Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T08:30:01.112Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Modulation of error monitoring in obsessive–compulsive disorder by individually tailored symptom provocation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 April 2017

D. Roh
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Chuncheon Sacred Heart Hospital, Hallym University College of Medicine, Chuncheon-si, Gangwon-do, Republic of Korea
J.-G. Chang
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Severance Hospital, Seoul, Republic of Korea
S. W. Yoo
Affiliation:
Yoo and Kim Mental Health Clinic, Seoul, Republic of Korea
J. Shin
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Severance Hospital, Seoul, Republic of Korea
C.-H. Kim*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Severance Hospital, Seoul, Republic of Korea Institute of Behavioral Science in Medicine, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, Republic of Korea
*
*Address for correspondence: C.-H. Kim, M.D., Ph.D., Department of Psychiatry, Severance Hospital, Yonsei University College of Medicine, 50-1, Yonsei-ro, Seodaemun-gu, Seoul 120-752, Republic ofKorea. (Email: [email protected])

Abstract

Background

The enhanced error monitoring in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD), typically measured with the error-related negativity (ERN), has been found to be temporally stable and independent of symptom expression. Here, we examined whether the error monitoring in patients with OCD could be experimentally modulated by individually tailored symptom provocation.

Method

Twenty patients with OCD and 20 healthy controls performed a flanker task in which OCD-relevant or neutral pictures were presented prior to a flanker stimulus. An individualized stimulus set consisting of the most provoking images in terms of OCD symptoms was selected for each patient with OCD. Response-locked event-related potentials were recorded and used to examine the error-related brain activity.

Results

Patients with OCD showed larger ERN amplitudes than did control subjects in both the OCD-symptom provocation and neutral conditions. Additionally, while patients with OCD exhibited a significant increase in the ERN under the OCD-symptom provocation condition when compared with the neutral condition, control subjects showed no variation in the ERN between the conditions.

Conclusions

Our results strengthen earlier findings of hyperactive error monitoring in OCD, as indexed by higher ERN amplitudes in patients with OCD than in controls. Importantly, we showed that the patients’ overactive error-signals were experimentally enhanced by individually tailored OCD-symptom triggers, thus suggesting convincing evidence between OCD-symptoms and ERN. Such findings imply that therapeutic interventions should target affective regulation in order to alleviate the perceived threatening value of OCD triggers.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abramowitz, JS, Deacon, BJ, Olatunji, BO, Wheaton, MG, Berman, NC, Losardo, D, Timpano, KR, McGrath, PB, Riemann, BC, Adams, T (2010). Assessment of obsessive–compulsive symptom dimensions: development and evaluation of the Dimensional Obsessive–Compulsive Scale. Psychological Assessment 22, 180198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) . American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Aouizerate, B, Guehl, D, Cuny, E, Rougier, A, Bioulac, B, Tignol, J, Burbaud, P (2004). Pathophysiology of obsessive–compulsive disorder: a necessary link between phenomenology, neuropsychology, imagery and physiology. Progress in Neurobiology 72, 195221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, AT, Ward, C, Mendelson, M (1961). Beck depression inventory (BDI). Archives of General Psychiatry 4, 561571.Google Scholar
Britton, JC, Stewart, SE, Killgore, WD, Rosso, IM, Price, LM, Gold, AL, Pine, DS, Wilhelm, S, Jenike, MA, Rauch, SL (2010). Amygdala activation in response to facial expressions in pediatric obsessive–compulsive disorder. Depression and Anxiety 27, 643651.Google Scholar
Cannistraro, PA, Wright, CI, Wedig, MM, Martis, B, Shin, LM, Wilhelm, S, Rauch, SL (2004). Amygdala responses to human faces in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry 56, 916920.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cardoner, N, Harrison, BJ, Pujol, J, Soriano-Mas, C, Hernández-Ribas, R, López-Solá, M, Real, E, Deus, J, Ortiz, H, Alonso, P (2011). Enhanced brain responsiveness during active emotional face processing in obsessive compulsive disorder. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry 12, 349363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavada, C, Tejedor, J, Cruz-Rizzolo, RJ, Reinoso-Suárez, F (2000). The anatomical connections of the macaque monkey orbitofrontal cortex. A review. Cerebral Cortex 10, 220242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavanagh, JF, Bismark, AJ, Frank, MJ, Allen, J (2011). Larger error signals in major depression are associated with better avoidance learning. Frontiers in Psychology 2, 331.Google Scholar
Cavanagh, JF, Shackman, AJ (2015). Frontal midline theta reflects anxiety and cognitive control: meta-analytic evidence. Journal of Physiology – Paris 109, 315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coan, JA, Allen, JJ, McKnight, PE (2006). A capability model of individual differences in frontal EEG activity. Biological Psychology 72, 198207.Google Scholar
Codispoti, M, De Cesarei, A, Ferrari, V (2012). The influence of color on emotional perception of natural scenes. Psychophysiology 49, 1116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cools, AR (1985). Brain and behavior: Hierarchy of feedback systems and control of input. In Perspectives in Ethology (ed. Bateson, P. P. G. and Klopfer, P. H.), pp. 109168. Plenum: New York.Google Scholar
de Bruijn, ER, Sabbe, BG, Hulstijn, W, Ruigt, GS, Verkes, RJ (2006). Effects of antipsychotic and antidepressant drugs on action monitoring in healthy volunteers. Brain Research 1105, 122129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Debener, S, Ullsperger, M, Siegel, M, Fiehler, K, Von Cramon, DY, Engel, AK (2005). Trial-by-trial coupling of concurrent electroencephalogram and functional magnetic resonance imaging identifies the dynamics of performance monitoring. Journal of Neuroscience 25, 1173011737.Google Scholar
Diniz, JB, Miguel, EC, de Oliveira, AR, Reimer, AE, Brandão, ML, de Mathis, MA, Batistuzzo, MC, Costa, DLC, Hoexter, MQ (2012). Outlining new frontiers for the comprehension of obsessive–compulsive disorder: a review of its relationship with fear and anxiety. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria 34, S81S103.Google Scholar
Edwards, BG, Barch, DM, Braver, TS (2010). Improving prefrontal cortex function in schizophrenia through focused training of cognitive control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 4, 32.Google ScholarPubMed
Endrass, T, Klawohn, J, Schuster, F, Kathmann, N (2008). Overactive performance monitoring in obsessive–compulsive disorder: ERP evidence from correct and erroneous reactions. Neuropsychologia 46, 18771887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Endrass, T, Riesel, A, Kathmann, N, Buhlmann, U (2014). Performance monitoring in obsessive–compulsive disorder and social anxiety disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 123, 705714.Google Scholar
Endrass, T, Schuermann, B, Kaufmann, C, Spielberg, R, Kniesche, R, Kathmann, N (2010). Performance monitoring and error significance in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder. Biological Psychology 84, 257263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Endrass, T, Ullsperger, M (2014). Specificity of performance monitoring changes in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 46, 124138.Google Scholar
Etkin, A, Egner, T, Kalisch, R (2011). Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15, 8593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitzgerald, KD, Welsh, RC, Gehring, WJ, Abelson, JL, Himle, JA, Liberzon, I, Taylor, SF (2005). Error-related hyperactivity of the anterior cingulate cortex in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry 57, 287294.Google Scholar
Foa, EB, Huppert, JD, Leiberg, S, Langner, R, Kichic, R, Hajcak, G, Salkovskis, PM (2002). The obsessive-compulsive inventory: development and validation of a short version. Psychological Assessment 14, 485496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gehring, WJ, Goss, B, Coles, MG, Meyer, DE, Donchin, E (1993). A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science 4, 385390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gehring, WJ, Himle, J, Nisenson, LG (2000). Action-monitoring dysfunction in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychological Science 11, 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, WK, Price, LH, Rasmussen, SA, Mazure, C, Fleischmann, RL, Hill, CL, Heninger, GR, Charney, DS (1989). The Yale–Brown obsessive compulsive scale: I. Development, use, and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry 46, 10061011.Google Scholar
Gründler, TO, Cavanagh, JF, Figueroa, CM, Frank, MJ, Allen, JJ (2009). Task-related dissociation in ERN amplitude as a function of obsessive–compulsive symptoms. Neuropsychologia 47, 19781987.Google Scholar
Grützmann, R, Endrass, T, Klawohn, J, Kathmann, N (2014). Response accuracy rating modulates ERN and Pe amplitudes. Biological Psychology 96, 17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hajcak, G, Franklin, M, Foa, E, Simons, R (2008). Increased error-related brain activity in pediatric obsessive–compulsive disorder before and after treatment. American Journal of Psychiatry 165, 116123.Google Scholar
Hajcak, G, Moser, JS, Yeung, N, Simons, RF (2005). On the ERN and the significance of errors. Psychophysiology 42, 151160.Google Scholar
Hajcak, G, Simons, RF (2002). Error-related brain activity in obsessive-compulsive undergraduates. Psychiatry Research 110, 6372.Google Scholar
Hamilton, M (1959). The assessment of anxiety states by rating. British Journal of Medical Psychology 32, 5055.Google Scholar
Hamilton, M (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 23, 5662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanna, GL, Carrasco, M, Harbin, SM, Nienhuis, JK, LaRosa, CE, Chen, P, Fitzgerald, KD, Gehring, WJ (2012). Error-related negativity and tic history in pediatric obsessive–compulsive disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 51, 902910.Google Scholar
Holroyd, CB, Coles, MG (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review 109, 679709.Google Scholar
Jocham, G, Ullsperger, M (2009). Neuropharmacology of performance monitoring. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33, 4860.Google Scholar
Kiehl, KA, Liddle, PF, Hopfinger, JB (2000). Error processing and the rostral anterior cingulate: an event-related fMRI study. Psychophysiology 37, 216223.Google Scholar
Klawohn, J, Endrass, T, Preuss, J, Riesel, A, Kathmann, N (2015). Modulation of hyperactive error signals in obsessive–compulsive disorder by dual-task demands. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 125, 292298.Google Scholar
Luck, SJ, Kappenman, ES (2011). Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components. Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA.Google Scholar
Melloni, M, Urbistondo, C, Sedeño, L, Gelormini, C, Kichic, R, Ibanez, A (2012). The extended fronto-striatal model of obsessive compulsive disorder: convergence from event-related potentials, neuropsychology and neuroimaging. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6, 124.Google Scholar
Menzies, L, Chamberlain, SR, Laird, AR, Thelen, SM, Sahakian, BJ, Bullmore, ET (2008). Integrating evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies of obsessive–compulsive disorder: the orbitofronto-striatal model revisited. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32, 525549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milad, MR, Rauch, SL (2012). Obsessive–compulsive disorder: beyond segregated cortico-striatal pathways. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16, 4351.Google Scholar
Mohanty, A, Engels, AS, Herrington, JD, Heller, W, Ringo Ho, MH, Banich, MT, Webb, AG, Warren, SL, Miller, GA (2007). Differential engagement of anterior cingulate cortex subdivisions for cognitive and emotional function. Psychophysiology 44, 343351.Google Scholar
Moser, JS, Moran, TP, Schroder, HS, Donnellan, MB, Yeung, N (2013). On the relationship between anxiety and error monitoring: a meta-analysis and conceptual framework. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Connell, RG, Dockree, PM, Bellgrove, MA, Kelly, SP, Hester, R, Garavan, H, Robertson, IH, Foxe, JJ (2007). The role of cingulate cortex in the detection of errors with and without awareness: a high-density electrical mapping study. European Journal of Neuroscience 25, 25712579.Google Scholar
Pitman, RK (1987). A cybernetic model of obsessive-compulsive psychopathology. Comprehensive Psychiatry 28, 334343.Google Scholar
Proudfit, GH, Inzlicht, M, Mennin, DS (2013). Anxiety and error monitoring: the importance of motivation and emotion. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 636.Google Scholar
Ramirez, G, Beilock, SL (2011). Writing about testing worries boosts exam performance in the classroom. Science 331, 211213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riba, J, Rodríguez-Fornells, A, Münte, TF, Barbanoj, MJ (2005). A neurophysiological study of the detrimental effects of alprazolam on human action monitoring. Cognitive Brain Research 25, 554565.Google Scholar
Ridderinkhof, KR, Ullsperger, M, Crone, EA, Nieuwenhuis, S (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science 306, 443447.Google Scholar
Riesel, A, Endrass, T, Auerbach, LA, Kathmann, N (2015). Overactive performance monitoring as an endophenotype for obsessive–compulsive disorder: evidence from a treatment study. American Journal of Psychiatry 172, 665673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riesel, A, Endrass, T, Kaufmann, C, Kathmann, N (2011). Overactive error-related brain activity as a candidate endophenotype for obsessive–compulsive disorder: evidence from unaffected first-degree relatives. American Journal of Psychiatry 168, 317324.Google Scholar
Riesel, A, Kathmann, N, Endrass, T (2014). Overactive performance monitoring in obsessive–compulsive disorder is independent of symptom expression. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 264, 707717.Google Scholar
Riesel, A, Weinberg, A, Endrass, T, Kathmann, N, Hajcak, G (2012). Punishment has a lasting impact on error-related brain activity. Psychophysiology 49, 239247.Google Scholar
Rodman, AM, Milad, MR, Deckersbach, T, Im, J, Chou, T, Dougherty, DD (2012). Neuroimaging contributions to novel surgical treatments for intractable obsessive–compulsive disorder. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics 12, 219227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roh, D, Chang, J-G, Kim, C-H (2016). Emotional interference modulates performance monitoring in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 68, 4451.Google Scholar
Santesso, DL, Segalowitz, SJ, Schmidt, LA (2006). Error-related electrocortical responses are enhanced in children with obsessive–compulsive behaviors. Developmental Neuropsychology 29, 431445.Google Scholar
Saxena, S, Brody, AL, Schwartz, JM, Baxter, LR (1998). Neuroimaging and frontal-subcortical circuitry in obsessive–compulsive disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry 35, 2637.Google Scholar
Semlitsch, HV, Anderer, P, Schuster, P, Presslich, O (1986). A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. Psychophysiology 23, 695703.Google Scholar
Simon, D, Adler, N, Kaufmann, C, Kathmann, N (2014). Amygdala hyperactivation during symptom provocation in obsessive–compulsive disorder and its modulation by distraction. NeuroImage: Clinical 4, 549557.Google Scholar
Simon, D, Kaufmann, C, Kniesche, R, Kischkel, E, Kathmann, N (2013). Autonomic responses and neural-cardiac coupling during individually tailored symptom provocation in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 27, 635644.Google Scholar
Simon, D, Kaufmann, C, Müsch, K, Kischkel, E, Kathmann, N (2010). Fronto-striato-limbic hyperactivation in obsessive–compulsive disorder during individually tailored symptom provocation. Psychophysiology 47, 728738.Google Scholar
Simon, D, Kischkel, E, Spielberg, R, Kathmann, N (2012). A pilot study on the validity of using pictures and videos for individualized symptom provocation in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Research 198, 8188.Google Scholar
Spielberger, CD, Lushene, RE (1966). State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) . Consulting Psychologists Press: California.Google Scholar
Stern, ER, Liu, Y, Gehring, WJ, Lister, JJ, Yin, G, Zhang, J, Fitzgerald, KD, Himle, JA, Abelson, JL, Taylor, SF (2010). Chronic medication does not affect hyperactive error responses in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychophysiology 47, 913920.Google Scholar
Summerfeldt, L, Kloosterman, P, Parker, J, Antony, M, Swinson, R (2001). Assessing and validating the obsessive-compulsive-related construct of incompleteness. In Annual Convention of the Canadian Psychological Association, Ste-Foy, Quebec. Abstract published in Canadian Psychology.Google Scholar
Tang, Y, Zhang, X, Simmonite, M, Li, H, Zhang, T, Guo, Q, Li, C, Fang, Y, Xu, Y, Wang, J (2013). Hyperactivity within an extensive cortical distribution associated with excessive sensitivity in error processing in unmedicated depression: a combined event-related potential and sLORETA study. International Journal of Psychophysiology 90, 282289.Google Scholar
Vaidyanathan, U, Nelson, LD, Patrick, CJ (2012). Clarifying domains of internalizing psychopathology using neurophysiology. Psychological Medicine 42, 447459.Google Scholar
Weinberg, A, Kotov, R, Proudfit, GH (2015). Neural indicators of error processing in generalized anxiety disorder, obsessive–compulsive disorder, and major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 124, 172185.Google Scholar
Weinberg, A, Olvet, DM, Hajcak, G (2010). Increased error-related brain activity in generalized anxiety disorder. Biological Psychology 85, 472480.Google Scholar
Weinberg, A, Riesel, A, Hajcak, G (2012). Integrating multiple perspectives on error-related brain activity: The ERN as a neural indicator of trait defensive reactivity. Motivation and Emotion 36, 84100.Google Scholar
Wiswede, D, Münte, TF, Goschke, T, Rüsseler, J (2009). Modulation of the error-related negativity by induction of short-term negative affect. Neuropsychologia 47, 8390.Google Scholar
Xiao, Z, Wang, J, Zhang, M, Li, H, Tang, Y, Wang, Y, Fan, Q, Fromson, JA (2011). Error-related negativity abnormalities in generalized anxiety disorder and obsessive–compulsive disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 35, 265272.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Roh supplementary material

Tables S1-S4

Download Roh supplementary material(File)
File 22.9 KB