Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T14:58:57.681Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Rumination, distraction and mindful self-focus: effects on mood, dysfunctional attitudes and cortisol stress response

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 May 2008

C. Kuehner*
Affiliation:
Research Group Longitudinal and Intervention Research, Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim, Germany
S. Huffziger
Affiliation:
Research Group Longitudinal and Intervention Research, Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim, Germany
K. Liebsch
Affiliation:
Research Group Longitudinal and Intervention Research, Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim, Germany
*
*Address for correspondence: C. Kuehner, Ph.D., Research Group Longitudinal and Intervention Research, Central Institute of Mental Health, PO Box 122120, D-68072 Mannheim, Germany. (Email: [email protected])

Abstract

Background

Although aggravating effects of rumination on dysfunctional cognitions and endocrine stress responses have been proposed, experimental studies testing these assumptions are lacking. In parallel, mindfulness theory suggests beneficial effects of mindfulness on dysfunctional cognitions. This study aimed to investigate the effects of induced rumination, distraction and mindful self-focus on mood and dysfunctional attitudes and to assess the possible impact of induced rumination on participants' cortisol responses.

Method

Sixty university students were subjected to negative mood induction and subsequently randomly assigned to a rumination, distraction or mindful self-focus condition. The latter included statements focusing on self-acceptance and awareness of the breath. Four saliva cortisol samples were selected during the session.

Results

Compared to induced rumination, distraction showed a clear beneficial effect on the course of dysphoric mood, whereas a mindful self-focus did not. In contrast to distraction and mindful self-focus, participants induced to ruminate showed significant increases in dysfunctional attitudes from baseline to post-induction. Although rumination was not itself linked to higher cortisol responses, participants scoring high on the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)-II who were induced to ruminate showed a smaller decrease in cortisol levels than those scoring low on the BDI-II.

Conclusions

This study indicates that rumination as a dysfunctional mode of cognitive processing is able to maintain depression-linked dysfunctional thought content. Furthermore, our study revealed preliminary indications for a link between induced rumination and the cortisol stress response in vulnerable individuals.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © 2008 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abela, JRZ, Brozina, K, Haigh, EP (2002). An examination of the response styles theory of depression in third- and seventh-grade children: a short-term longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 30, 515527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
APA (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn. American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Bagby, RM, Rector, NA, Bacchiochi, JR, McBride, C (2004). The stability of the Response Styles Questionnaire rumination scale in a sample of patients with major depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research 28, 527538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, AT, Rush, AJ, Shaw, BF, Emery, G (1979). Cognitive Therapy of Depression. Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Broderick, PC (2005). Mindfulness and coping with dysphoric mood: contrasts with rumination and distraction. Cognitive Therapy and Research 29, 501510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brosschot, JF, Gerin, W, Thayer, JF (2006). The perseverative cognition hypothesis: a review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiological activation, and health. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 60, 113124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burke, HM, Davis, CG, Otte, C, Mohr, DC (2005). Depression and cortisol responses to psychological stress: a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 846856.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, JR, Henry, JD (2004). The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS): construct validity, measurement properties and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 43, 245265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donaldson, C, Lam, D (2004). Rumination, mood and social problem-solving in major depression. Psychological Medicine 34, 13091318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fekedulegn, DB, Andrew, ME, Burchfiel, CM, Violanti, JM, Hartley, TA, Charles, LE, Miller, DB (2007). Area under the curve and other summary indicators of repeated waking cortisol measurements. Psychosomatic Medicine 69, 651659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fresco, DM, Heimberg, RG, Abramowitz, A, Bertram, TL (2006). The effect of a negative mood priming challenge on dysfunctional attitudes, explanatory style, and explanatory flexibility. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 45, 167183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gemar, MC, Segal, ZV, Sagrati, S, Kennedy, SJ (2001). Mood-induced changes on the Implicit Association Test in recovered depressed patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 110, 282289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodyer, IM, Herbert, J, Tamplin, A (2003). Psychoendocrine antecedents of persistent first-episode major depression in adolescents: a community-based longitudinal enquiry. Psychological Medicine 33, 601610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grossman, P, Tiefenthaler-Gilmer, U, Raysz, A, Kesper, U (2007). Mindfulness training as an intervention for fibromyalgia: evidence of postintervention and 3-year follow-up benefits in well-being. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 76, 226233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hautzinger, M, Joormann, J, Keller, F (2005). DAS: Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale, German Version [in German]. Hogrefe: Göttingen.Google Scholar
Hautzinger, M, Keller, F, Kuehner, C (2006). Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI-II), German Version [in German]. Harcourt Test Services: Frankfurt a. M.Google Scholar
Heidenreich, T, Michalak, J (2003). Mindfulness as a treatment principle in behaviour therapy [in German]. Verhaltenstherapie 13, 264274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hertel, PT, Gerstle, M (2003). Depressive deficits in forgetting. Psychological Science 14, 573578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hong, RY (2007). Worry and rumination: differential associations with anxious and depressive symptoms and coping behavior. Behaviour Research and Therapy 45, 277290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ingram, RE, Miranda, J, Segal, ZV (1998). Cognitive Vulnerability to Depression. Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Jain, S, Shapiro, S, Swanick, S, Roesch, S, Mills, P, Bell, I, Schwartz, G (2007). A randomized controlled trial of mindfulness meditation versus relaxation training: effects on distress, positive states of mind, rumination, and distraction. Annals of Behavioral Medicine 33, 1121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joormann, J, Siemer, M (2004). Memory accessibility, mood regulation, and dysphoria: difficulties in repairing sad mood with happy memories? Journal of Abnormal Psychology 113, 179188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kenny, MA, Williams, JM (2007). Treatment-resistant depressed patients show a good response to mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy 45, 617625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kingston, T, Dooley, B, Bates, A, Lawlor, E, Malone, K (2007). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for residual depressive symptoms. Psychology and Psychotherapy 80, 193203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krohne, HW, Egloff, B, Kohlmann, CW, Tausch, A (1996). Investigations with a German version of the ‘Positive and Negative Affect Schedule’ (PANAS). Diagnostica 42, 139156.Google Scholar
Kudielka, BM, Schommer, NC, Hellhammer, DH, Kirschbaum, C (2004). Acute HPA axis responses, heart rate, and mood changes to psychosocial stress (TSST) in humans at different times of day. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29, 983992.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuehner, C, Buerger, C, Keller, F, Hautzinger, M (2007 b). Reliability and validity of the Revised Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II). Results from German samples. Nervenarzt 78, 651656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuehner, C, Holzhauer, S, Huffziger, S (2007 a). Decreased cortisol response to awakening is associated with cognitive vulnerability to depression in a nonclinical sample of young adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 199209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuehner, C, Weber, I (1999). Responses to depression in unipolar depressed patients: an investigation of Nolen-Hoeksema's response styles theory. Psychological Medicine 29, 13231333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lara, ME, Klein, DN, Kasch, KL (2000). Psychosocial predictors of the short-term course and outcome of major depression: a longitudinal study of a nonclinical sample with recent-onset episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109, 644650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lavender, A, Watkins, E (2004). Rumination and future thinking in depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 43, 129142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyubomirsky, S, Kasri, F, Zehm, K (2003). Dysphoric rumination impairs concentration on academic tasks. Cognitive Therapy and Research 27, 309330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyubomirsky, S, Tkach, C (2004). The consequences of dysphoric rumination. In Depressive Rumination. Nature, Theory and Treatment (ed. Papageorgiou, C. and Wells, A.), pp. 2142. Wiley: Chichester.Google Scholar
Ma, SH, Teasdale, JD (2004). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: replication and exploration of differential relapse prevention effects. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 72, 3140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, M (1990). On the induction of mood. Clinical Psychology Review 10, 669697.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCullough, ME, Orsulak, P, Brandon, A, Akers, L (2007). Rumination, fear, and cortisol: an in vivo study of interpersonal transgressions. Health Psychology 26, 126132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miranda, J, Gross, JJ, Persons, JB, Hahn, J (1998). Mood matters: negative mood induction activates dysfunctional attitudes in women vulnerable to depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research 22, 363376.Google Scholar
Monroe, SM, Slavich, GM, Torres, LD, Gotlib, IH (2007). Major life events and major chronic difficulties are differentially associated with history of major depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 116, 116124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolen-Hoeksema, S (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 100, 569582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolen-Hoeksema, S (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109, 504511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolen-Hoeksema, S (2004). The response styles theory. In Depressive Rumination. Nature, Theory and Treatment (ed. Papageorgiou, C. and Wells, A.), pp. 107124. Wiley: Chichester.Google Scholar
Nolen-Hoeksema, S, Parker, LE, Larson, J (1994). Ruminative coping with depressed mood following loss. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67, 92104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolen-Hoeksema, S, Stice, E, Wade, E, Bohon, C (2007). Reciprocal relations between rumination and bulimic, substance abuse, and depressive symptoms in female adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 116, 198207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruessner, J, Kirschbaum, C, Meinlschmid, G, Hellhammer, DH (2003). Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology 28, 916931.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raes, F, Hermans, D, Williams, JM, Beyers, W, Eelen, P, Brunfaut, E (2006). Reduced autobiographical memory specificity and rumination in predicting the course of depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 115, 699704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramel, W, Goldin, PR, Carmona, PE, McQuaid, JR (2004). The effects of mindfulness meditation on cognitive processes and affect in patients with past depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research 28, 433455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ray, RD, Ochsner, KN, Cooper, JC, Robertson, ER, Gabrieli, JDE, Gross, JJ (2005). Individual differences in trait rumination and the neural systems supporting cognitive reappraisal. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 5, 156168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rimes, KA, Watkins, E (2005). The effects of self-focused rumination on global negative self-judgements in depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy 43, 16731681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sarin, S, Abela, JRZ, Auerbach, RP (2005). The response styles theory of depression: a test of specificity and causal mediation. Cognition and Emotion 19, 751761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segal, ZV, Gemar, M, Williams, S (1999). Differential cognitive response to a mood challenge following successful cognitive therapy or pharmacotherapy for unipolar depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 108, 310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Segal, ZV, Kennedy, S, Gemar, M, Hood, K, Pedersen, R, Buis, T (2006). Cognitive reactivity to sad mood provocation and the prediction of depressive relapse. Archives of General Psychiatry 63, 749755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Segal, ZV, Williams, JMG, Teasdale, JD (2002). Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for Depression: A New Approach to Preventing Relapse. Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Shapiro, SL, Brown, KW, Biegel, GM (2007). Teaching self-care to caregivers: effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on the mental health of therapists in training. Training and Education in Professional Psychology 1, 105115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegle, GJ, Carter, CS, Thase, ME (2006). Use of fMRI to predict recovery from unipolar depression with cognitive behavior therapy. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 735738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, AR, Dobson, KS (2007). An experimental investigation of the cognitive vulnerability to depression. Behavior Research and Therapy 45, 563575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, JM, Alloy, LB, Abramson, LY (2006). Cognitive vulnerability to depression, rumination, hopelessness, and suicidal ideation: multiple pathways to self-injurious thinking. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior 36, 443454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spasojevic, J, Alloy, LB (2001). Rumination as a common mechanism relating depressive risk factors to depression. Emotion 1, 2537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teasdale, JD, Segal, ZV, Williams, JM, Ridgeway, VA, Soulsby, JM, Lau, MA (2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 68, 615623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watkins, E, Moulds, M (2005). Distinct modes of ruminative self-focus: impact of abstract versus concrete rumination on problem solving in depression. Emotion 5, 319328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watkins, E, Teasdale, JD (2001). Rumination and overgeneral memory in depression: effects of self-focus and analytic thinking. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 110, 353357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watkins, E, Teasdale, JD (2004). Adaptive and maladaptive self-focus in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders 82, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D, Clark, L, Tellegen, A (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54, 10631070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westermann, R, Spies, K, Stahl, GA, Hesse, FW (1996). Relative effectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures: a meta-analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology 4, 557580.3.0.CO;2-4>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, EA, Nolen-Hoeksema, S (2001). Effect of ruminations on the saliva cortisol response to a social stressor. Psychoneuroendocrinology 26, 319329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed