Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T01:05:02.306Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The silent epidemic of loneliness: identifying the antecedents of loneliness using a lagged exposure-wide approach

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 March 2024

Joanna H. Hong*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada Human Flourishing Program, Institute for Quantitative Social Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
Julia S. Nakamura
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
Sakshi S. Sahakari
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
William J. Chopik
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, USA
Koichiro Shiba
Affiliation:
Human Flourishing Program, Institute for Quantitative Social Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA Department of Epidemiology, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, MA, USA
Tyler J. VanderWeele
Affiliation:
Human Flourishing Program, Institute for Quantitative Social Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA Department of Epidemiology, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, MA, USA Department of Biostatistics, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, MA, USA
Eric S. Kim
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada Human Flourishing Program, Institute for Quantitative Social Science, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA Lee Kum Sheung Center for Health and Happiness, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Boston, MA, USA
*
Corresponding author: Joanna H. Hong; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

A large and accumulating body of evidence shows that loneliness is detrimental for various health and well-being outcomes. However, less is known about potentially modifiable factors that lead to decreased loneliness.

Methods

We used data from the Health and Retirement Study to prospectively evaluate a wide array of candidate predictors of subsequent loneliness. Importantly, we examined if changes in 69 physical-, behavioral-, and psychosocial-health factors (from t0;2006/2008 to t1;2010/2012) were associated with subsequent loneliness 4 years later (t2;2014/2016).

Results

Adjusting for a large range of covariates, changes in certain health behaviors (e.g. increased physical activity), physical health factors (e.g. fewer functioning limitations), psychological factors (e.g. increased purpose in life, decreased depression), and social factors (e.g. greater number of close friends) were associated with less subsequent loneliness.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest that subjective ratings of physical and psychological health and perceived social environment (e.g. chronic pain, self-rated health, purpose in life, anxiety, neighborhood cohesion) are more strongly associated with subsequent loneliness. Yet, objective ratings (e.g. specific chronic health conditions, living status) show less evidence of associations with subsequent loneliness. The current study identified potentially modifiable predictors of subsequent loneliness that may be important targets for interventions aimed at reducing loneliness.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Loneliness, the subjective perception that one lacks social connection, is recognized as one of the most urgent public health threats in the United States for several reasons (Leigh-Hunt et al., Reference Leigh-Hunt, Bagguley, Bash, Turner, Turnbull, Valtorta and Caan2017; Rubin, Reference Rubin2017). First, a large and growing body of research has documented the detrimental effects of loneliness on a wide range of health and well-being outcomes (Perissinotto, Holt-Lunstad, Periyakoil, & Covinsky, Reference Perissinotto, Holt-Lunstad, Periyakoil and Covinsky2019; Perissinotto, Stijacic Cenzer, & Covinsky, Reference Perissinotto, Stijacic Cenzer and Covinsky2012). For example, a meta-analysis of 19 longitudinal studies found that greater loneliness is associated with greater incidence of subsequent dementia (Lara et al., Reference Lara, Martín-María, De la Torre-Luque, Koyanagi, Vancampfort, Izquierdo and Miret2019). Further, loneliness predicts premature mortality at rates comparable to traditional physical health risk factors such as smoking, obesity, and physical inactivity (Holt-Lunstad, Smith, & Layton, Reference Holt-Lunstad, Smith and Layton2010, Reference Holt-Lunstad, Smith, Baker, Harris and Stephenson2015). Loneliness may also influence various health outcomes through multiple pathways, including (1) psychological pathways (e.g. increased negative social expectations leading to behaviors that confirm these negative expectations from others such as physical distancing) (Newall et al., Reference Newall, Chipperfield, Clifton, Perry, Swift and Ruthig2009), (2) physiological pathways (e.g. increased sensitivity to stressors leading to maladaptive stress responses including greater inflammation, cardiovascular activation, and hyperactivity of the HPA axis) (Park et al., Reference Park, Majeed, Gill, Tamura, Ho, Mansur and McIntyre2020), and (3) behavioral pathways (e.g. decreased physical activity, health care use, and poorer sleep quality) (Hawkley & Cacioppo, Reference Hawkley and Cacioppo2010; Hom, Chu, Rogers, & Joiner, Reference Hom, Chu, Rogers and Joiner2020).

Second, contrary to popular belief, loneliness impacts individuals of all ages and is not limited to older adults (Victor & Yang, Reference Victor and Yang2012). According to a 2018 survey of over 20 000 US adults aged >18, nearly half of the population reported sometimes or always feeling alone or left out (Polack, Reference Polack2018). Third, loneliness has economic consequences. A report by the London School of Economics estimated that over a 10-year period, loneliness could add an additional >$2000 of healthcare costs per person when comparing lonely v. non-lonely older adults (McDaid, Bauer, & Park, Reference McDaid, Bauer and Park2017). They also note, how loneliness costs United Kingdom employers more than $3.5 billion dollars per year (Jeffrey, Abdallah, & Michaelson, Reference Jeffrey, Abdallah and Michaelson2017). Fourth, the COVID-19 pandemic and its accompanying physical distancing rules/recommendations have increased societal awareness about the pernicious effects of loneliness (Holt-Lunstad, Reference Holt-Lunstad2020).

With the aim of offsetting the troublesome realities that loneliness causes, social/health organizations, educational agencies, and governments worldwide are creating interventions and policies aimed at reducing loneliness. For example, the UK and Japan recently appointed their first ‘ministers of loneliness’ to combat loneliness at a national level (Fried et al., Reference Fried, Prohaska, Burholt, Burns, Golden, Hawkley and O'Sullivan2020), and a growing number of British doctors are now embracing ‘social prescription’ practices (i.e. writing prescriptions for patients to engage in social activities at subsidized prices) (Harris, Reference Harris2018). Similarly, China implemented a ‘must visit parents’ mandate as part of the new Elderly Rights Law, which requires adult children to visit their aging parents to stem the rising problem of loneliness (Hatton, Reference Hatton2013). In the US, the Surgeon General Vivek Murthy has repeatedly raised concerns about the potential increase in loneliness following the COVID-19 pandemic, which he termed as a ‘social recession’. Thus, identifying potential predictors that decrease loneliness can aid in this growing global effort. While an extensive literature has identified sociodemographic factors that predict subsequent loneliness (e.g. gender, age, income, widowed/divorced) (Cohen-Mansfield, Shmotkin, & Goldberg, Reference Cohen-Mansfield, Shmotkin and Goldberg2009; Dahlberg, Andersson, McKee, & Lennartsson, Reference Dahlberg, Andersson, McKee and Lennartsson2015; Franssen, Stijnen, Hamers, & Schneider, Reference Franssen, Stijnen, Hamers and Schneider2020; Victor & Bowling, Reference Victor and Bowling2012), accumulating evidence shows that loneliness is predicted by other potentially modifiable factors, including some health behaviors (e.g. poor sleep quality) (Jia & Yuan, Reference Jia and Yuan2020; Macià et al., Reference Macià, Cattaneo, Solana, Tormos, Pascual-Leone and Bartrés-Faz2021), indicators of physical health (e.g. self-rated health) (Swader, Reference Swader2019; Yang & Gu, Reference Yang and Gu2020), psychological factors (e.g. depression, negative affect) (Böger & Huxhold, Reference Böger and Huxhold2018; Dahlberg et al., Reference Dahlberg, Andersson, McKee and Lennartsson2015; Hsueh, Chen, Hsiao, & Lin, Reference Hsueh, Chen, Hsiao and Lin2019), and social factors (e.g. social support) (Mann et al., Reference Mann, Bone, Lloyd-Evans, Frerichs, Pinfold, Ma and Johnson2017). Yet, other research has yielded mixed findings. For example, while some studies find that greater physical activity is associated with lower loneliness (Kim, Lee, Chun, Han, & Heo, Reference Kim, Lee, Chun, Han and Heo2017), others find no associations (McAuley et al., Reference McAuley, Blissmer, Marquez, Jerome, Kramer and Katula2000; Tully et al., Reference Tully, McMullan, Blackburn, Wilson, Coll-Planas, Deidda and Rothenbacher2019).

These seminal studies have contributed greatly to the literature, yet, are limited. First, most prior research evaluates predictors of subsequent loneliness accumulated across the life-course and at one point in time, rather than evaluating how changes in predictors might influence changes in subsequent loneliness. Examining how changes in the predictors might influence changes in subsequent loneliness is desirable because it can strengthen causal inference by addressing confounding by all time-invariant individual characteristics, including unmeasured ones. Thus, answering the latter question produces results that are more useful for interventions (VanderWeele, Mathur, & Chen, Reference VanderWeele, Mathur and Chen2020). Second, most research only examined a limited range of predictors, which (1) limits the potential pool of building blocks for developing loneliness-reducing interventions and policies, and (2) makes it challenging to compare effect sizes across candidate predictors as potential intervention targets. Third, less is known about how initial experiences of loneliness (i.e. whether people are already experiencing loneliness or not) shape associations between predictors and subsequent loneliness. The regulatory loop model of loneliness (Hawkley & Cacioppo, Reference Hawkley and Cacioppo2010) indicates that lonely individuals show greater surveillance for and sensitivity to stressors (e.g. increased attention to threats, better memory of negative social experiences). This hypersensitivity leads to increased negative social expectations and elicits behaviors that confirm these negative expectations which, in turn, leads to greater loneliness. This self-reinforcing loop increases lonely individuals' susceptibility to various risk factors for loneliness.

In a nationally representative sample of adults aged >50, we used a lagged exposure-wide analytic approach (see Statistical analysis section) (VanderWeele et al., Reference VanderWeele, Mathur and Chen2020), to evaluate how changes in 69 predictors might lead to changes in subsequent loneliness 4 years later. The predictors were chosen for two reasons. First, they are commonly included in the conceptualization of key gerontological models that characterize the antecedents, processes, and outcomes that increase people's ability to age well (Aldwin & Igarashi, Reference Aldwin, Igarashi, Lichtenberg, Mast, Carpenter and Loebach Wetherell2015; Depp & Jeste, Reference Depp and Jeste2006; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Tkatch, Martin, MacLeod, Sandy and Yeh2021; Rowe & Kahn, Reference Rowe and Kahn1987; Ryff & Singer, Reference Ryff, Singer, Bengston, Gans, Pulney and Silverstein2009). Second, research indicates that several of our predictors can be modified through existing interventions and/or with further research.

Methods

Study population

Participants were from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS) – a nationally representative sample of adults aged >50 in the US. In 2006, approximately 50% of HRS respondents completed an enhanced face-to-face interview, whereas the other half of respondents were assessed in 2008. Following the interview, participants completed a psychosocial questionnaire which they mailed to the University of Michigan. Response rates were 88% in 2006 and 84% in 2008 (Smith, Ryan, Fisher, Sonnega, & Weir, Reference Smith, Ryan, Fisher, Sonnega and Weir2017). To increase sample size and statistical power, we combined data from 2006 and 2008 to create the pre-baseline wave. All study measures were comparable between the two cohorts. Because more than half of the study predictors were measured in the psychosocial questionnaire, participants were excluded if they did not report psychosocial data in this pre-baseline wave.

This study used three waves of data collected 4 years apart (t 0, pre-baseline; t 1, baseline; and t 2, outcome). All covariates were measured in the pre-baseline wave (t 0;2006/2008), candidate predictors were measured in the baseline wave (t 1;2010/2012), and the outcome (loneliness) was measured in the outcome wave (t 2;2014/2016). Study documentation can be found on the HRS website (http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/). The HRS is conducted by the University of Michigan and supported by the National Institute on Aging (NIA U01AG009740). Because we used de-identified and publicly available data, the ethics review board at the University of British Columbia exempted this study from human subjects review.

Measures

Loneliness

Loneliness was measured using a three-item loneliness scale, shortened from the 20-item Revised UCLA loneliness Scale (Russell, Peplau, & Cutrona, Reference Russell, Peplau and Cutrona1980). Participants indicated how much of the time they feel ‘lack of companionship’, ‘left out’, and ‘isolated from others’ on a three-point Likert scale (1 [often], 2 [some of the time], 3 [hardly ever or never]). Responses were reverse-coded, and all items were averaged to create a composite score (range: 1–3). Greater values indicated greater loneliness, and we standardized the variable (mean = 0, standard deviation = 1) so that the results can be interpreted as a standard deviation change in loneliness (α = 0.80).

Covariates

We included a large number of covariates that were measured in the pre-baseline wave (t 0;2006/2008). Covariates included sociodemographic factors (age, gender, race/ethnicity, marital status, annual household income [<$50 000, $50 000–$74 999, $75 000–$99 999, ⩾$100 000]), total wealth (based on quintiles of the score distribution for total wealth in this sample), educational attainment (no degree, GED/high school diploma, ⩾college degree), geographic region (Northeast, Midwest, South, West), personality (openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, neuroticism; continuous), and childhood physical abuse (yes/no). Pre-baseline values of all predictors (t 0;2006/2008) were also included as covariates to examine change in each predictor variable. We also adjusted for pre-baseline loneliness to reduce the possibility of reverse causation. The analyses can be interpreted as how a change in loneliness over 4 years, rather than prevalent loneliness, affects subsequent loneliness 4 years later; the effects of pre-baseline loneliness on subsequent loneliness is substantial but the relationship is not deterministic because loneliness may change due to a variety of factors.

Predictors

In the baseline wave (t 1;2010/2012), 69 candidate predictors were assessed including indicators of: health behaviors (binge drinking; smoking; physical activity; sleep problems), physical health (total number of chronic conditions; diabetes; hypertension; stroke; cancer; heart disease; lung disease; arthritis; overweight/obesity; physical functioning limitations; cognitive impairment; chronic pain; self-rated health; hearing; eyesight), psychological well-being (positive affect; life satisfaction; optimism; purpose in life; mastery; health mastery; financial mastery), psychological distress (depression; depressive symptoms; hopelessness; negative affect; perceived constraints; anxiety; trait anger; state anger; cynical hostility; stressful life events; financial strain; daily discrimination; major discrimination), and social factors (living alone; frequency of contact with children, other family, and friends; closeness with spouse; having any child, other family, and friends; number of close children, other family, and friends; positive social support from spouse, children, other family, and friends; negative social strain from spouse, children, other family, and friends; neighborhood cohesion; neighborhood disorder; social effort and reward; non-religious social activity participation; volunteer activity; religious service attendance; helping friends, neighbors, and relative social status ladder ranking; and change in social status ladder ranking), employment status, and health insurance. For further details about each of the covariates and predictor variables, see Supplementary Text 1 and HRS documentation (Fisher, Faul, Weir, & Wallace, Reference Fisher, Faul, Weir and Wallace2005; Jenkins, Ofstedal, & Weir, Reference Jenkins, Ofstedal and Weir2008; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Fisher, Ryan, Clarke, House and Weir2013).

Statistical analysis

We used a lagged exposure-wide analytic approach (VanderWeele et al., Reference VanderWeele, Mathur and Chen2020) and conducted separate analyses for each exposure (candidate predictor). Because loneliness was a continuous variable, we used linear regression to examine how each baseline (t 1;2010/2012) exposure variable (candidate predictor) was independently related to subsequent loneliness in the outcome wave (t 2:2014/2016) controlling for all pre-baseline variables (t 0;2006/2008). The outcome (subsequent loneliness) and all other continuous exposures (predictors) were standardized (mean = 0, standard deviation = 1) so that each effect estimate could be interpreted as a standard deviation change in the exposure. When considering categorical exposures, each effect estimate corresponds to associations between the exposure at baseline (t 1;2010/2012) subsequent loneliness in the outcome wave (t 2:2014/2016), conditional on the exposures and covariates in the pre-baseline wave (t 0;2006/2008). We used Bonferroni correction to account for multiple testing. In our tables, we marked multiple p value thresholds because different investigators often use different standards in interpreting evidence. For ease of reviewing results, the tables include p value thresholds of: p < 0.05, p < 0.01, or a Bonferroni correction to account for multiple testing (0.05/69 predictors = p < 0.0.00072463768). In our results section, we comment on traditional 0.05 p value threshold (without Bonferroni correction), but in all cases we also provide 95% confidence intervals which give what are often considered preferable assessments of uncertainty since all thresholds are ultimately arbitrary.

Additional analyses

We conducted four additional analyses. First, we conducted E-value analyses to examine the minimum strength of unmeasured confounding associations on the risk ratio scale (with both the exposure and subsequent loneliness) needed to explain away the association between the exposure and subsequent loneliness. This analysis was conducted to assess the robustness of our results to potential unmeasured confounding (VanderWeele & Ding, Reference VanderWeele and Ding2017). Second, we repeated these analyses restricting analytic samples to: (a) non-lonely people at baseline (composite loneliness score = 1, people whose composite score indicates experiencing loneliness ‘hardly ever or never’) and (b) lonely people at baseline (baseline composite loneliness score >1, participants whose composite scores indicate experiencing loneliness ‘some of the time’ or ‘often’). Third, we conducted these analyses again using only complete-cases to assess the impact of multiple imputation.

Missing data

To address potential selection bias due to missing data (Hernán, Hernández-Díaz, & Robins, Reference Hernán, Hernández-Díaz and Robins2004), we imputed all missing exposures, covariates, and outcome data using multiple imputation by chained equations, which created five datasets. We performed analyses using each imputed dataset and combined estimates across imputations (Rubin, Reference Rubin2004).

Results

In the pre-baseline wave (t 0;2006/2008), when all covariates were assessed, the average age of participants was 68 years old (s.d. = 10), more likely women (58%) and married (62%). Table 1 summarizes participant characteristics. Table 2 describes changes in loneliness from the pre-baseline to the outcome wave.

Table 1. Characteristics of participants at pre-baseline (N = 13 365)a,b,c

aThis table was created based on non-imputed data.

bAll variables in Table 1 were used as covariates, and assessed in the pre-baseline wave (t 0;2006/2008).

cThe percentages in some sections may not add up to 100% due to rounding.

Table 2. Change in loneliness from the pre-baseline wave (t 0) to the outcome wave (t 2)a

a The percent of lonely and non-lonely individuals in the pre-baseline wave (t 0) who were lonely or non-lonely in the outcome wave (t 2).

Table 3 shows the associations between the candidate predictors and subsequent loneliness. When considering health behaviors, two out of four predictors were associated with subsequent loneliness. Those who engaged in more frequent physical activity (β = −0.07, 95% CI −0.11, −0.03) and binge drinking (β = −0.11, 95% CI −0.20, −0.03) reported less subsequent loneliness 4 years later. For physical health indicators, five out of 15 candidate predictors were associated with subsequent loneliness. Those with fewer physical functioning limitations (β = 0.11, 95% CI 0.03–0.18), and better self-rated health (β = −0.09, 95% CI −0.13 to −0.05) were most strongly associated with lower subsequent loneliness in comparison to other physical health factors.

Table 3. Candidate predictors of loneliness (Health and Retirement Study [HRS]: N = 13 771)a,b,c,d

CI, confidence interval.

*p < 0.05 before Bonferroni correction; **p < 0.01 before Bonferroni correction; ***p < 0.05 after Bonferroni correction (the p value cutoff for Bonferroni correction is p = 0.05/69 predictors = p < 0.0.00072463768).

aThe analytic sample was restricted to those who had participated in the pre-baseline wave (2006 or 2008). Multiple imputation was performed to impute missing data on the exposures, covariates, and outcome. Candidate antecedents were assessed, one at a time, in wave 2 (2010/2012), and the outcome (loneliness) was assessed in wave 3 (2014/2016). The following covariates were controlled for at wave 1 (2006/2008): sociodemographic characteristics (age, sex, race/ethnicity, marital status, income, total wealth, level of education, employment status, health insurance, geographic region), childhood abuse, personality factors (openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, neuroticism), and all of the predictor variables, including health behaviors (physical activity, smoking, binge drinking, sleep problems), physical health (total number of physical conditions, heart disease, cancer, stroke, arthritis, hypertension, overweight/obese, diabetes, lung disease, chronic pain, hearing, eyesight, self-rated health, physical functioning limitations, cognitive impairment), social factors (live with spouse, frequency of contact with children, frequency of contact with other family, frequency of contact with friends, closeness with spouse, having any children, other family, friends, number of close children, number of close other family, number of close friends, positive social support from spouse, positive social support from children, positive social support from friends, positive social support from other family, social strain from spouse, social strain from children, social strain from other family, social strain from friends, volunteering, neighborhood cohesion, neighborhood disorder, social effort/reward, non-religious social activity, religious service attendance, helping friends/neighbors/relatives, employment status, perceived social status, change in perceived social status, loneliness), psychological well-being factors (life satisfaction, positive affect, purpose in life, optimism, health mastery, financial mastery, mastery), and psychological distress (depressive symptoms, hopelessness, negative affect, constraints, anxiety, trait anger, state anger, daily discrimination, major discrimination, cynical hostility, stressful life events, financial strain), health insurance.

bAll continuous candidate antecedents were standardized (mean = 0; standard deviation = 1).

cAn exposure-wide analytic approach was used, and a separate model for each exposure was run. Because loneliness was a continuous outcome, we ran linear regressions.

dThe final estimates of the predictors reflect changes in these values from pre-baseline to baseline waves.

All psychological factors (20 out of 20 predictors) were associated with subsequent loneliness. For example, among psychological well-being factors, life satisfaction (β = −0.16, 95% CI −0.20 to −0.11) and purpose in life (β = −0.16, 95% CI −0.19 to −0.13) were most strongly associated with lower subsequent loneliness in comparison to other psychological well-being factors. Among psychological distress factors, depression (β = 0.33, 95% CI 0.25–0.41) and negative affect (β = 0.23, 95% CI 0.18–0.27) were most strongly associated with greater subsequent loneliness. For social factors, 19 out of 28 predictors were associated with subsequent loneliness. Having friends (>1×/week; β = −0.23, 95% CI −0.34 to −0.13) and frequent interactions with friends (>3×/week; β = −0.22, 95% CI −0.31 to −0.14) were most strongly associated with lower subsequent loneliness.

Additional analyses

We conducted several additional analyses. First, E-values indicated that many of the observed associations were potentially somewhat robust to unmeasured confounding (Table 4), though for some estimates, a combination of unmeasured confounding and statistical uncertainty might explain away the results. For example, for purpose in life, an unmeasured confounder would have to be associated with both loneliness and purpose in life by risk ratios of 1.58 each (above and beyond the covariates already adjusted for) to explain away the association. Further, to shift the CI to include the null, an unmeasured confounder would have to be associated with both loneliness and purpose in life by risk ratios of 1.51. Second, results varied when comparing candidate predictors among the subpopulations of people who were not lonely at baseline compared to who were lonely at baseline (Supplementary Table S1). Most associations between many candidate predictors and subsequent loneliness were observed more strongly among individuals who were lonely at baseline compared to those not lonely at baseline. Third, results from the complete-case analyses showed weaker associations between the candidate predictors and subsequent loneliness compared to results from the imputed analyses (Supplementary Table S2).

Table 4. Robustness to unmeasured confounding (E-values) for the associations between candidate predictors and subsequent loneliness (N = 13 771)a

a See VanderWeele and Ding (Reference VanderWeele and Ding2017) for the formula for calculating E-values.

b The E-values for effect estimates are the minimum strength of association on the risk ratio scale that an unmeasured confounder would need to have with both the exposure and the outcome to fully explain away the observed association between the exposure and outcome, conditional on the measured covariates.

c The E-values for the limit of the 95% confidence interval (CI) closest to the null denote the minimum strength of association on the risk ratio scale that an unmeasured confounder would need to have with both the exposure and the outcome to shift the confidence interval to include the null value, conditional on the measured covariates.

Discussion

In a diverse, large, prospective, and nationally representative sample of US adults aged >50, we examined how changes in 69 candidate predictors (e.g. physical health, health behaviors, and psychosocial factors) were associated with subsequent loneliness 4 years later. Results identified some health behaviors (e.g. physical activity), physical health conditions (e.g. physical functioning limitations), psychological factors (e.g. purpose in life, life satisfaction, depression), and social factors (e.g. frequency of contact with friends, attending religious services) as candidate predictors of subsequent loneliness. However, there was little evidence of associations with subsequent loneliness for other factors. This study is among one of the few to consider such a broad array of predictors of subsequent loneliness – moving us closer to a fuller understanding of the antecedents of a major public health concern and possible intervention targets. All models used the same analytic method and adjusted for the same covariates to allow for comparison of effect sizes among all variables in the same sample.

Our findings converge with some previous studies, indicating that being more active (Boekhout, Berendsen, Peels, Bolman, & Lechner, Reference Boekhout, Berendsen, Peels, Bolman and Lechner2019), having fewer functional limitations (Theeke, Reference Theeke2009), less chronic pain (Loeffler & Steptoe, Reference Loeffler and Steptoe2021; Smith, Dainty, Williamson, & Martin, Reference Smith, Dainty, Williamson and Martin2019), and better: self-rated health (Cohen-Mansfield et al., Reference Cohen-Mansfield, Shmotkin and Goldberg2009; Theeke, Reference Theeke2009), psychological well-being (Aartsen & Jylhä, Reference Aartsen and Jylhä2011; Dahlberg et al., Reference Dahlberg, Andersson, McKee and Lennartsson2015; VanderWeele, Hawkley, & Cacioppo, Reference VanderWeele, Hawkley and Cacioppo2012), and more protective social factors (e.g. greater contact with friends, number of close friends, less strain from others, volunteering, and attending religious services) (Aartsen & Jylhä, Reference Aartsen and Jylhä2011; Cohen-Mansfield et al., Reference Cohen-Mansfield, Shmotkin and Goldberg2009; Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Kaye, Jacobs, Quinones, Dodge, Arnold and Thielke2016; Yang & Gu, Reference Yang and Gu2020) are associated with decreased subsequent loneliness. For example, our findings align with previous study which found that increasing subjective well-being by one standard deviation is associated with decreased subsequent loneliness both 1 and 2 years later (VanderWeele et al., Reference VanderWeele, Hawkley and Cacioppo2012). However, our results also diverged from the results of previous studies, which found associations between other factors and subsequent lower loneliness. For example, past research found that living with others (Theeke, Reference Theeke2009), fewer sleep problems (Hom et al., Reference Hom, Chu, Rogers and Joiner2020), less cognitive impairment (Zhong, Chen, Tu, & Conwell, Reference Zhong, Chen, Tu and Conwell2017), improved hearing (Maharani, Pendleton, & Leroi, Reference Maharani, Pendleton and Leroi2019; Strawbridge, Wallhagen, Shema, & Kaplan, Reference Strawbridge, Wallhagen, Shema and Kaplan2000), and fewer physical health conditions (Cohen-Mansfield et al., Reference Cohen-Mansfield, Shmotkin and Goldberg2009) were related to less loneliness, but we did not find evidence of the association between these factors and subsequent loneliness in this study. Several potential reasons may explain our diverging results, including (1) differences in study design (e.g. varying follow-up periods, simultaneous controlling/covarying of predictors), (2) composition of the study sample, (3) measurement/categorization of exposures (e.g. stable levels of predictors v. changes in predictors) and outcomes (e.g. different types of loneliness scales, using 3 v. 11 v. 20 item UCLA loneliness scales), (4) number of covariates (e.g. differences in specific questionnaires/items, including fewer v. a larger range of covariates) and measurement of covariates (e.g. while some studies used self-reported ratings of physical activity [Macià et al., Reference Macià, Cattaneo, Solana, Tormos, Pascual-Leone and Bartrés-Faz2021] others used objective measurement tools such as accelerometers), or another reason entirely (Schrempft, Jackowska, Hamer, & Steptoe, Reference Schrempft, Jackowska, Hamer and Steptoe2019; Tully et al., Reference Tully, McMullan, Blackburn, Wilson, Coll-Planas, Deidda and Rothenbacher2019).

Importantly, our results corroborate previous research that loneliness is a subjective phenomenon, conceptualized as the perceived discrepancy between desired and experienced social connectedness (Perlman & Peplau, Reference Perlman and Peplau1981). People can feel lonely even when with others, and research consistently finds that loneliness is only weakly associated with objective social isolation (Cacioppo & Cacioppo, Reference Cacioppo and Cacioppo2018; Larson, Reference Larson1990). Accordingly, our findings showed that subjective ratings of physical and psychological health (e.g. self-rated health, pain, purpose in life, negative affect) and the perceived social environment (strain from others, perceptions of positive social support from others, subjective rating of one status on a social ladder) were most strongly associated with subsequent loneliness. In contrast, we observed less evidence for associations between more objective indicators of individuals' physical health (e.g. specific health conditions) and subsequent loneliness.

Despite the acknowledgment that loneliness is a subjective evaluation, more objective social factor characteristics also predicted declines in subsequent loneliness (e.g. contact with friends, religious service attendance, volunteering, and the number of close friends). Improving people's objective social environment (e.g. increasing the size of one's network and increasing frequency of contact) is often proposed as ostensibly the only ‘cure’ for loneliness (Burholt & Scharf, Reference Burholt and Scharf2014; Rokach, Orzeck, & Neto, Reference Rokach, Orzeck and Neto2004). Although this is a valuable effort (also supported by the current study), our findings indicate that fostering people's subjective assessments of psychological well-being and social relationships may be an equally powerful and complementary pathway to decreasing subsequent loneliness. However, there is no one-size-fits-all approach to combatting loneliness. Interventions should use both psychological (e.g. cognitive-therapy) and objective intervention methods (e.g. increasing social engagement through improved transportation, access to technology) to meet the specific needs of individuals (Mann et al., Reference Mann, Bone, Lloyd-Evans, Frerichs, Pinfold, Ma and Johnson2017).

Overall, the effect sizes of the various exposures examined were relatively modest, often corresponding to only a 0.05 or a 0.15 standard deviation reduction in subsequent loneliness. This was true for both the more psychological and the more objective social exposures; no single exposure seemed dominant. On the other hand, there was a wide range of physical, psychological, and social exposures associated with lower subsequent loneliness, which suggests that a multi-faceted intervention approach might be most effective in trying to alleviate loneliness.

The effects of a change in loneliness can differ for individuals who are initially lonely (v. are not), as is the case for the other factors. By adjusting for pre-baseline loneliness in the analysis using the overall sample, we estimated the population-average effects of the predictors on loneliness among the whole sample that included both those who were initially lonely and those who were not. Our supplementary analyses revealed that effect sizes of most predictors were greater for those who were lonely (v. not lonely) at baseline (Supplementary Table S1). Importantly, these patterns were particularly evident among psychological well-being, psychological distress, and social factors. Further, the mere presence of a friendship (regardless of the amount or closeness to a friend) was associated with less subsequent loneliness only for those lonely (v. not lonely) at baseline. Thus, friendships may play an important role in reducing subsequent loneliness, particularly for those who are already experiencing loneliness.

Results from the sensitivity analysis that stratify by lonely and non-lonely individuals at baseline should be interpreted cautiously. Stronger associations between various predictors and subsequent loneliness among those lonely at baseline (v. those not lonely) may be explained by the regulatory loop model of loneliness, which posits that lonely individuals are more likely to continue experiencing loneliness due to their increased susceptibility to stressors (Hawkley & Cacioppo, Reference Hawkley and Cacioppo2010) or less variability in loneliness among those who are already lonely. Notably, growing research suggests that many predictors identified in the current study are modifiable through various intervention efforts. These predictors include but are not limited to behavioral (e.g. increasing physical activity) (Conn, Hafdahl, & Mehr, Reference Conn, Hafdahl and Mehr2011) and psychosocial factors (e.g. positive affect, purpose, mastery, number of friends, depression, negative affect, anxiety) (Moskowitz et al., Reference Moskowitz, Hult, Duncan, Cohn, Maurer, Bussolari and Acree2012; Stewart, Craig, MacPherson, & Alexander, Reference Stewart, Craig, MacPherson and Alexander2001). For example, a meta-analysis of over 358 reports found that interventions using behavioral strategies (e.g. physical activity behavior feedback, goal-setting, self-monitoring) increase physical activity (Conn et al., Reference Conn, Hafdahl and Mehr2011). Studies also indicate that various skills-based training improves positive affect (e.g. noticing minor positive events, mindfulness, positive reappraisal, acts of kindness) (Moskowitz et al., Reference Moskowitz, Hult, Duncan, Cohn, Maurer, Bussolari and Acree2012). Similarly, cognitive behavioral therapy that focuses on reshaping misconceptions and biases about one's mastery and control over a situation/task improves a sense of mastery. Research also indicates that cognitive-behavioral therapy and mindfulness practices reduce depression and anxiety (Hong et al., Reference Hong, Lachman, Charles, Chen, Wilson, Nakamura and Kim2021). Further, accumulating research also indicates that friendships can also be expanded. Various friendship interventions including, Friendship Enrichment Programs, The Fast Friend (FF) Procedure, and Befriending Interventions help individuals develop and maintain positive and close friendships (Stevens, Martina, & Westerhof, Reference Stevens, Martina and Westerhof2006; via enhancing: [1] listening, [2] self-disclosure, [3] expressing gratitude, [4] setting relationship boundaries, [5] increasing self-esteem) (Aron, Melinat, Aron, Vallone, & Bator, Reference Aron, Melinat, Aron, Vallone and Bator1997; Hong et al., Reference Hong, Yeh, Sandy, Fellows, Martin, Shaeffer and Kim2022; Siette, Cassidy, & Priebe, Reference Siette, Cassidy and Priebe2017). Finally, studies have identified building-blocks that can be potentially intervened upon (e.g. volunteering, physical activity) to enhance a sense of purpose in life (Nakamura, Chen, VanderWeele, & Kim, Reference Nakamura, Chen, VanderWeele and Kim2022).

There are important future directions for loneliness research. First, future research should examine how the mechanisms that explain the associations between candidate predictors and subsequent loneliness may vary by key social structural factors. Previous research indicates that loneliness varies by gender, age, socioeconomic status, and race/ethnicity. For example, greater loneliness is often reported by men (v. women) (Barreto et al., Reference Barreto, Victor, Hammond, Eccles, Richins and Qualter2021), among younger adults (v. older adults) (Franssen et al., Reference Franssen, Stijnen, Hamers and Schneider2020), people in lower socioeconomic positions (v. people in higher socioeconomic positions) (De Koning, Stathi, & Richards, Reference De Koning, Stathi and Richards2017), and in the US and UK (v. Japan) (DiJulio, Hamel, Muñana, & Brodie, Reference DiJulio, Hamel, Muñana and Brodie2018). Examining these underlying pathways may help reduce loneliness among the most vulnerable populations (e.g. low SES). Second, growing research suggests that various health and well-being factors are differentially associated with transient v. chronic loneliness. For example, one study among older adults found that living alone and higher socioeconomic status are associated only with transient loneliness. In contrast, the death of a spouse within the past year is associated only with trait loneliness (Hector-Taylor & Adams, Reference Hector-Taylor and Adams1996). Thus, future research should further examine how a wide range of factors are associated with transient and chronic loneliness. Our study also had several limitations. First, many physical health outcomes and health behaviors were self-reported and thus are vulnerable to self-report bias. Study participants, however, were not aware of the study's hypothesis at the time of data collection. Second, a longer scale may more accurately assess loneliness. However, only the three-item loneliness scale was available in the 2006 wave of the HRS. The three-item loneliness scale, however, has shown strong reliability and validity when used in large population-based surveys (Hughes, Waite, Hawkley, & Cacioppo, Reference Hughes, Waite, Hawkley and Cacioppo2004). Third, there is potentially unmeasured confounding. Yet, we were able to evaluate this concern specifically with robust covariate adjustment, use of a prospective design, and E-value analyses. Fourth, temporal factors should be taken into consideration when interpreting findings. Some factors that were associated with subsequent loneliness in our study (e.g. depression) are subject to acute intra-individual changes, and their impact on loneliness could be transitory. Thus, a 4-year interval between measurements may be too long to capture such phenomenon. The current study also had several strengths, such as the use of a prospective, large, diverse, and nationally representative sample of US adults who were 50 years and older. Third, although we adjusted for baseline loneliness to address potential reverse causation, this adjustment may have led to conservative associations between predictors and subsequent loneliness because baseline loneliness can be a mediator of associations.

Meeting the unique needs of our rapidly growing older adult population is expected to be one of the next major global public health challenges. Despite substantial gains in life expectancy, the number of years lost to disability has also increased. Thus, as populations rapidly age, identifying modifiable risk factors that reduce disease risk is important for stemming the rising tide of chronic conditions and associated healthcare costs. The COVID-19 pandemic, with its accompanying social distancing rules, further highlighted the growing concerns over the adverse health outcomes associated with loneliness. Our results highlight potential factors that can be targeted as we continue efforts to develop, refine, and deploy scalable loneliness interventions. Implementing individual- and community-level interventions and policies aimed at combatting loneliness may be a promising and innovative way to improve a wide range of health and well-being outcomes for our rapidly growing older adult population.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723002581.

Acknowledgements

We would like to acknowledge and thank the Health and Retirement Study, which is conducted by the Institute for Social Research at the University of Michigan, with grants from the National Institute on Aging (U01AG09740) and the Social Security Administration. We would also like to thank all authors for their contributions to this manuscript.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the John Templeton Foundation (T. J. V., grant number 61075) and the Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research (E. S. K.).Competing interests

Eric S. Kim has worked as a consultant with AARP and UnitedHealth Group. Tyler J. VanderWeele has received personal fees from Flerish and Flourishing Metrics.

Ethical standards

The ethics review board at the University of British Columbia exempted this study from human subjects review because we used de-identified and publicly available data.

Footnotes

*

Both authors contributed equally as last authors.

References

Aartsen, M., & Jylhä, M. (2011). Onset of loneliness in older adults: Results of a 28 year prospective study. European Journal of Ageing, 8(1), 3138. doi: 10.1007/s10433-011-0175-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aldwin, C. M., & Igarashi, H. (2015). Successful, optimal, and resilient aging: A psychosocial perspective. In Lichtenberg, P. A., Mast, B. T., Carpenter, B. D., & Loebach Wetherell, J. (Eds.), APA handbook of clinical geropsychology, Vol. 1. History and status of the field and perspectives on aging (pp. 331359). American Psychological Association. doi: 10.1037/14458-014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aron, A., Melinat, E., Aron, E. N., Vallone, R. D., & Bator, R. J. (1997). The experimental generation of interpersonal closeness: A procedure and some preliminary findings. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(4), 363377. doi: 10.1177/0146167297234003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barreto, M., Victor, C., Hammond, C., Eccles, A., Richins, M. T., & Qualter, P. (2021). Loneliness around the world: Age, gender, and cultural differences in loneliness. Personality and Individual Differences, 169(1), 110066. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110066CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boekhout, J. M., Berendsen, B. A., Peels, D. A., Bolman, C. A., & Lechner, L. (2019). Physical impairments disrupt the association between physical activity and loneliness: A longitudinal study. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 27(6), 787796. doi: 10.1123/japa.2018-0325CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Böger, A., & Huxhold, O. (2018). Do the antecedents and consequences of loneliness change from middle adulthood into old age? Developmental Psychology, 54(1), 181197. doi: 10.1037/dev0000453CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burholt, V., & Scharf, T. (2014). Poor health and loneliness in later life: The role of depressive symptoms, social resources, and rural environments. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 69(2), 311324. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt121CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, J. T., & Cacioppo, S. (2018). The growing problem of loneliness. The Lancet, 391(10119), 426. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)30142-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen-Mansfield, J., Shmotkin, D., & Goldberg, S. (2009). Loneliness in old age: Longitudinal changes and their determinants in an Israeli sample. International Psychogeriatrics, 21(6), 11601170. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)30142-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conn, V. S., Hafdahl, A. R., & Mehr, D. R. (2011). Interventions to increase physical activity among healthy adults: Meta-analysis of outcomes. American Journal of Public Health, 101(4), 751758. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2010.194381CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahlberg, L., Andersson, L., McKee, K. J., & Lennartsson, C. (2015). Predictors of loneliness among older women and men in Sweden: A national longitudinal study. Aging & Mental Health, 19(5), 409417. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2014.944091CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Koning, J. L., Stathi, A., & Richards, S. (2017). Predictors of loneliness and different types of social isolation of rural-living older adults in the United Kingdom. Ageing & Society, 37(10), 20122043. doi: 10.1017/S0144686X16000696CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Depp, C. A., & Jeste, D. V. (2006). Definitions and predictors of successful aging: A comprehensive review of larger quantitative studies. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14(1), 620. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000192501.03069.bcCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DiJulio, B., Hamel, L., Muñana, C., & Brodie, M. (2018). Loneliness and social isolation in the United States, the United Kingdom, and Japan: An international survey. The Kaiser Family Foundation. Retrieved from https://files.kff.org/attachment/Report-Loneliness-and-Social-Isolation-in-the-United-States-the-United-Kingdom-and-Japan-An-International-SurveyGoogle Scholar
Fisher, G. G., Faul, J. D., Weir, D. R., & Wallace, R. B. (2005). Documentation of chronic disease measures in the Health and Retirement Study (HRS/AHEAD). University of Michigan. Retrieved from https://hrs.isr.umich.edu/sites/default/files/biblio/dr-009.pdfCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franssen, T., Stijnen, M., Hamers, F., & Schneider, F. (2020). Age differences in demographic, social and health-related factors associated with loneliness across the adult life span (19–65 years): A cross-sectional study in the Netherlands. BMC Public Health, 20(1), 1118. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-09208-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fried, L., Prohaska, T., Burholt, V., Burns, A., Golden, J., Hawkley, L., … O'Sullivan, R. (2020). A unified approach to loneliness. The Lancet, 395(10218), 114. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(19)32533-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, B. (2018, February 19). Doctors in the UK are prescribing social activities to fight against loneliness. Retrieved from https://www.weforum.org/agenda/2018/02/line-dancing-and-baking-a-prescription-for-good-health/Google Scholar
Hatton, C. (2013, July 1). New China law says children ‘must visit parents’. Retrieved from https://www.bbc.com/news/world-asia-china-23124345Google Scholar
Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness matters: A theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 40(2), 218227. doi: 10.1007/s12160-010-9210-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hector-Taylor, L., & Adams, P. (1996). State versus trait loneliness in elderly New Zealanders. Psychological Reports, 78(3_suppl), 13291330. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1996.78.3c.1329CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hernán, M. A., Hernández-Díaz, S., & Robins, J. M. (2004). A structural approach to selection bias. Epidemiology, 15(5), 615625. doi: 10.1097/01.ede.0000135174.63482.43CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holt-Lunstad, J. (2020). The double pandemic of social isolation and COVID-19: Cross-sector policy must address both. Health Affairs Blog. doi: 10.1377/forefront.20200609.53823Google Scholar
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T., & Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: A meta-analytic review. Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 10(2), 227237, doi: 10.1177/1745691614568352CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., & Layton, J. B. (2010). Social relationships and mortality risk: A meta-analytic review. PLoS Medicine, 7(7), e1000316. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hom, M. A., Chu, C., Rogers, M. L., & Joiner, T. E. (2020). A meta-analysis of the relationship between sleep problems and loneliness. Clinical Psychological Science, 8(5), 799824. doi: 10.1177/2167702620922969CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hong, J. H., Lachman, M. E., Charles, S. T., Chen, Y., Wilson, C. L., Nakamura, J. S., … Kim, E. S. (2021). The positive influence of sense of control on physical, behavioral, and psychosocial health in older adults: An outcome-wide approach. Preventive Medicine, 149, 106612. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2021.106612CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hong, J. H., Yeh, C. S., Sandy, L. G., Fellows, A., Martin, D. C., Shaeffer, J. A., … Kim, E. S. (2022). Friendship and loneliness: A prototype roadmap for health system action. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 63(1), 141145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsueh, Y.-C., Chen, C.-Y., Hsiao, Y.-C., & Lin, C.-C. (2019). A longitudinal, cross-lagged panel analysis of loneliness and depression among community-based older adults. Journal of Elder Abuse & Neglect, 31(4–5), 281293. doi: 10.1080/08946566.2019.1660936CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). A short scale for measuring loneliness in large surveys: Results from two population-based studies. Research on Aging, 26(6), 655672. doi: 10.1177/0164027504268574CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jeffrey, K., Abdallah, S., & Michaelson, J. (2017). The cost of loneliness to UK employers. New Economic Foundation. Retrieved from https://neweconomics.org/uploads/images/2017/02/NEF_COST-OF-LONELINESS_DIGITAL-Final.pdfGoogle Scholar
Jenkins, K. R., Ofstedal, M. B., & Weir, D. (2008). Documentation of health behaviors and risk factors measured in the Health and Retirement Study (HRS/AHEAD). University of Michigan. Retrieved from https://hrs.isr.umich.edu/sites/default/files/biblio/dr-010.pdfCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jia, G., & Yuan, P. (2020). The association between sleep quality and loneliness in rural older individuals: A cross-sectional study in Shandong Province, China. BMC Geriatrics, 20, 180. doi: 10.1186/s12877-020-01554-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, E. S., Tkatch, R., Martin, D., MacLeod, S., Sandy, L., & Yeh, C. (2021). Resilient aging: Psychological well-being and social well-being as targets for the promotion of healthy aging. Gerontology and Geriatric Medicine, 7, 23337214211002951. doi: 10.1177/23337214211002951CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, J., Lee, S., Chun, S., Han, A., & Heo, J. (2017). The effects of leisure-time physical activity for optimism, life satisfaction, psychological well-being, and positive affect among older adults with loneliness. Annals of Leisure Research, 20(4), 406415. doi: 10.1080/11745398.2016.1238308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lara, E., Martín-María, N., De la Torre-Luque, A., Koyanagi, A., Vancampfort, D., Izquierdo, A., & Miret, M. (2019). Does loneliness contribute to mild cognitive impairment and dementia? A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Ageing Research Reviews, 52, 716. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2019.03.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larson, R. W. (1990). The solitary side of life: An examination of the time people spend alone from childhood to old age. Developmental Review, 10(2), 155183. doi: 10.1016/0273-2297(90)90008-RCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leigh-Hunt, N., Bagguley, D., Bash, K., Turner, V., Turnbull, S., Valtorta, N., & Caan, W. (2017). An overview of systematic reviews on the public health consequences of social isolation and loneliness. Public Health, 152, 157171. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2017.07.035CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loeffler, A., & Steptoe, A. (2021). Bidirectional longitudinal associations between loneliness and pain, and the role of inflammation. Pain, 162(3), 930937. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002082CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macià, D., Cattaneo, G., Solana, J., Tormos, J. M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Bartrés-Faz, D. (2021). Meaning in life: A major predictive factor for loneliness comparable to health status and social connectedness. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 627547. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.627547CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maharani, A., Pendleton, N., & Leroi, I. (2019). Hearing impairment, loneliness, social isolation, and cognitive function: Longitudinal analysis using English Longitudinal Study on Ageing. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 27(12), 13481356. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2019.07.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann, F., Bone, J. K., Lloyd-Evans, B., Frerichs, J., Pinfold, V., Ma, R., … Johnson, S. (2017). A life less lonely: The state of the art in interventions to reduce loneliness in people with mental health problems. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 52(6), 627638. doi: 10.1007/s00127-017-1392-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAuley, E., Blissmer, B., Marquez, D. X., Jerome, G. J., Kramer, A. F., & Katula, J. (2000). Social relations, physical activity, and well-being in older adults. Preventive Medicine, 31(5), 608617. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2000.0740CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDaid, D., Bauer, A., & Park, A.-L. (2017). Making the economic case for investing in actions to prevent and/or tackle loneliness: A systematic review. London School of Economics and Political Science. Retrieved from http://cdn.basw.co.uk/upload/basw_51039-5.pdfGoogle Scholar
Moskowitz, J. T., Hult, J. R., Duncan, L. G., Cohn, M. A., Maurer, S., Bussolari, C., & Acree, M. (2012). A positive affect intervention for people experiencing health-related stress: Development and non-randomized pilot test. Journal of Health Psychology, 17(5), 676692. doi: 10.1177/1359105311425275CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakamura, J. S., Chen, Y., VanderWeele, T. J., & Kim, E. S. (2022). What makes life purposeful? Identifying the antecedents of a sense of purpose in life using a lagged exposure-wide approach. Social Science and Medicine – Population Health, 19, 101235. doi: 10.1016/j.ssmph.2022.101235Google ScholarPubMed
Newall, N. E., Chipperfield, J. G., Clifton, R. A., Perry, R. P., Swift, A. U., & Ruthig, J. C. (2009). Causal beliefs, social participation, and loneliness among older adults: A longitudinal study. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 26(2–3), 273290. doi: 10.1177/0265407509106718CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, C., Majeed, A., Gill, H., Tamura, J., Ho, R. C., Mansur, R. B., … McIntyre, R. S. (2020). The effect of loneliness on distinct health outcomes: A comprehensive review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research, 294, 113514. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113514CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perissinotto, C., Holt-Lunstad, J., Periyakoil, V. S., & Covinsky, K. (2019). A practical approach to assessing and mitigating loneliness and isolation in older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 67(4), 657662. doi: 10.1111/jgs.15746CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perissinotto, C. M., Stijacic Cenzer, I., & Covinsky, K. E. (2012). Loneliness in older persons: A predictor of functional decline and death. Archives of Internal Medicine, 172(14), 10781084. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2012.1993CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perlman, D., & Peplau, L. A. (1981). Toward a social psychology of loneliness. Personal Relationships, 3, 3156. Retrieved from: https://peplau.psych.ucla.edu/wp-content/uploads/sites/141/2017/07/Perlman-Peplau-81.pdfGoogle Scholar
Petersen, J., Kaye, J., Jacobs, P. G., Quinones, A., Dodge, H., Arnold, A., & Thielke, S. (2016). Longitudinal relationship between loneliness and social isolation in older adults: Results from the cardiovascular health study. Journal of Aging and Health, 28(5), 775795. doi: 10.1177/0898264315611664CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polack, E. (2018, May 1). New Cigna study reveals loneliness at epidemic levels in America. https://www.multivu.com/players/English/8294451-cigna-us-loneliness-survey/Google Scholar
Rokach, A., Orzeck, T., & Neto, F. (2004). Coping with loneliness in old age: A cross-cultural comparison. Current Psychology, 23(2), 124137. doi: 10.1007/BF02903073CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowe, J. W., & Kahn, R. L. (1987). Human aging: Usual and successful. Science, 237(4811), 143149. doi: 10.1126/science.3299702CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, D. B. (2004). Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys (Vol. 81). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Rubin, R. (2017). Loneliness might be a killer, but what's the best way to protect against it. JAMA, 318(19), 18531855. doi: 10.1001/jama.2017.14591CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., & Cutrona, C. E. (1980). The revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(3), 472480. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.39.3.472CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. (2009). Understanding healthy aging: Key components and their integration. In Bengston, V. L., Gans, D., Pulney, N. M. & Silverstein, M. (Eds.), Handbook of theories of ageing (pp. 117144). New York: Springer Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Schrempft, S., Jackowska, M., Hamer, M., & Steptoe, A. (2019). Associations between social isolation, loneliness, and objective physical activity in older men and women. BMC Public Health, 19, 74. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-6424-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siette, J., Cassidy, M., & Priebe, S. (2017). Effectiveness of befriending interventions: A systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open, 7(4), e014304. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-014304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, J., Fisher, G., Ryan, L., Clarke, P., House, J., & Weir, D. (2013). Psychosocial and lifestyle questionnaire 2006-2010: Documentation report. University of Michigan. Retrieved from https://hrs.isr.umich.edu/sites/default/files/biblio/HRS2006-2010SAQdoc.pdfGoogle Scholar
Smith, J., Ryan, L., Fisher, G. G., Sonnega, A., & Weir, D. (2017). Documentation report core section LB. University of Michigan. Retrieved from https://hrs.isr.umich.edu/sites/default/files/biblio/HRS%202006-2016%20SAQ%20Documentation_07.06.17_0.pdGoogle Scholar
Smith, T. O., Dainty, J. R., Williamson, E., & Martin, K. R. (2019). Association between musculoskeletal pain with social isolation and loneliness: Analysis of the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. British Journal of Pain, 13(2), 8290. doi: 10.1177/2049463718802868CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, N. L., Martina, C. M. S., & Westerhof, G. J. (2006). Meeting the need to belong: Predicting effects of a friendship enrichment program for older women. The Gerontologist, 46(4), 495502. doi: 10.1093/geront/46.4.495CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, M., Craig, D., MacPherson, K., & Alexander, S. (2001). Promoting positive affect and diminishing loneliness of widowed seniors through a support intervention. Public Health Nursing, 18(1), 5463. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1446.2001.00054.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strawbridge, W. J., Wallhagen, M. I., Shema, S. J., & Kaplan, G. A. (2000). Negative consequences of hearing impairment in old age: A longitudinal analysis. The Gerontologist, 40(3), 320326. doi: 10.1093/geront/40.3.320CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swader, C. S. (2019). Loneliness in Europe: Personal and societal individualism-collectivism and their connection to social isolation. Social Forces, 97(3), 13071336. doi: 10.1093/sf/soy088CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Theeke, L. A. (2009). Predictors of loneliness in U.S. adults over age sixty-five. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 23(5), 387396. doi: 10.1016/j.apnu.2008.11.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tully, M. A., McMullan, I. I., Blackburn, N. E., Wilson, J. J., Coll-Planas, L., Deidda, M., … Rothenbacher, D. (2019). Is sedentary behavior or physical activity associated with loneliness in older adults? Results of the European-wide SITLESS Study. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 28(4), 549555. doi: 10.1123/japa.2019-0311CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VanderWeele, T. J., & Ding, P. (2017). Sensitivity analysis in observational research: Introducing the E-value. Annals of Internal Medicine, 167(4), 268274. doi: 10.7326/M16-2607CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VanderWeele, T. J., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2012). On the reciprocal association between loneliness and subjective well-being. American Journal of Epidemiology, 176(9), 777784. doi: 10.1093/aje/kws173CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VanderWeele, T. J., Mathur, M. B., & Chen, Y. (2020). Outcome-wide longitudinal designs for causal inference: A new template for empirical studies. Statistical Science, 35(3), 437466. doi: 10.1214/19-STS728CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Victor, C. R., & Bowling, A. (2012). A longitudinal analysis of loneliness among older people in Great Britain. The Journal of Psychology, 146(3), 313331. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2011.609572CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Victor, C. R., & Yang, K. (2012). The prevalence of loneliness among adults: A case study of the United Kingdom. The Journal of Psychology, 146(1–2), 85104. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2011.613875CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, F., & Gu, D. (2020). Predictors of loneliness incidence in Chinese older adults from a life course perspective: A national longitudinal study. Aging & Mental Health, 24(6), 879888. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2018.1558174CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhong, B.-L., Chen, S.-L., Tu, X., & Conwell, Y. (2017). Loneliness and cognitive function in older adults: Findings from the Chinese longitudinal healthy longevity survey. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 72(1), 120128. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Characteristics of participants at pre-baseline (N = 13 365)a,b,c

Figure 1

Table 2. Change in loneliness from the pre-baseline wave (t0) to the outcome wave (t2)a

Figure 2

Table 3. Candidate predictors of loneliness (Health and Retirement Study [HRS]: N = 13 771)a,b,c,d

Figure 3

Table 4. Robustness to unmeasured confounding (E-values) for the associations between candidate predictors and subsequent loneliness (N = 13 771)a

Supplementary material: File

Hong et al. supplementary material

Hong et al. supplementary material
Download Hong et al. supplementary material(File)
File 89.9 KB