaves, B., & Freeman, D. (2018). Disrupting sleep: The effects of sleep loss on psychotic experiences tested in an experimental study with mediation analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 44(3), 662671. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx103.Google Scholar
Reeve, S., Nickless, A., Sheaves, B., & Freeman, D. (2018). Insomnia, negative affect, and psychotic experiences: Modelling pathways over time in a clinical observational study. Psychiatry Research, 269, 673680. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.08.090CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, A. J., Rowse, G., & Webb, T. L. (2017). A structural equation model of the relationship between insomnia, negative affect, and paranoid thinking. PLoS ONE, 12, 123. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0186233CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talbot, L. S., Stone, S., Gruber, J., Hairston, I. S., Eidelman, P., & Harvey, A. G. (2012). A test of the bidirectional association between sleep and mood in bipolar disorder and insomnia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 3950. doi: 10.1037/a0024946CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waters, F., Chiu, V., Atkinson, A., & Blom, J. D. (2018). Severe sleep deprivation causes hallucinations and a gradual progression toward psychosis with increasing time awake. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, 303. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waters, F., Sinclair, C., Rock, D., Jablensky, A., Foster, R. G., & Wulff, K. (2011). Daily variations in sleep-wake patterns and severity of psychopathology: A pilot study in community-dwelling individuals with chronic schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 187, 304306. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.01.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, S. N. (2011). Fast stable restricted maximum likelihood and marginal likelihood estimation of semiparametric generalized linear models. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society (B), 73, 336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xiang, Y. T., Weng, Y. Z., Leung, C. M., Tang, W. K., Lai, K. Y., & Ungvari, G. S. (2009). Prevalence and correlates of insomnia and its impact on quality of life in Chinese schizophrenia patients. Sleep, 32, 105109.Google ScholarPubMed
Yates, N. J. (2016). Schizophrenia: The role of sleep and circadian rhythms in regulating dopamine and psychosis. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 27, 669687. https://dx.doi.org/10.1515/revneuro-2016-0030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. (a) Simulated time-series for one participant. An increase in the severity of symptom A appears to precede that of symptom B, and the temporal relationship is suggestive of A driving B. DTVEM looks for the same pattern across the entire time-series, for all participants. (b) Lagged regression between the two symptom time-series produces two coefficients, A to B and B to A. Here, symptom A maximally predicts symptom B over a threshold of statistical significance, with a lag of 3–17 days. However, symptom B does not significantly predict symptom A.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Screenshots of Sleepsight user-facing app: (a) splash page, (b) sleep diary, (c) mood symptoms, (d) psychosis symptoms. Note that only a section of screens (bd) are visible.

Figure 2

Fig. 3. Example responses over 12 months from one participant, fitted with a loess smoother with degree 1 polynomial. Higher scores indicate better sleep quality and positive affect, longer sleep duration, and worse negative affect and psychosis symptoms.

Figure 3

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics on study entry (n = 36), and summary statistics of self-reported sleep variables (n = 33)

Figure 4

Table 2. Self-reported sleep variables: lag relationships between single predictor–outcome pairs

Figure 5

Table 3. Mediated lag relationships and effect sizes

Supplementary material: PDF

Meyer et al. supplementary material

Meyer et al. supplementary material

Download Meyer et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 121.9 KB