Background
Executive functioning (EF) refers to higher-level cognitive abilities that enable behavior in a goal-directed manner, allowing the individual to adapt to changing or unfamiliar circumstances. EF is primarily related to frontal lobe functions (Diamond, Reference Diamond2013; Friedman & Miyake, Reference Friedman and Miyake2017). EF is a broad cognitive domain often clustered into three main functions: response inhibition, working memory updating, and mental flexibility which are characterized as shared, yet distinct functions Adaptive human behavior is widely dependent on EF, playing a vital role in daily life, academic achievement, and occupational success (Miller, Nevado-Montenegro, & Hinshaw, Reference Miller, Nevado-Montenegro and Hinshaw2012; Pearson et al., Reference Pearson, Bornstein, Cordero, Scerif, Mahedy, Evans, Abioye and Stein2016). Consequently, individuals who exhibit good performance in EF tend to demonstrate resilience towards adverse life events and generally experience a higher quality of life (Diamond, Reference Diamond2013; Masten & Tellegen, Reference Masten and Tellegen2012; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Nevado-Montenegro and Hinshaw2012). Conversely, individuals with impaired EF are more prone to risk-seeking behavior and externalizing problems (McKewen et al., Reference McKewen, Skippen, Cooper, Wong, Michie, Lenroot and Karayanidis2019; Mullin, Perks, Haraden, Snyder, & Hankin, Reference Mullin, Perks, Haraden, Snyder and Hankin2020). Lastly, impaired EF increases vulnerability to various manifestations of psychopathology, and impaired EF is common across most neuropsychiatric disorders, especially neurodevelopmental disorders (Diamond, Reference Diamond2013; Harden et al., Reference Harden, Engelhardt, Mann, Patterson, Grotzinger, Savicki, Thibodeaux, Freis, Tackett, Church and Tucker-Drob2020). Hence, impaired EF has been suggested as a transdiagnostic factor and thus an important endophenotypic trait (Zelazo, Reference Zelazo2020).
Interleukin 6 (IL-6), traditionally associated with immune response, has also been shown to play a role in neurodevelopment, influencing neuronal proliferation, cellular differentiation, and neuronal cell survival (Hsiao & Patterson, Reference Hsiao and Patterson2011; Wu, Hsiao, Yan, Mazmanian, & Patterson, Reference Wu, Hsiao, Yan, Mazmanian and Patterson2017). Rapidly produced in response to changes in homeostasis, such as infections and injuries, IL-6 is linked to the subsequent production of the acute-phase reactant C-reactive protein (CRP) (Del Giudice & Gangestad, Reference Del Giudice and Gangestad2018). Furthermore, IL-6 is considered a central mediator of the effects of the Maternal Immune Activation hypothesis, suggesting that intrauterine inflammatory disturbances can have adverse effects on fetal brain development (Smith, Li, Garbett, Mirnics, & Patterson, Reference Smith, Li, Garbett, Mirnics and Patterson2007; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Hsiao, Yan, Mazmanian and Patterson2017). Ultimately, perturbations resulting in elevated maternal IL-6 levels during pregnancy are hypothesized to contribute to neurodevelopmental disorders, such as pervasive developmental disorders, hyperkinetic disorder, attention deficit disorder without hyperactivity, and schizophrenia (Allswede, Yolken, Buka, & Cannon, Reference Allswede, Yolken, Buka and Cannon2020; Gustafsson et al., Reference Gustafsson, Sullivan, Battison, Holton, Graham, Karalunas, Fair, Loftis and Nigg2020; Hsiao & Patterson, Reference Hsiao and Patterson2011; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Hsiao, Yan, Mazmanian and Patterson2017).
Maternal IL-6 levels during pregnancy have also been linked to a negative impact on the intelligence of the offspring (Rasmussen et al., Reference Rasmussen, Graham, Entringer, Gilmore, Styner, Fair, Wadhwa and Buss2019). To our knowledge, only a limited number of studies have evaluated the association between higher inflammation during pregnancy and compromised EF in the offspring. One cohort study (N=84) reported that higher IL-6 during pregnancy was associated with reduced efficiency of impulse control (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Rasmussen, Rudolph, Heim, Gilmore, Styner, Potkin, Entringer, Wadhwa, Fair and Buss2018) and working memory (Rudolph et al., Reference Rudolph, Graham, Feczko, Miranda-Dominguez, Rasmussen, Nardos, Entringer, Wadhwa, Buss and Fair2018) in early life. Another study (N=40) found an indirect association between IL-6 and preschool EF through infant cognitive ability (Camerota et al., Reference Camerota, Wylie, Goldblum, Wideman, Cheatham and Propper2022). Dozmorov et al (N=246) did not find any significant association between maternal IL-6 and EF in the offspring (Dozmorov et al., Reference Dozmorov, Bilbo, Kollins, Zucker, Do, Schechter and Fuemmeler2018). Finally, a study by Morgan et al. (N=100) found higher maternal CRP levels during the third trimester were associated with poorer cognitive flexibility in offspring aged 4–6 years (Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Lee, Mahrer, Guardino, Davis, Shalowitz and Dunkel Schetter2020).
In addition to exposure to inflammation during pregnancy, several environmental factors have been shown to influence EF. These include maternal and gestational factors, for example, birth weight, gestational age, preeclampsia, maternal age, smoking during pregnancy, maternal perinatal mental health, prenatal obesity, and socioeconomic status (Micalizzi & Knopik, Reference Micalizzi and Knopik2018; Mina et al., Reference Mina, Lahti, Drake, Denison, Räikkönen, Norman and Reynolds2017; Pearson et al., Reference Pearson, Bornstein, Cordero, Scerif, Mahedy, Evans, Abioye and Stein2016; Rätsep et al., Reference Rätsep, Hickman, Maser, Pudwell, Smith, Brien, Stroman, Adams, Reynolds, Croy and Paolozza2016; Sarsour et al., Reference Sarsour, Sheridan, Jutte, Nuru-Jeter, Hinshaw and Boyce2011; van Houdt, Oosterlaan, van Wassenaer-Leemhuis, van Kaam, & Aarnoudse-Moens, Reference van Houdt, Oosterlaan, van Wassenaer-Leemhuis, van Kaam and Aarnoudse-Moens2019).
Finally, EF etiology has a substantial genetic component, with twin studies reporting high heritability estimates when treated as a single common factor (Friedman & Miyake, Reference Friedman and Miyake2017; Miguel, Meaney, & Silveira, Reference Miguel, Meaney and Silveira2023), the underlying genetic structure of EF is complex, and genome-wide association studies reveal a highly polygenic nature, with polygenic risk scores (PRS) currently explaining 10.4–18.9% of the variance (Hatoum et al., Reference Hatoum, Morrison, Mitchell, Lam, Benca-Bachman, Reineberg, Palmer, Evans, Keller and Friedman2023; Miguel et al., Reference Miguel, Meaney and Silveira2023).
We used data from The COpenhagen Prospective Study on Neuro-PSYCHiatric Development (COPSYCH) study of the prospective Copenhagen Prospective Studies on Asthma in Childhood 2010 (COPSAC2010) birth cohort including 700 mother-child pairs representative of the general population. We investigated a association between maternal IL-6 levels (main analyses) and hs-CRP (secondary analyses) in pregnancy week 24 and every day- and performance-based EF in the offspring, at age 10. We hypothesized that a higher level of maternal inflammation would be associated with impaired EF in the child in middle childhood.
Methods
Study design and participants
The COPSYCH study included 10-year-old children from the deeply phenotyped prospective mother-child cohort, COPSAC2010, clinically assessed from January 2019 to December 2021 (Mohammadzadeh et al., Reference Mohammadzadeh, Rosenberg, Vinding, Møllegaard Jepsen, Lindberg, Følsgaard, Erlang Sørensen, Sulaiman, Bilenberg, Mitta Raghava, Fagerlund, Vestergaard, Pantelis, Stokholm, Chawes, Larsson, Glenthøj, Bønnelykke, Ebdrup and Bisgaard2022). All raters who administered the tests received extensive training and continuous supervision (anonymized). The COPSAC2010 cohort includes 700 unselected mother-child pairs where the children have undergone extensive phenotyping and biological sampling from 15 clinical visits from pregnancy to age 10 years (Bisgaard et al., Reference Bisgaard, Vissing, Carson, Bischoff, Følsgaard, Kreiner-Møller, Chawes, Stokholm, Pedersen, Bjarnadóttir, Thysen, Nilsson, Mortensen, Olsen, Schjørring, Krogfelt, Lauritzen, Brix and Bønnelykke2013). The COPSAC2010 includes a double-blinded, randomized, and placebo-controlled study, in which pregnant women received either fish oil (2.4 g/day of n-3 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids or placebo, and/or high dose Vitamin D (70 microgram/day) or recommended standard dose Vitamin D (10 microgram/day) from 24 weeks of gestation until one week after birth. The primary outcome of the trial was asthma and persistent wheezing at age 3, and findings have been reported elsewhere (Bisgaard et al., Reference Bisgaard, Stokholm, Chawes, Vissing, Bjarnadóttir, Schoos, Wolsk, Pedersen, Vinding, Thorsteinsdóttir, Følsgaard, Fink, Thorsen, Pedersen, Waage, Rasmussen, Stark, Olsen and Bønnelykke2016; Chawes et al., Reference Chawes, Bønnelykke, Stokholm, Vissing, Bjarnadóttir, Schoos, Wolsk, Pedersen, Vinding, Thorsteinsdóttir, Arianto, Hallas, Heickendorff, Brix, Rasmussen and Bisgaard2016).
For twin pairs, Twin B was excluded to ensure data independence, as five pairs of twins were included in COPSYCH. Additionally, children with a birth weight under 1,500 g or a gestational age under 28 weeks were excluded, though none of the participants in the COPSYCH visit met these criteria. Initial quality control was performed on the raw data, excluding unconfirmed extremely high values of biomarkers; one mother with an exceptionally high CRP level that could not be confirmed as an infections using a registered prescription of antibiotics (N=1). All IL-6 values were within expected ranges.
Maternal inflammation
Blood levels of IL-6 and hs-CRP were measured at 24 weeks gestation for mothers. Serum IL-6 and hs-CRP were collected in EDTA tubes and blood samples were centrifuged and stored at −80° C. Concentrations were determined by a high-sensitivity electrochemiluminescence assay from MesoScale Discovery with a lower level of detection of 0.178 pg/mL for IL-6 and 0.007 ng/mL for hs-CRP. Samples were analyzed in duplicate and read using the Sector Image 2400A (Meso Scale Discovery, Gaithersburg, MD). Overall CV coefficients for IL6: 1.99% and for hs-CRP: 1.57%.
Psychopathology
Neurodevelopment, as reflected by neurocognitive and psychopathological indices, was comprehensively assessed at the COPSYCH 10-year visit (Mohammadzadeh et al., Reference Mohammadzadeh, Rosenberg, Vinding, Møllegaard Jepsen, Lindberg, Følsgaard, Erlang Sørensen, Sulaiman, Bilenberg, Mitta Raghava, Fagerlund, Vestergaard, Pantelis, Stokholm, Chawes, Larsson, Glenthøj, Bønnelykke, Ebdrup and Bisgaard2022). Research diagnoses were based on the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL) as well as any other available clinical and anamnestic information (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Rao, Flynn, Moreci, Williamson and Ryan1997). K-SADS-PL interviews were administered by trained personnel, video recorded, and supervised by a senior research psychologist with a specialty in child and adolescent psychiatry (anonymized). Consensus diagnoses or status as mentally healthy was established at a weekly diagnostic conference, followed by external validation from a clinical professor of child and adolescent psychiatry (anonymized). The distribution of diagnoses according to the ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioral Disorders, 10th Revision (WHO, 1992) is provided in Table 1. We defined children with any neurodevelopmental disorder, according to ICD-10 codes, as follows: Pervasive Developmental Disorders (F84, F84.5, F84.8); Other Disorders of Psychological Development (F88); and Unspecified disorder of psychological development (F89), Hyperkinetic disorders (F90.0, F90.8); Attention deficit disorder without hyperactivity (F98.8); Chronic motor or vocal tics (F95.1); Tourette syndrome (F95.2).
Table 1. Baseline characteristics

Diagnoses: Frequency of diagnoses (Incl. phobia, enuresis, transient tic) found in the COPSYCH study (N=593, number of children that completed KSADS) according to classification. ‘–’ Indicates no diagnosis in that category. Idividuals presenting more than one condition are included in all the diagnostic categories. ICD-10 codes: codes for the diagnoses in the Classification of Mental and Behavioral disorders (ref); N (%): number of cases (percentage). Following diagnoses are included in the following categories: F20–29: Psychotic disorder (F28), F40–49: Anxiety disorder (phobia) (40.1, 40.2, 40.8), Obsessive-compulsive disorder (F42, F42.1, F42.2), Adjustment disorder (43.2), F80–89: Pervasive developmental disorder (F84, F84.5, F84.8), other neurodevelopmental disorders (F88 and F89), Hyperkinetic disorder (F90.0*, F90.8*), Conduct disorders (F91.1 F91.2 F91.3), Anxiety (F93.0, F93.2, F93.8), Transient tic disorder (F95.0), Chronic motor or vocal tics (F95.1), Tourette (F95.2), Non-organic nuresis, non-organic encopresis (F98.0, F98.1), Attention deficit disorder without hyperactivity (F98.8)
Executive functioning
EF was assessed using two sources of data: an everyday executive functioning rating scale and performance-based EF test outcomes.
The parents evaluated the child’s everyday executive functioning behavior using the parental version of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, second edition (BRIEF-2). BRIEF-2 is a 63-item questionnaire covering a broad range of executive functioning. BRIEF-2 has nine clinical subscales: Inhibit, Self-Monitor, Shift, Emotional Control, Initiate/Task Completion, Working Memory, Plan/Organize, Task-Monitor, Organization of Materials in their daily environment (G. A. Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kemworthy, Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kemworthy2015). All items are rated on a three-point Likert scale assessing frequency (‘Never’, ‘Sometimes’, ‘Often’) of the behavior during the last 6 months. The subscales can be combined into three index scores: Behavior Regulation Index, Emotion Regulation Index, and Cognitive Regulation Index. The sum of the three index scores represents the total score, denoted the Global Executive Composite score (Gioia et al., Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kemworthy2015).
BRIEF-2 is validated in children aged 5–18 years, showing a high test-retest reliability, and standardized scores derived from normative samples are available (Davidson, Cherry, & Corkum, Reference Davidson, Cherry and Corkum2016; Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kenworthy2010). BRIEF-2 has previously been used in both neurotypical children and children with neurodevelopmental disorders (Thorup et al., Reference Thorup, Jepsen, Ellersgaard, Burton, Christiani, Hemager, Skjærbæk, Ranning, Spang, Gantriis, Greve, Zahle, Mors, Plessen and Nordentoft2015). For interpretation purposes, T scores (~2 SD) of 70 or higher are indicative of clinical levels of problems (Gioia et al., Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kemworthy2015). In the present cohort, this threshold corresponded to a Global Executive Composite raw score of 133.0 in boys and 122.7 in girls (Figure 1).

Figure 1. Association between maternal IL-6 level and Executive Composite Score, BRIEF 2, sex-stratified. Interleukin 6 in pregnancy week 24 (log2 transformed) and Global Executive Composite score, Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, second edition, in offspring at age 10. Stratified by sex.
The neuropsychological battery included a broad range of well-validated tests (Andrikopoulos, Reference Andrikopoulos2021; De Luca et al., Reference De Luca, Wood, Anderson, Buchanan, Proffitt, Mahony and Pantelis2003; Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & Holdnack, Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Holdnack2004). From the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) (Fray & Robbins, Reference Fray and Robbins1996; Sahakian & Owen, Reference Sahakian and Owen1992) we included following EF: Spatial Working Memory (SWM), testing spatial working memory and strategy formation; One-touch Stockings of Cambridge (OTS) testing planning; Spatial Span (SSP) testing spatial short-term memory, and Intra-Extra Dimensional Set Shifting (IED), testing mental flexibility (Table 2).
Table 2. Executive functioning outcomes

WISC-IV, wechsler intelligence scale for children—fourth edition.
D-KEFS, Delis-Kaplan executive function system.
CANTAB, Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery.
BRIEF-2, the behaviour rating inventory of executive function—second edition.
¤Summary score calculated based on the three sub-indices Behavior regulating index, Cognitive regulating index, Emotion regulating index.
a Extra-dimensional stage errors denote the number of errors made in the extra-dimensional stage of the task, where the child is required to make an extradimensional shift.
b Span length denotes the longest sequence successfully recalled by the child (three attempts at each level).
c Total errors comprise the number of times a box is selected that does not have a hidden target that must be found, and therefore, should not have been visited by the child.
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, Second Edition.
d Estimated full-scale intelligence quotient, based on N=589 who underwent complete testing on monolingual children (bilingual did not perform the WISC vocabulary test, and therefore, they were not included).
From the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) (David, Reference David2003) we included the Digit Span, and Letter–Number Sequencing subtests to examine verbal working memory. Finally, Trail Making 4 from the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS), was used to measure mental flexibility (Delis, Kaplan, & Kramer, Reference Delis, Kaplan and Kramer2001). In addition to EF, we estimated the full-scale intelligence quotient (FSIQ) based on the following subtests from WISC-IV: Matrices, Vocabulary, Digit Span, Letter-Number Sequencing, Coding, and Symbol Search.
Polygenic risk score for EF
In the absence of a maternal EF measure, we explored the genetic contribution to the children’s EF by generating a polygenic risk score (EF-PRS) for the children and mothers (EF-PRS), separately based on a genome-wide association study of executive function in 427,037 individuals from the UK Biobank (Hatoum et al., Reference Hatoum, Morrison, Mitchell, Lam, Benca-Bachman, Reineberg, Palmer, Evans, Keller and Friedman2023).
For details on the construction of PRS, see Supplementary, Methods section.
Weighing of outcome measures
EF rating scales and EF tests offer complementary information, but EF rating scales have higher ecological validity, and generally predict EF outcomes better than laboratory EF tests (Toplak, West, & Stanovich, Reference Toplak, West and Stanovich2013). Therefore, our primary outcome was the overall summary score from BRIEF-2, the Global Executive Composite score, and the three index scores: Behavior Regulation Index, Emotion Regulation Index, and Cognitive Regulation Index. Our secondary outcome was from a principal component analysis (PCA) of the neuropsychological EF test outcomes (EF-PCA). The main reasons for using PCA were to reduce variables for statistical efficiency and lower the risk of Type I errors. Additionally, PCA aligns with the theoretical framework of EF, considered ‘shared but distinct,’ allowing for collective examination (Friedman & Miyake, Reference Friedman and Miyake2017). We included nine EF outcome variables from CANTAB, WISC-IV, and D-KEFS (Table 2) based on z-scored values from participants with complete EF test datasets. Four of the variables were inverted prior to making the PCA align so that higher z-scores consistently represented better performance (SWM strategy, SWM between errors, IED errors, and D-KEFS trail making).
Multiple testing correction using the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure was applied specifically to the four main outcomes from the BRIEF-2, with IL-6 as the primary determinant. The additional analyses were either secondary (hs-CRP) or exploratory and were not subjected to multiple testing corrections.
Strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ)
We included the SDQ as an outcome measure as a supplementary analysis, focusing on the externalizing subscale and the total difficulties score. The externalizing subscale consists of the behavioral and hyperactivity subscales (Goodman et al., 2010). For a description of the SDQ and the outcomes, see Supplementary Methods section.
Statistical analysis
Main, secondary, and exploratory analysis
Maternal IL-6 and hs-CRP were log2 transformed in all analyses, for example, an increase in 1 is equivalent to a doubling of IL-6 and hs-CRP, respectively. The purpose was to depict the association across the whole range. Scores from BRIEF-2 are presented as raw scores. Missing data in the BRIEF-2 questionnaire were imputed by simple imputation; the first missing value, it was replaced by the value of 1, as indicated in the BRIEF-2 manual (Gioia et al., Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kemworthy2015). Subsequent missing values were imputed by the rounded mean of the remaining values within the same subscale.
All associations between the two maternal biomarkers and EF were investigated using linear regression, with IL-6-EF associations as the main analyses and hs-CRP-EF associations as secondary analyses. Associations were adjusted for: the child’s sex, gestational age, birth weight, parity, maternal and paternal age, pre-pregnancy maternal Body Mass Index (BMI), maternal education level, smoking during pregnancy, alcohol use during pregnancy, maternal diabetes during pregnancy, preeclampsia, antibiotic use during pregnancy, maternal EF-PRS, and household income. Furthermore, we adjusted for the RCT interventions (Vitamin D and fish oil).
In case the overall association between IL-6, respectively, hs-CRP and EF were significant we tested for interaction between the association and the following: sex, maternal education, household income, maternal pre-pregnancy BMI, birth weight, and any neurodevelopmental disorder present/absent, with Global Executive Composite score from BRIEF-2 as outcome. We evaluated whether the presence of neurodevelopmental disorders influenced an association by stratifying our analysis (exploratory). First, we excluded children with these diagnoses and performed the analysis. Then, we analyzed only children with neurodevelopmental disorders. We also conducted further analyses stratified by both sex and NDD status (results in Supplementary). All analyses were performed ‘unadjusted’, with age and sex being the only adjustments made, unless explicitly stated otherwise. This ‘unadjusted’ model included these two covariates given the significant influence of age at examination and sex on cognitive performance. In addition, we report estimates based on an ‘adjusted’ model, where the estimate was adjusted for both sex and age, along with all the covariates specified above. All results presented in the text are based on the main adjusted analyses unless otherwise specified. Results were reported as beta-coefficients with a 95% confidence interval [95%CI]. In all analyses, statistical significance was set as p-value<0.05 (two-sided). P-values for multiple BRIEF-2 outcomes were adjusted with the Benjamini–Hochberg correction method, and significance was determined based on these adjusted p-values.
Statistical analyses were performed using R statistical software (version R4.2.1, R Core Team (2022)).
Sensitivity analyses
The specificity of IL-6 and EF was investigated with an association analysis of estimated full-scale intelligence quotient and IL-6 levels. Furthermore, we performed correlational analyses using Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient, between EF-PCA outcomes and all nine clinical BRIEF-2 subscales. Finally, we repeated the models without PRS adjustment for all significant main analyses to assess potential genetic confounding, as PRS explains only a small fraction of the genetic contribution.
Supplementary analysis
For the SDQ supplementary analyses, we conducted both unadjusted and adjusted linear regression analyses to examine the associations between maternal inflammatory markers and SDQ outcomes. Additionally, we used Spearman’s correlation analysis to assess the bivariate relationships between the BRIEF-2 GEC and SDQ outcomes.
Results
Baseline
Six hundred and four children (86.3% of the original cohort) participated in the 10-year visit (see Table 1 for demographics). A total of five children met the exclusion criteria and this is a study of 599 children in total (85.6% of the original cohort). There were no significant differences in baseline characteristics between participants in the 10-year visit and non-participants (Supplementary Table S1). We had complete data for 577 children in the unadjusted analysis of maternal IL-6 and EF, and complete data including all covariates for 544 children (Figure S1).
Five hundred and fifty-one children with complete data (95.5% of 577) underwent psychopathological assessment, and 80 (13.9% out of 577) children fulfilled the criteria for at least one neurodevelopmental disorder (Table 1).
For baseline IL-6, hs-CRP, and BRIEF-2 data analyses, see Supplementary, Results section.
Main analyses
Maternal pregnancy IL-6 levels and BRIEF-2 outcomes in offspring at 10 years of age
A total of 547 children (90.6%) had complete BRIEF-2 data. Nine (1.5%) did not fill out any items in the questionnaire (63/63 missing), 40 (6.6%) missed 1/63 values and were assigned the value ‘1’, and 8 (1.3%) had 2-11/63 missing values (Gioia et al., Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kemworthy2015).
IL-6 in pregnancy was significantly associated with EF measured by the Global Executive Composite score (β[95%CI]=2.48 [0.80–4.16], p=0.004), indicating a 2.48 higher point score in EF problems per log2 increase in IL-6 (Figure 1). When subdivided into the three index scores we also observed significant associations with Behavior Regulation (β[95%CI]=0.52 [0.15–0.88] p=0.006), Emotion Regulation Index (0.58 [0.03–1.12] p=0.04), and Cognitive Regulation Index (1.39 [0.40–2.38], p=0.007) (Figure 2 and Supplementary Table S2).

Figure 2. Forest plot showing an association between EF outcomes and maternal IL-6. Forest plot showing estimates and confidence intervals from linear regression analyses of maternal interleukin 6 level at pregnancy week 24 and A) Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, second edition outcomes at age 10. (black=adjusted, grey=unadjusted); B) Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, second edition outcomes at age 10, gender stratified (red=girls, blue=boys); and C) Executive Functioning-Principal Component Analysis outcomes at age 10, adjusted.
Maternal pregnancy IL-6 levels and performance-based indices of executive functions
A total of 581 (96.19%) children who underwent cognitive examination had complete data. The PCA of the executive functioning test outcomes resulted in three components with an eigenvalue >1.00, and accounting for 55.17% of the variance. The components represented Verbal working memory with a 28.62% variance explained, Spatial working memory, 14.45% variance explained, and Planning, 12.10% variance explained (Figure S2).
We found a significant association between maternal IL-6 levels in pregnancy and the Verbal working memory component from the EF-PCA, but this finding became non-significant after adjusting for covariates (unadjusted: −0.16 [−0.29 to −0.04], p=0.01, adjusted: β[95%CI]=−0.09 [−0.22–0.04], p=0.17). We did not find a significant association between maternal IL-6 and the Spatial working memory or Planning components (Figure 2 and Supplementary Table S6).
Individual analyses for each EF test outcome can be found in Supplementary Table S7. All results were insignificant after adjustment.
Secondary analyses
Maternal hs-CRP week 24 in pregnancy and EF outcomes
A significant association with BRIEF-2 outcomes was found in the unadjusted analyses but became insignificant after adjustment (Supplementary Table S3). No associations were found with EF-PCA (p-values > 0.12) or with the individual neuropsychological test outcomes (Supplementary Table S4).
Explorative IL-6 analyses – analyses of effect modification
Effect modification by sex
Parents reported more frequent EF problems among boys than girls (Global Executive Composite girls: median [IQR]=77 [68.0–89.5] versus Global Executive Composite boys: median [IQR]=85 [72.0–104.25], p<0.001) (Figure 3). There was a significant interaction between maternal IL-6 levels in pregnancy and the child’s sex in relation to EF (p=0.01). Sex-stratified analyses showed that the association between the Global Executive Composite score and maternal IL-6 in pregnancy was only significant in boys (Global Executive Composite, males: β[95%CI]=5.28 [2.19–8.37], p<0.001; Global Executive Composite score, girls, 1.08 [−0.91–3.06], p=0.29). Behavior Regulation Index, Emotion Regulation Index, and Cognitive Regulation Index also exhibited significant associations with the maternal IL-6 level in pregnancy week 24 only in boys (Figure 2 and Supplementary Table S2).

Figure 3. Distribution of the Global Executive Composite score from BRIEF-2, sex stratified. Histogram showing the Global Executive Composite scores (raw scores) from the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, second edition for girls and boys. Higher scores correspond to poorer performance.
Effect modification by neurodevelopmental disorders
Analyses evaluating the children according to the absence or presence of any neurodevelopmental disorder did not yield significant associations between maternal IL-6 levels in pregnancy week 24. Excluding children with neurodevelopmental diagnoses (N=472), yielded β[95%CI]=0.99 [−0.51–2.50], p=0.20. Analyzing only children with neurodevelopmental disorders (N=80), resulting in β[95%CI]=−0.45 [−6.46–5.55], p=0.88.
There were no significant associations to any of the three BRIEF-2 index scores in any of the groups (p>0.17).
Further analysis stratified by sex and according to absence or presence of any neurodevelopmental disorder, can be found in Supplementary Table S5.
The remaining interaction analyses are provided in Supplementary Table S10.
Sensitivity analyses
Associations between BRIEF-2 and EF-PCA
The verbal working memory component was weakly but significantly correlated with most BRIEF-2 subscales. Only a few correlations were observed between the Spatial working memory, respectively, Planning component, and BRIEF-2 subscales (Supplementary Table S8).
Maternal IL-6 at 24 weeks pregnancy and estimated FSIQ quotient at age 10
The mean estimated FSIQ for all children was 102.7 (SD=12.1), and slightly higher for girls compared to boys (girls, mean (SD) =104.4(11.3), boys, mean (SD)=101.1(12.7), p<0.001). There was no association between estimated FSIQ and maternal IL-6 during pregnancy (p=0.29). Furthermore, we found no interaction with sex and no significant associations with IL-6 in sex stratified analyses (p-values>0.32).
Maternal IL-6 at 24 weeks pregnancy and EF outcomes without PRS adjustment
Analyses without PRS adjustment showed largely consistent results for all significant main and explorative effect modification analyses (see Supplementary Table S9).
Supplementary analyses
Maternal inflammatory markers and SDQ outcomes at age 10
There were significant associations between IL-6 and hs-CRP, respectively, and SDQ outcomes: IL-6 and SDQ total difficulties: Adjusted β[95%CI], p-value: 0.57 [0.168, 0.977], 0.006, IL-6 and SDQ externalizing: 0.36 [0.094, 0.618], 0.008, hs-CRP and SDQ total difficulties score: 0.29 [0.020, 0.558], 0.036 hs-CRP and SDQ externalizing 0.23 [0.054, 0.402], 0.011, (full results in Supplementary Table S11).
BRIEF-2 and SDQ outcomes at age 10
BRIEF-2 GEC and SDQ outcomes were highly correlated (Supplementary Table S12).
Contributing factors to maternal IL-6 levels during pregnancy
In our covariate association analysis, evaluating contributing factors to IL-6, we found that maternal pre-pregnancy BMI was highly associated with maternal IL-6 levels (unadjusted p<0.001) (Supplementary Table S13). Likewise, in our supplementary analysis, we noted a similar strong association between maternal pre-pregnancy BMI and hs-CRP levels (unadjusted p<0.001).
Discussion
This study explores thoroughly adjusted analyses of associations between maternal inflammation during pregnancy and EF in middle childhood.
Our main analysis showed a significant association between higher maternal IL-6 levels in pregnancy week 24 and less efficient every day, but not performance-based EF in offspring at the age of 10 years. Specifically, a doubling in maternal IL-6 level (due to the log2 transformation) was associated with a 2.48 points higher score (β[95%CI]=2.48 [0.80- 4.16], p=0.004), on the BRIEF-2 Global Executive Composite score (GEC). A higher score was observed across all three index scores, meaning more problems and thus less efficient EF evident in multiple domains: Behavior Regulation Index, Emotion Regulation Index, and Cognitive Regulation Index.
In our exploratory analysis, when stratifying the analyses based on sex, we found that these associations were solely present in boys, suggesting that only male offspring are susceptible to high levels of IL-6 during pregnancy.
Additionally, stratifying the analysis based on whether or not children had an NDD did not reveal a significant association between IL-6 and EF in children.
A subsequent subanalysis (data not shown) found that the association remained evident for two indices only, namely the GEC and the Behavioral Regulation Index among boys without an NDD. However, this association was not observed among boys with an NDD, possibly due to the limited sample size (N=59). This sex-specific sensitivity to IL-6 inflammation has been previously observed in a recent study where elevated IL-6 levels during pregnancy were associated with the severity of depressive symptoms in male offspring but not in female offspring (Dahabreh & Bibbins-Domingo, Reference Dahabreh and Bibbins-Domingo2024). These data could suggest that the fetal developing brain, particularly the male brain, appears to be vulnerable to an adverse intrauterine environment (Kaczkurkin, Raznahan, & Satterthwaite, Reference Kaczkurkin, Raznahan and Satterthwaite2019; McCarthy & Arnold, Reference McCarthy and Arnold2011). One potential explanation might be that male brains develop at a slower pace and thereby are more vulnerable to environmental exposures (Bale, Reference Bale2016).
Overall, our results are in line with prior findings from another cohort that also found elevated IL-6 levels in pregnancy were associated with diminished EF efficiency (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Rasmussen, Rudolph, Heim, Gilmore, Styner, Potkin, Entringer, Wadhwa, Fair and Buss2018; Rudolph et al., Reference Rudolph, Graham, Feczko, Miranda-Dominguez, Rasmussen, Nardos, Entringer, Wadhwa, Buss and Fair2018). We did not find an association between the child’s 6 months IL-6 levels and EF at age 10, nor estimated FSIQ, indicating that IL-6 in pregnancy might be adverse specifically for EF.
Our secondary analyses showed no association between elevated maternal hs-CRP and any EF outcomes after covariate adjustment.
Our supplementary analysis explored the relationship between inflammatory markers, BRIEF-2 outcomes, and the SDQ to better understand how behavioral problems intersect with executive function impairments and inflammatory markers.
The BRIEF-2 measures EF in real-life contexts by capturing behavioral indicators of impairment. The BRIEF-2 GEC substantially overlaps with behavioral problems assessed by the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ), as greater EF impairments on the BRIEF-2 are associated with more severe behavioral problems on the SDQ, underscoring their close relationship (Ng et al., Reference Ng, Bjornsson, Fahrner and Harris2024).
We found that IL-6 and hs-CRP were significantly associated with both SDQ outcomes (externalizing subscale and total difficulties score), supporting their role in neurodevelopmental processes and behavioral dysregulation (Allswede et al., Reference Allswede, Yolken, Buka and Cannon2020; Gustafsson et al., Reference Gustafsson, Sullivan, Battison, Holton, Graham, Karalunas, Fair, Loftis and Nigg2020; Hsiao & Patterson, Reference Hsiao and Patterson2011; Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Lee, Mahrer, Guardino, Davis, Shalowitz and Dunkel Schetter2020; Rasmussen et al., Reference Rasmussen, Graham, Entringer, Gilmore, Styner, Fair, Wadhwa and Buss2019; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Li, Garbett, Mirnics and Patterson2007; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Hsiao, Yan, Mazmanian and Patterson2017). The findings indicate that these inflammatory markers are associated with both behavioral problems and everyday EF, with a somewhat stronger association with EF.
To examine the specificity of BRIEF-2 outcomes, we conducted a bivariate correlation analysis, which revealed strong associations between the BRIEF-2 GEC and both SDQ outcomes. This suggests that the BRIEF-2 captures EF aspects that substantially overlap with broader behavioral problems. The shared variance highlights their close relationship, though these associations may be inflated due to overlapping item content (Ng et al., Reference Ng, Bjornsson, Fahrner and Harris2024). Additionally, these associations may have been inflated by shared method variance, as both measures were parent-reported. These limitations should be considered when interpreting the results.
Cognitive heterogeneity in findings for performance-based EF test scores and BRIEF-2 ratings
Our primary associations were predominantly evident in the outcomes derived from parental-rated BRIEF-2. Although we initially identified an association between maternal IL-6 levels during pregnancy week 24 and the Verbal Working Memory component of the EF-PCA, this association became non-significant after adjusting for covariates. Furthermore, we observed no associations with IL-6 inflammation for the remaining neuropsychological outcomes (Spatial Working memory, respectively, Planning component). We predominantly found non-significant or very weak correlations in our correlation analysis between the lab-based tests and BRIEF-2. This difference in the results aligns with prior research, from across various clinical groups, including individuals with ADHD, emphasizing that performance-based tests of EF and behavioral ratings of EF reflect distinct underlying mental constructs exhibiting minimal overlap, and therefore, they should not be considered equivalent (Toplak et al., Reference Toplak, West and Stanovich2013; Toplak, Bucciarelli, Jain, & Tannock, Reference Toplak, Bucciarelli, Jain and Tannock2009). Recent studies in adults with schizophrenia spectrum disorders, as well as adolescents and adults with schizophrenia, also confirm these findings (Haugen et al., Reference Haugen, Stubberud, Ueland, Haug and Oie2021; van Aken et al., Reference van Aken, Rietveld, Wierdsma, Voskes, Pijnenborg, van Weeghel and Mulder2023). Performance-based measures of EF offer valuable insights into a child’s performance in controlled and structured settings with predetermined testing goals and outcomes and estimate processing efficacy and specific cognitive sub-domains. However, performance-based measures may lack ecological validity when it comes to assessing real-world executive functioning and explain little unique variance, whereas rating scales such as BRIEF-2 can be considered more sensitive, though possibly less specific, offer insight into an individual’s attainment of goals in their familiar environment (Toplak et al., Reference Toplak, West and Stanovich2013). In conclusion, these two different assessment tools were not robustly correlated in our analysis, emphasizing the diversity between performance-based testing and every day rating scale of executive functioning.
The present findings should be considered within the strengths and limitations of the study. The study has several strengths, including a comparably large group of participants from the general population, ensuring its representation. Due to the comprehensive data collection of the COPSAC database, we were able to adjust for multiple relevant covariates, aiding in the control and assessment of potential confounding factors. The included covariates also provided insight into potential factors contributing to elevated inflammation, such as a high pre-pregnancy BMI or preeclampsia.
Certain limitations should be acknowledged. Firstly, our reliance on a single blood measurement during pregnancy, as opposed to a repeated assessment in each trimester, is a limitation. Secondly, our study predominantly focuses on IL-6 as the primary marker of inflammation during pregnancy. Although we acknowledge IL-6 as an important marker of inflammation during pregnancy, other inflammatory markers may also show an association with EF and future research should consider exploring other biomarkers. In our secondary analyses, we observed no association between elevated maternal hs-CRP and any EF outcomes after adjustment.
Furthermore, the absence of direct cognitive measurements for mothers is another limitation, as EF is highly heritable. Instead, EF-PRS was a proxy measure for maternal executive functioning. However, EF-PRS does not accurately represent the actual heritability of EF, making it a sub-optimal measurement due to the low explained variance. Therefore, relying on EF-PRS as a measure could be considered inadequate for drawing firm conclusions regarding the potential genetic contribution. Finally, the inability to calculate the ICC for maternal inflammatory markers is a limitation, as it prevents assessing measurement reliability.
Maternal inflammation can have various origins, including infections, conditions such as preeclampsia, chronic low-grade inflammation linked to obesity, and anticipated higher levels of inflammatory markers during healthy pregnancies (Fu, Tang, Hu, Xiang, & Hu, Reference Fu, Tang, Hu, Xiang and Hu2020; Gustafsson et al., Reference Gustafsson, Sullivan, Battison, Holton, Graham, Karalunas, Fair, Loftis and Nigg2020; Redman & Sargent, Reference Redman and Sargent2009). In our analysis of covariates associated with maternal IL-6 (and hs-CRP), we observed a significant association between maternal pre-pregnancy BMI and IL-6 levels, emphasizing that low-grade inflammation, rather than infections, stands as a potential risk factor for less efficient executive functioning.
However, other mechanisms underlying maternal inflammation during pregnancy and associations with fetal neurodevelopment may be at play. Maternal-fetal IL-6 transfer may be speculated to result in sustained microglia activation that plays a crucial role in brain connectivity and increases the risk of later psychiatric disorders (Kern et al., Reference Kern, Geier, King, Sykes, Mehta and Geier2015; Zaretsky, Alexander, Byrd, & Bawdon, Reference Zaretsky, Alexander, Byrd and Bawdon2004).
Conclusion
Our findings suggest that prenatal inflammation as measured by IL-6 is associated with less efficient every day, but not performance-based EF in offspring at age 10. These findings thereby encourage future studies to further interrogate the impact of early-life inflammation on cognition across diverse populations. If replicated, preventive strategies targeting inflammation in pregnancy may alleviate adverse cognitive outcomes in offspring.
Abbreviations
- BMI
-
Body Mass Index
- BRIEF-2
-
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, Second Edition
- COPSAC
-
Copenhagen Prospective Studies on Asthma in Childhood
- COPSYCH
-
COpenhagen Prospective Study on Neuro-PSYCHiatric Development
- DSM-5
-
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition
- EF
-
Executive Functioning
- EF-PCA
-
Executive Functioning Principal Component Analysis
- EF-PRS
-
Executive Functioning Polygenic Risk Score
- FSIQ
-
full-scale intelligence quotient
- HS-CRP
-
High Sensitivity C-Reactive Protein
- ICD-10
-
International Classification of Diseases, Tenth Revision
- IL-6
-
Interleukin 6
- PRS
-
Polygenic Risk Score
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291725000674.
Acknowledgments
We extend our profound acknowledgments to the late Professor Hans Bisgaard, the innovative founder of COPSAC and leader of our clinical research center. Professor Bisgaard’s work as a physician-scientist significantly advanced the field of asthma research. His legacy continues to inspire the studies within the COPSAC birth cohorts.
Special appreciation goes to Lisbeth Buus Rosholm from the Technical University of Denmark for her contributions to cytokine measurements. Special thanks to Louise Monnerup and Marie-Louise Tamsmark for their invaluable assistance during the children’s neurocognitive and psychopathological assessments.
Author contribution
Klaus Bønnelykke and Bjørn H. Ebdrup shared senior contribution
Parisa Mohammadzadeh, Cecilie K. Lemvigh, Jens Richardt Møllegaard Jepsen, and Bjørn H. Ebdrup formulated the research question. Parisa Mohammadzadeh performed the analyses and drafted the manuscript, under supervision from Bjørn H. Ebdrup, Jens Richardt Møllegaard Jepsen, Cecilie K. Lemvigh. Bjørn H. Ebdrup and Klaus Bønnelykke are the principal investigators of the COPSYCH study and made substantial and equal contributions to the study’s design, providing the foundational framework for our research.
Parisa Mohammadzadeh and Julie B. Rosenberg were the primary clinical investigators in the COPSYCH 10-year follow-up visit and substantial contributors to the data collection. Jens Richardt Møllegaard Jepsen and Nilo Vahman supervised cognitive and psychopathological assessments. Susanne Brix was responsible for the data collection of biomarkers. All co-authors critically reviewed and revised the manuscript for intellectual content, provided important intellectual input and contributed considerably to the analysis plan and interpretation of the results. All authors have read and approved the final version of the manuscript, ensuring its accuracy and integrity.
The corresponding author, with full access to the data, had final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication. It is important to note that no honorarium, grant, or any form of payment was provided to any of the authors in the production of this manuscript.
Funding source
All funding received by COPSAC is listed on www.copsac.com. The COPSYCH study was funded by The Lundbeck Foundation (Grant no R269-2017-5); The Ministry of Health (Grant no 903516); Danish Council for Strategic Research (Grant no 0603-00280B) and The Capital Region Research Foundation provided core support to the COPSAC Research Center. CP was supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) L3 Investigator Grant (1196508) and by a grant from the Lundbeck Foundation (ID: R246-2016-3237).
Competing interests
AMS is part of the Advisory Board of ALK and has received lecture fees from Thermo Fisher Scientific.
BYG has been the leader of a Lundbeck Foundation Centre of Excellence for Clinical Intervention and Neuropsychiatric Schizophrenia Research (CINS) (January 2009 to December 2021), which was partially financed by an independent grant from the Lundbeck Foundation based on an international review and partially financed by the Mental Health Services in the Capital Region of Denmark, the University of Copenhagen, and other foundations. All grants are the property of the Mental Health Services in the Capital Region of Denmark and are administered by them. She has no other conflicts to disclose.
BE is part of the Advisory Board of Eli Lilly Denmark A/S, Janssen-Cilag, Lundbeck Pharma A/S, and Takeda Pharmaceutical Company Ltd; and has received lecture fees from Bristol-Myers Squibb, Boehringer Ingelheim, Otsuka Pharma Scandinavia AB, Eli Lilly Company, and Lundbeck Pharma A/S. The remaining authors declare no conflicts of interest, whether potential, perceived, or actual, pertaining to the content of this manuscript.