Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T01:04:10.173Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Performance of long-stay schizophrenics on matched verbal and visuospatial recall tasks

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

Avraham Calev*
Affiliation:
Jerusalem Mental Health Center, Ezrath-Nashim Hospital, Jerusalem and Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry, Hebrew University of Jerusalem
Sanda Edelist
Affiliation:
Jerusalem Mental Health Center, Ezrath-Nashim Hospital, Jerusalem and Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry, Hebrew University of Jerusalem
Sol Kugelmass
Affiliation:
Jerusalem Mental Health Center, Ezrath-Nashim Hospital, Jerusalem and Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry, Hebrew University of Jerusalem
Bernard Lerer
Affiliation:
Jerusalem Mental Health Center, Ezrath-Nashim Hospital, Jerusalem and Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry, Hebrew University of Jerusalem
*
1 Address for correspondence: Dr Avraham Calev, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, Health Sciences Center, State University of New York at Stony Brook, Stony Brook, New York 11794–8101, USA.

Synopsis

A verbal and a visuospatial recall task were compared for discriminating power, using the matched-tasks methodology. These tasks were administered to long-hospitalized schizophrenics. No evidence of a differential deficit, that is, better recall of either the verbal or the visuospatial materials, emerged in the patients. The results replicate a former finding showing no difference between verbal and visuospatial recall in schizophrenics, using memory tasks which were less sensitive as left- and right-hemisphere measures and a non-verbal task less affected by verbal mediation. This replication questions the assumption that the hemispheric differences observed in schizophrenics affect such memory tasks.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1991

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association (1987). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd edn. revised). AMA: Washington DC.Google Scholar
Ammons, R. B. & Ammons, C. H. (1962). Quick Test. Psychological Tests Specialists: Missoula, MT.Google Scholar
Andreasen, N. C., Olsen, S. A., Dennert, J. W. & Smith, M. R. (1982). Ventricular enlargement in schizophrenia: relationship to positive and negative symptoms. American Journal of Psychiatry 139, 297302.Google ScholarPubMed
Ariel, R. N., Golden, C. J., Berg, R. A., Quaife, M. A., Dirksen, J. N., Forsell, T., Wilson, J. & Graber, B. (1983). Regional cerebral blood flow in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 40, 258263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R., Colter, N., Corsellis, N., Crow, T. J., Frith, C. D., Jagoe, R., Johnstone, E. C. & Marsh, L. (1986). Post mortem evidence of structural brain changes in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 43, 3642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calev, A. (1984 a). Recall and recognition in mildly disturbed schizophrenics: use of matched tasks. Psychological Medicine 14, 425429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calev, A. (1984 b). Recall and recognition in chronic nondemented schizophrenics: use of matched tasks. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 93, 172177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calev, A. (1990). Memory in schizophrenia. In International Perspectives in Schizophrenia (ed. Weller, P. I.), pp. 2941. Libby: London.Google Scholar
Calev, A. & Monk, A. F. (1982). Verbal memory tasks showing no deficit in schizophrenia – fact or artefact? British Journal of Psychiatry 141, 528530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calev, A., Venables, P. H. & Monk, A. F. (1983). Evidence for distinct verbal memory pathologies in severely and mildly disturbed schizophrenics. Schizophrenia Bulletin 9, 247264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calev, A., Korin, Y., Kugelmass, S. & Lerer, B. (1987). Performance of chronic schizophrenics on matched word and design recall tasks. Biological Psychiatry 22, 699709.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, L. J. & Chapman, J. P. (1973). Disordered Thought in Schizophrenia. Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York.Google Scholar
Chapman, L. J. & Chapman, J. P. (1978). The measurement of differential deficits. Journal of Psychiatric Research 14, 303311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coffman, B. A. & Nasrallah, H. A. (1984). Brain density patterns in schizophrenia and mania. Journal of Affective Disorders 6, 307315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crow, T. J. & Grove-White, I. G. (1973). An analysis of the learning deficit following hycosine administration to man. British Journal of Psychopharmacology 49, 322327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flor-Henry, P., Koles, Z. J. & Howarth, B. G. (1979). Neurophysiology studies of schizophrenia, mania, and depression. In Hemisphere Asymmetries of Function in Psychopathology ed. Gruzilier, J. and Flor-Henry, P., pp. 189222. Elsevier/North-Holland: New York.Google Scholar
Goldberg, E. (1985). Akinesia, tardive dysmentia, and frontal lobe disorder in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 11, 255263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gur, R. E. (1978). Right hemisphere dysfunction and left hemisphere overactivation in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 87, 226238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gur, R. E. (1979). Cognitive concomitants of hemispheric dysfunction in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 36, 269274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gur, R. E., Gur, R. C., Skolnick, B. E. & Caloff, S. (1985). Brain function in psychiatric disorders. III. Regional cerebral blood flow in unmedicated schizophrenics. Archives of General Psychiatry 42, 329334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gur, R. E., Resnick, S. M., Gur, R. C., Alavi, A., Caroff, S., Kushner, M. & Reivich, M. (1987). Regional brain function in schizophrenia. II. Repeated evaluation with positron emission tomography. Archives of General Psychiatry 44, 126129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joanette, Y., Goulet, P., Ska, B. & Nespoulous, J. (1986). Informative content of narrative discourse in right-brain-damaged right-handers. Brain and Language 29, 81105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, P. & Neale, J. M. (1974). Hippocampal damage in schizophrenia. Abnormal Psychology 83, 9196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koh, S. D. (1978). Remembering of verbal materials by schizophrenic young adults. In Language and Cognition in Schizophrenia (ed. Schwartz, S.), (pp. 5559). Erlbaum: Hillsdale NJ.Google Scholar
Kolb, B. & Whishaw, I. Q. (1983). Performance of schizophrenic patients on tests sensitive to left or right frontal, temporal, or parietal function in neurological patients. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 171, 435443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lerner, J., Nachshon, J. & Carmon, A. (1977). Responses of paranoid and nonparanoid schizophrenics in a dichotic listening task. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 164, 247252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lezak, M. D. (1983). Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford University Press: New York.Google Scholar
Luchins, D. J., Weinberger, D. R. & Wyatt, R. J. (1982). Schizophrenia and cerebral asymmetry detected by computed tomography. American Journal of Psychiatry 139, 753757.Google ScholarPubMed
Milner, B. (1975). Psychological aspects of focal epilepsy and its neurological management. D. P. Purpura, J. K. Perry and R. D. Walter, Advances in Neurology, Vol. 8, pp. 299321. Raven Press: New York.Google Scholar
Nigal, D., Calev, A., Kugelmass, S. & Lerer, B. (1991). The effect of neuroleptic and anticholinergic drug withdrawal on memory function in chronic long-stay schizophrenics. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry (in the press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roman, M., Brownell, H. H., Potter, H. H., Seibold, M. S. & Gardner, H. (1987). Script knowledge in right-hemisphere-damaged and in normal elderly adults. Brain and Language 31, 151170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spitzer, R. L., Endicott, J. & Robins, E. (1977). Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC) for a Selected Group of Functional Disorders, (3rd edn.) New York Psychiatric Institute: New York.Google Scholar
Squire, L. R. (1987). Memory and Brain. Oxford University Press: New York.Google Scholar
Squire, L. R. & Chace, P. M. (1975). Memory functions six to nine months after electroconvulsive therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry 32, 15571564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L. R. & Slater, P. C. (1978). Bilateral and unilateral ECT: effects on verbal and nonverbal memory. American Journal of Psychiatry 11, 13161320.Google Scholar
Suddath, R. L., Casanova, M. E., Goldberg, T. E., Daniel, D. G., Kelsoe, J. R. & Weinberger, D. R. (1989). Temporal lobe pathology in schizophrenia: a quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study. American Journal of Psychiatry 146, 464472.Google ScholarPubMed
Tubi, N. & Calev, A. (1989). Verbal and visuospatial recall by young and old normals: use of matched tasks. Psychology and Aging 4, 493495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tune, L. E., Strauss, M. F., Lew, M. F., Breitlinger, E. & Coyle, J. T. (1983). Serum levels of anticholinergic drugs and impaired recent memory in chronic schizophrenic patients. American Journal of Psychiatry 139, 14601462.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale – Revised Manual. Psychological Corp., Janovich Inc: San Antonio.Google Scholar
Wexler, B. E. (1980). Cerebral laterality and psychiatry: a review of the literature. American Journal of Psychiatry 137, 279291.Google ScholarPubMed