Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T06:26:59.088Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Situational versus pervasive hyperactivity in a community sample

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

T. P. Ho*
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychiatry and Statistics, University of Hong Kong; Department of Psychology, Chinese University of Hong Kong; Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne, Australia; and MRC Child Psychiatry Unit, Institute of Psychiatry, London
E. S. L. Luk
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychiatry and Statistics, University of Hong Kong; Department of Psychology, Chinese University of Hong Kong; Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne, Australia; and MRC Child Psychiatry Unit, Institute of Psychiatry, London
P. W. L. Leung
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychiatry and Statistics, University of Hong Kong; Department of Psychology, Chinese University of Hong Kong; Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne, Australia; and MRC Child Psychiatry Unit, Institute of Psychiatry, London
E. Taylor
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychiatry and Statistics, University of Hong Kong; Department of Psychology, Chinese University of Hong Kong; Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne, Australia; and MRC Child Psychiatry Unit, Institute of Psychiatry, London
F. Lieh-Mak
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychiatry and Statistics, University of Hong Kong; Department of Psychology, Chinese University of Hong Kong; Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne, Australia; and MRC Child Psychiatry Unit, Institute of Psychiatry, London
J. Bacon-Shone
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychiatry and Statistics, University of Hong Kong; Department of Psychology, Chinese University of Hong Kong; Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne, Australia; and MRC Child Psychiatry Unit, Institute of Psychiatry, London
*
1Address for correspondence: Dr T. P. Ho, Department of Psychiatry, Queen Mary Hospital, University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam Road, Hong Kong.

Synopsis

Groups of home and school situational hyperactive primary schoolboys identified from the community were compared with pervasive hyperactive and non-hyperactive controls on a wide range of measures. The hyperactive groups tended to persist in the same category over a half-year period. Both situational hyperactive groups had lower measured activity levels than the pervasive hyperactive group and only the latter differed from non-hyperactive controls. Home hyperactivity was characterized by poor family relationships and was not distinguishable from non-hyperactive home-antisocial controls. School hyperactive boys had specific correlates of low intelligence, motor clumsiness, poor reading and academic abilities. Pervasive hyperactive subjects differed from both situational groups in showing a higher percentage of delayed language development. While home hyperactivity has dubious identity, the distinct pattern of external correlates in school and pervasive hyperactivity speak for the need to regard these as separate entities.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H. & Howell, C. T. (1987). Child/adolescent behavioral and emotional problems: implications of cross-informant correlations for situational specificity. Psychological Bulletin 101, 213232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (1980). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd edn. (DSM-III). American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (1987). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd edn. Revised (DSM-III-R). American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (1993). DSM-IV Draft Criteria. Task Force on DSM-IV, American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
August, G. J. & Garfinkel, B. D. (1989). Behavioral and cognitive subtypes of ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 28, 739748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, S. J., Young, G. H. & Tarnowski, K. J. (1990). Maternal characteristics and perceptions of pervasive and situational hyperactives and normal controls. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 29, 558565.Google Scholar
Biederman, J., Keenan, K. & Faraone, S. V. (1990). Parent-based diagnosis of attention deficit disorder predicts a diagnosis based on teacher report. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 29, 698701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boudreault, M., Thivierge, J., Cote, R., Boutin, P., Julien, Y. & Bergeron, S. (1988). Cognitive development and reading achievement in pervasive-ADD. situational-ADD and control children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 29, 611619.Google Scholar
Cairns, E. & Cammock, T. (1978). Development of a more reliable version of the matching familiar figure test. Developmental Psychology 14, 556560.Google Scholar
Chee, P., Logan, G., Schachar, R., Lindsay, P. & Wachsmuth, R. (1989). Effects of event rate and display time on sustained attention in hyperactive, normal and control children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 17, 371391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, N. J. & Minde, K. (1983). The ‘hyperactive syndrome’ in kindergarten children: comparison of children with pervasive and situational symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 24, 443455.Google Scholar
Costello, E. J., Loeber, R. & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1991). Pervasive and situational hyperactivity-confounding effect of informant: a research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 32, 367376.Google Scholar
Ekblad, S. (1990). The children's behaviour questionnaire for completion by parents and teachers in a Chinese sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 31, 775791.Google Scholar
Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L. & Cornblatt, B. (1978). Attentional measures in a study of children at high-risk for schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research 14, 9398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fergusson, D. M. & Horwood, L. J. (1992). Attention deficit and reading achievement. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 33, 375386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R. & Stevenson, J. (1989 a). A twin study of hyperactivity. I. An examination of hyperactivity scores and categories derived from Rutter Teacher and Parent Questionnaires. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 30, 671689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R. & Stevenson, J. (1989 b). A twin study of hyperactivity. II. The aetiological role of genes, family relationships and perinatal adversity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 30, 691709.Google Scholar
Gubbay, S. S. (1975). The Clumsy Child: A Study of Developmental Apraxia and Agnosic Ataxia. Saunders: London.Google Scholar
Lee, P. L. M., Priester, H. J. & Yung, G. Y. M. (1983). Factor analysis and construct validity of the Hong Kong Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (HK-WISC). (Unpublished manuscript.)Google Scholar
Leung, P. W. L. & Luk, S. L. (1988). Differences in attention control between ‘clinic-observable’ and ‘reported’ hyperactivity: a preliminary report. Child: Care, Health and Development 14, 199211.Google Scholar
Luk, S. L., Leung, P. W. L. & Yuen, J. (1991). Clinic observations in the assessment of pervasiveness of childhood hyperactivity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 32, 833850.Google Scholar
McGee, R. & Share, D. L. (1988). Attention deficit disorder – hyperactivity and academic failure: which comes first and what should be treated? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 27, 318325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGee, R., Silva, P. A. & Williams, S. (1984 a). Behaviour problems in a population of seven-year-old children: prevalence, stability and types of disorder – a research report. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 25, 251259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGee, R., Williams, S. & Silva, P. A. (1984 b). Behaviour and developmental characteristics of aggressive, hyperactive and aggressive–hyperactive boys. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry 23, 270279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGee, R., Williams, S., Share, D. L., Anderson, J. & Silva, P. A. (1986). The relationship between specific reading retardation, general reading backwardness and behavioural problems in a large sample of Dunedin boys: a longitudinal study from five to eleven years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 27, 597610.Google Scholar
Matsuura, M., Okubo, Y., Kojima, T., Takahashi, R., Wang, Y. F., Shen, Y. C. & Lee, C. K. (1993). A cross-national prevalence study of children with emotional and behavioural problems – a WHO collaborative study in the Western Pacific region. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 34, 307316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pastore, R. A. & Scheirer, C. J. (1974). Signal detection theory: consideration for general application. Psychological Bulletin 81, 945958.Google Scholar
Quinton, D., Rutter, M. & Liddle, C. (1984). Institutional rearing, parenting difficulties and marital support. Psychological Medicine 14, 107124.Google Scholar
Rapoport, J. L., Donnelly, M., Zametkin, A. & Carrougher, J. (1986). ‘Situational hyperactivity’ in a US clinical setting. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 27, 639646.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowe, K. J. & Rowe, K. S. (1992). The relationship between inattentiveness in the classroom and reading achievement (Part B): an explanatory study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 31, 357368.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (1982). Syndromes attributed to ‘minimal brain dysfunction’ in childhood. American Journal of Psychiatry 139, 2133.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., Tizard, J. & Whitmore, K. (1970). Education, Health and Behaviour. Longman: London.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., Yule, W., Berger, M., Yule, B., Morton, J. & Bagley, C. (1974). Children of West Indian immigrants. I. Rates of behavioural deviance and of psychiatric disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 15, 241262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandberg, S. T. (1986). Overactivity: behaviour or syndrome? In Overactive Child (ed. Taylor, E.), pp. 4172. MacKeith Press Blackwell Scientific: London.Google Scholar
Sandberg, S. T., Rutter, M. & Taylor, E. (1978). Hyperkinetic disorder in psychiatric clinic attenders. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 20, 279299.Google Scholar
Sandberg, S. T., Wieselberg, M. & Shaffer, D. (1980). Hyperkinetic and conduct problem children in a primary school population: some epidemiological implications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 21, 293311.Google Scholar
Schachar, R., Rutter, M. & Smith, A. (1981). The characteristics of situationally and pervasively hyperactive children: implications for syndrome definition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 22, 375392.Google Scholar
Schleifer, M., Weiss, G., Cohen, N., Elman, M., Cvejic, H. & Kruger, E. (1975). Hyperactivity in preschoolers and the effect of methylphenidate. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 45, 3850.Google Scholar
Szatmari, P., Boyle, M. & Offord, D. (1989). ADDH and conduct disorder: degree of diagnostic overlap and differences among correlates. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 28, 865872.Google Scholar
Szatmari, P., Offord, D. R., Siegel, L. S., Finlayson, M. A. & Tuff, L. (1990). The clinical significance of neurocognitive impairments among children with psychiatric disorders: diagnosis and situational specificity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 31, 287299.Google Scholar
Taylor, E., Everitt, B., Thorley, G., Schachar, R., Rutter, M. & Weiselberg, M. (1986). Conduct disorder and hyperactivity. II. A cluster analytic approach to the identification of a behavioural syndrome. British Journal of Psychiatry 149, 768777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, E., Sandberg, S., Thorley, G. & Giles, S. (1991). The Epidemiology of Childhood Hyperactivity. Institute of Psychiatry Maudsley Monographs (33). Oxford University Press: London.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (1992). The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders. World Health Organization: Geneva.Google Scholar
Yule, W. & Taylor, E. (1987). Classification of soft signs. In Soft Neurological Signs (ed. Tupper, D. E.), pp. 1943. Grune & Stratton: Orlando.Google Scholar