Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T08:11:15.798Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Longitudinal alterations in motivational salience processing in ultra-high-risk subjects for psychosis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 October 2016

A. Schmidt*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, London, UK
M. Antoniades
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, London, UK
P. Allen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, London, UK Department of Psychology, University of Roehampton, London, UK
A. Egerton
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, London, UK
C. A. Chaddock
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, London, UK
S. Borgwardt
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, London, UK Department of Psychiatry (UPK), University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
P. Fusar-Poli
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, London, UK OASIS Clinic, SLaM NHS Foundation Trust, London, UK
J. P. Roiser
Affiliation:
Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, UK
O. Howes
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, London, UK Psychiatric Imaging, MRC Clinical Sciences Centre, Hammersmith Hospital, London, UK
P. McGuire
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, London, UK OASIS Clinic, SLaM NHS Foundation Trust, London, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: A. Schmidt, Ph.D., Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, PO63 De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK. (Email: [email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Impairments in the attribution of salience are thought to be fundamental to the development of psychotic symptoms and the onset of psychotic disorders. The aim of the present study was to explore longitudinal alterations in salience processing in ultra-high-risk subjects for psychosis.

Method

A total of 23 ultra-high-risk subjects and 13 healthy controls underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging at two time points (mean interval of 17 months) while performing the Salience Attribution Test to assess neural responses to task-relevant (adaptive salience) and task-irrelevant (aberrant salience) stimulus features.

Results

At presentation, high-risk subjects were less likely than controls to attribute salience to relevant features, and more likely to attribute salience to irrelevant stimulus features. These behavioural differences were no longer evident at follow-up. When attributing salience to relevant cue features, ultra-high-risk subjects showed less activation than controls in the ventral striatum at both baseline and follow-up. Within the high-risk sample, amelioration of abnormal beliefs over the follow-up period was correlated with an increase in right ventral striatum activation during the attribution of salience to relevant cue features.

Conclusions

These findings confirm that salience processing is perturbed in ultra-high-risk subjects for psychosis, that this is linked to alterations in ventral striatum function, and that clinical outcomes are related to longitudinal changes in ventral striatum function during salience processing.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016

Introduction

According to the aberrant salience model of psychosis (Heinz, Reference Heinz2002; Kapur, Reference Kapur2003; Howes & Murray, Reference Howes and Murray2014), psychotic symptoms develop as a result of the inappropriate assignment of salience to contextually irrelevant internal and external experiences. This model is supported by evidence that patients with schizophrenia respond faster to task-irrelevant stimulus features than healthy controls (HCs) (Pankow et al. Reference Pankow, Katthagen, Diner, Deserno, Boehme, Kathmann, Gleich, Gaebler, Walter, Heinz and Schlagenhauf2016), and that patients with prominent delusions rate irrelevant stimuli as more potentially rewarding than patients without delusions (Roiser et al. Reference Roiser, Stephan, den Ouden, Barnes, Friston and Joyce2009). This ‘aberrant’ attribution of salience is also evident in people at ultra-high risk (UHR) for psychosis, who are more likely to attribute salience to irrelevant stimulus features than HCs, with this tendency again related to the severity of abnormal beliefs (Roiser et al. Reference Roiser, Howes, Chaddock, Joyce and McGuire2013).

Experiments in animals suggest that stimuli become motivationally salient when the release of dopamine in the striatum coincides with their perception (Schultz et al. Reference Schultz, Dayan and Montague1997; Kapur, Reference Kapur2003). In healthy individuals, aberrant salience measures are positively associated with ventral striatal (VS) presynaptic dopamine levels (Boehme et al. Reference Boehme, Deserno, Gleich, Katthagen, Pankow, Behr, Buchert, Roiser, Heinz and Schlagenhauf2015). Dopamine function in the striatum is abnormally elevated in both schizophrenia (Reith et al. Reference Reith, Benkelfat, Sherwin, Yasuhara, Kuwabara, Andermann, Bachneff, Cumming, Diksic, Dyve, Etienne, Evans, Lal, Shevell, Savard, Wong, Chouinard and Gjedde1994; Laruelle et al. Reference Laruelle, Abi-Dargham, van Dyck, Gil, D'Souza, Erdos, McCance, Rosenblatt, Fingado, Zoghbi, Baldwin, Seibyl, Krystal, Charney and Innis1996; Breier et al. Reference Breier, Su, Saunders, Carson, Kolachana, de Bartolomeis, Weinberger, Weisenfeld, Malhotra, Eckelman and Pickar1997; Laruelle et al. Reference Laruelle, Abi-Dargham, Gil, Kegeles and Innis1999; Abi-Dargham et al. Reference Abi-Dargham, Rodenhiser, Printz, Zea-Ponce, Gil, Kegeles, Weiss, Cooper, Mann, Van Heertum, Gorman and Laruelle2000; Kumakura et al. Reference Kumakura, Cumming, Vernaleken, Buchholz, Siessmeier, Heinz, Kienast, Bartenstein and Gründer2007; Howes & Kapur, Reference Howes and Kapur2009; Howes et al. Reference Howes, Kambeitz, Kim, Stahl, Slifstein, Abi-Dargham and Kapur2012) and UHR subjects (Howes et al. Reference Howes, Montgomery, Asselin, Murray, Valli, Tabraham, Bramon-Bosch, Valmaggia, Johns, Broome, McGuire and Grasby2009b , Reference Howes, Bose, Turkheimer, Valli, Egerton, Stahl, Valmaggia, Allen, Murray and McGuire2011a , Reference Howes, Bose, Turkheimer, Valli, Egerton, Stahl, Valmaggia, Allen, Murray and McGuire b ; Egerton et al. Reference Egerton, Chaddock, Winton-Brown, Bloomfield, Bhattacharyya, Allen, McGuire and Howes2013; Mizrahi et al. Reference Mizrahi, Kenk, Suridjan, Boileau, George, McKenzie, Wilson, Houle and Rusjan2014) and the aberrant salience hypothesis proposes that this causes attribution of salience to irrelevant stimuli (Heinz & Schlagenhauf, Reference Heinz and Schlagenhauf2010; Winton-Brown et al. Reference Winton-Brown, Fusar-Poli, Ungless and Howes2014). In addition, it has been hypothesized that because dopaminergic neurons may show more burst firing in psychosis (Goto & Grace, Reference Goto and Grace2005; Winton-Brown et al. Reference Winton-Brown, Fusar-Poli, Ungless and Howes2014) the normal phasic dopaminergic response to relevant stimuli may become relatively diminished due to the high level of noise in the system (Heinz, Reference Heinz2002; Kapur, Reference Kapur2003; Howes et al. Reference Howes, Egerton, Allan, McGuire, Stokes and Kapur2009a ). Psychosis may thus be associated with a reduced attribution of salience to relevant stimuli as well as increased attribution of salience to irrelevant stimuli. This is consistent with data from recent studies in UHR subjects and in patients with psychosis, which report impairments in both forms of salience processing (Roiser et al. Reference Roiser, Stephan, den Ouden, Barnes, Friston and Joyce2009, Reference Roiser, Howes, Chaddock, Joyce and McGuire2013, Pankow et al. Reference Pankow, Katthagen, Diner, Deserno, Boehme, Kathmann, Gleich, Gaebler, Walter, Heinz and Schlagenhauf2016).

Data from functional neuroimaging studies suggest that UHR subjects and patients with psychosis show altered activation in the VS during tasks that engage motivational salience processing. A recent meta-analysis suggested that reduced VS responses occur in patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders relative to controls during the processing of contextually relevant information and that left VS hypoactivation was more severe in patients with high scores of negative symptoms (Radua et al. Reference Radua, Schmidt, Borgwardt, Heinz, Schlagenhauf, McGuire and Fusar-Poli2015). The relationship between VS activation during reward prediction and positive symptoms requires further investigation because only six studies were available (Simon et al. Reference Simon, Biller, Walther, Roesch-Ely, Stippich, Weisbrod and Kaiser2010; Esslinger et al. Reference Esslinger, Englisch, Inta, Rausch, Schirmbeck, Mier, Kirsch, Meyer-Lindenberg and Zink2012; Nielsen et al. Reference Nielsen, Rostrup, Wulff, Bak, Lublin, Kapur and Glenthøj2012a ; Roiser et al. Reference Roiser, Howes, Chaddock, Joyce and McGuire2013; Wotruba et al. Reference Wotruba, Heekeren, Michels, Buechler, Simon, Theodoridou, Kollias, Rössler and Kaiser2014; de Leeuw et al. Reference de Leeuw, Kahn and Vink2015) and there was residual heterogeneity among them (Radua et al. Reference Radua, Schmidt, Borgwardt, Heinz, Schlagenhauf, McGuire and Fusar-Poli2015). Interestingly, individual treatment with antipsychotics was associated with a normalization of VS activation during reward prediction, and this improvement was associated with the improvement of positive symptoms (Nielsen et al. Reference Nielsen, Rostrup, Wulff, Bak, Broberg, Lublin, Kapur and Glenthoj2012b ). With respect to contextually irrelevant information, it has been shown that striatal activation during incorrect distracter trials was positively correlated with aberrant salience symptoms in schizophrenia patients (Ceaser & Barch, Reference Ceaser and Barch2015). In UHR subjects, the VS response to irrelevant stimulus features was found to be associated with the severity of abnormal beliefs (Roiser et al. Reference Roiser, Howes, Chaddock, Joyce and McGuire2013). However, it is not known if altered VS activation during salience processing normalizes in UHR individuals whose psychotic symptoms have remitted.

The Salience Attribution Test (SAT) is a paradigm that can be used to assess task-relevant and task-irrelevant motivational salience responses, termed adaptive and aberrant salience, respectively (Roiser et al. Reference Roiser, Stephan, den Ouden, Barnes, Friston and Joyce2009, Reference Roiser, Stephan, den Ouden, Friston and Joyce2010). Our objective was to assess the relationship between changes in clinical features in a UHR cohort and longitudinal changes in VS activation elicited during the SAT paradigm. Our first hypothesis was that at clinical presentation, UHR subjects would show increased aberrant but reduced adaptive salience processing compared with HCs, and that these differences would be associated with concomitant alterations in VS activation. Our second hypothesis was that clinical improvements the UHR subjects subsequent to presentation would be associated with a longitudinal normalization of behavioural and neural responses during salience processing.

Method

Participants

A total of 29 individuals who met the Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental States (CAARMS) (Yung et al. Reference Yung, Yuen, McGorry, Phillips, Kelly, Dell'Olio, Francey, Cosgrave, Killackey, Stanford, Godfrey and Buckby2005) criteria for the UHR state were recruited from Outreach and Support in South London (OASIS; Fusar-Poli et al. Reference Fusar-Poli, Byrne, Badger, Valmaggia and McGuire2013b ), a clinical service for people at high risk for psychosis. According to international standard UHR criteria (for a comprehensive review, see Fusar-Poli et al. Reference Fusar-Poli, Borgwardt, Bechdolf, Addington, Riecher-Rossler, Schultze-Lutter, Keshavan, Wood, Ruhrmann, Seidman, Valmaggia, Cannon, Velthorst, De Haan, Cornblatt, Bonoldi, Birchwood, McGlashan, Carpenter, McGorry, Klosterkotter, McGuire and Yung2013a ), inclusion required the presence of one or more of the following: (i) presence of attenuated psychosis symptoms (APS); (ii) genetic risk and deterioration syndrome (GRD); or (iii) brief limited and intermittent psychotic symptoms (BLIPS). In all, 24 individuals were included based on APS, three based on BLIPS and two based on APS + GRD. Following presentation, all subjects were provided with clinical care from OASIS (Fusar-Poli et al. Reference Fusar-Poli, Byrne, Badger, Valmaggia and McGuire2013b ). Three subjects received antipsychotic medication and were thus excluded from the analysis. Of the subjects, 26 also received cognitive–behavioural therapy (CBT), which at the time of writing had been completed in nine subjects (26 sessions on average, range 14–65). Of the subjects, seven received low-dose antidepressants: five citalopram (3 × 20, 1 × 40 mg, and 1 × unknown dose), one mirtazapine (dose unknown) and one sertraline (100 mg). The specific treatments offered by OASIS have been detailed elsewhere (Fusar-Poli et al. Reference Fusar-Poli, Frascarelli, Valmaggia, Byrne, Stahl, Rocchetti, Codjoe, Weinberg, Tognin, Xenaki and McGuire2015). All subjects were managed in the community, attending regular out-patient appointments.

A total of 15 HCs from the same geographical area were recruited via local advertisements. Absence of psychiatric illness history was confirmed with the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Inventory (Sheehan et al. Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs, Weiller, Hergueta, Baker and Dunbar1998). None of the HC subjects had a history of neurological illness, or Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV) drug or alcohol dependence (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). All subjects provided informed written consent to participate and the study was approved by the local National Health Service Research Ethics Committee.

The SAT

The SAT has been previously described in detail elsewhere (Roiser et al. Reference Roiser, Stephan, den Ouden, Barnes, Friston and Joyce2009, Reference Roiser, Stephan, den Ouden, Friston and Joyce2010). In brief, the SAT is a speeded-response game, rewarded with money, which measures responses to cue features, which can be either task-relevant or task-irrelevant. On each trial of the task, participants were required to respond to a briefly presented square. Before the square appeared, a cue was shown indicating the likelihood of obtaining a reward for the forthcoming response. Participants received a monetary reward on 50% of trials, with more money awarded for faster responses. The cues varied in two different visual dimensions; colour (red or blue) and shape (animals or household objects). One of these cue dimensions was task-relevant and the other task-irrelevant. One task-relevant feature was highly associated with receiving a reward, with 87.5% of these trial types rewarded (e.g. blue stimuli). The other task-relevant dimension (e.g. red stimuli) was not rewarded on any trials. For the task-irrelevant dimension, an equal proportion of both features (e.g. animal and household stimuli) was rewarded. Participants were not informed about these contingencies, which remained the same over the two blocks of 64 trials within a testing session, and instead had to learn them over successive trials of the task. To avoid practice effects between baseline and follow-up, four different versions of the task were used, counterbalanced across participants, each with a different stimulus feature (blue, red, animal or household) rewarded with high probability.

Participants performed the task on two occasions, while being scanned using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The baseline assessment was performed at the time of clinical presentation. The follow-up assessment was carried out approximately 17 months later. On each visit they performed the same version of the task twice. The SAT provides behavioural measures of adaptive (relevant) and aberrant (irrelevant) motivational salience on the basis of reaction times (RTs: implicit salience) and visual analogue scale (VAS) ratings from 0 to 100% (explicit salience). Implicit adaptive salience is defined as the speeding of responses on high- relative to low-probability reward trials. Explicit adaptive salience is defined as the increase in VAS ratings on high- relative to low-probability reward trials. Implicit aberrant salience and explicit aberrant salience are defined as the absolute difference in RT and VAS ratings, respectively, between the two levels of the task-irrelevant stimulus dimension (Roiser et al. Reference Roiser, Stephan, den Ouden, Barnes, Friston and Joyce2009).

Behavioural analysis

Behavioural scores on the SAT were analysed using a repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with time as within-subject and group as between-subject factors and years of education as a covariate. To test for group differences at baseline and follow-up separately, univariate ANOVA with education as covariate was used. Using box-and-whisker plots on each SAT measure for both groups separately, two HCs and two UHR subjects were excluded as outliers.

fMRI data acquisition and analysis

Scanning was performed on a whole-body 3 T MRI General Electric (USA) system. During each of the four scanning runs (two per day), we acquired T2*-weighted echo-planar images (EPIs) with the following parameters: 50 axial slices (sequential and top-down acquisition) of 2.4 mm thickness, 2.7 mm interslice gap, field of view 240 mm2 and matrix size 64 × 64. The repetition time was 2.5 s and the echo time 25 ms. A total of 237 image volumes were acquired in a single functional run.

EPIs were analysed using an event-related design with SPM12 (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Pre-processing was performed for each subject and time point separately. In brief, slice-timing correction was first performed on each volume using the middle slice as the reference. The images were then realigned to the first image in the series (following removal of dummy scans), spatially normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template and smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm full half-width maximum (FWHM). All images underwent visual inspection and participants with a high number of severely corrupted images and/or gross artefacts were excluded (two HCs and one UHR). Additionally, all images were checked for movement artefacts, and all scans with more than 5 mm deviation from the previous scan in any dimension, resulting in corrupted volumes, were excluded and replaced with the average of the neighbouring volumes (5.1% in HCs and 1.5% in UHRs). Subjects with more than 10% corrupted volumes were excluded (two UHRs). In the final sample of 23 UHR subjects, 19 subjects were included based on BLIPS, three based on BLIPS and one based on APS + GRD.

Voxel-wise maximum likelihood parameter estimates were calculated during the first-level analysis using the general linear model. Our design matrix included an autoregressive AR(1) model of serial correlations and a high-pass filter with a cut-off of 128 s. The onsets of each event were convolved with the SPM synthetic haemodynamic response function. In this model, we included four ‘cue’ regressors, representing the different cue types and an ‘outcome’ regressor representing the time points when reward feedback was provided during the task. Cues on which participants failed to respond entirely were excluded from the analysis (regressor of no interest) due to the possibility that participants were not attending during the trial. Eight contrast images were generated per participant: adaptive and aberrant reward prediction at baseline and follow-up separately; average images over both visits for adaptive and aberrant reward prediction (to test for main effect of group); and two images subtracting the contrast vector of baseline from follow-up (to test for the main effect of time and time x group interaction). Adaptive reward prediction contrasts were defined as: high-probability reward cue features minus low-probability reward cue features across the task-relevant dimension. Aberrant reward prediction contrasts were defined as subjective ‘high-probability’ reward cue features minus subjective ‘low-probability’ reward cue features (based on the subject's VAS ratings for that run) across the task-irrelevant dimension (Roiser et al. Reference Roiser, Stephan, den Ouden, Friston and Joyce2010).

Two-sample tests were conducted at the second level to test for group effects at baseline and follow-up separately, as well as to test for main effects of group and time and for time x group interactions. Significance was assessed at a cluster-level threshold of p < 0.05 family-wise error (FWE) corrected across the whole brain, using an uncorrected cluster-forming threshold of p < 0.001 (Petersson et al. Reference Petersson, Nichols, Poline and Holmes1999; Woo et al. Reference Woo, Krishnan and Wager2014) with an extent threshold of 20 voxels. We also focused our analysis on the VS as this was part of our primary hypothesis, using a voxel-level approach. The VS region of interest was defined using coordinates taken from a previous SAT fMRI study in an independent UHR cohort (Roiser et al. Reference Roiser, Howes, Chaddock, Joyce and McGuire2013): right (x = 12; y = 12; z = −3) and left (x = −12; y = 9; z = −3). Small volume correction was applied for this analysis using 15 mm spheres around these coordinates (Roiser et al. Reference Roiser, Howes, Chaddock, Joyce and McGuire2013) and a voxel-level threshold of p < 0.05 FWE corrected was considered significant. As groups differed in years of education, this variable was added as a covariate in the second-level model.

Relationships between brain activation, behaviour and symptoms

Relationships between neural responses and behavioural and clinical features were identified by including outcome measures from the SAT, CAARMS and Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) as covariates in second-level models. The same procedure for correction for multiple comparisons as described above was employed. Relationships between behavioural salience responses and symptomatology in UHR subjects were tested with Pearson's correlation coefficients using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS 16, SPSS Inc., USA).

Results

Demographical and clinical features

The two groups did not differ in age, gender, handedness, intelligence quotient or cigarette, alcohol, cannabis and cocaine consumption, but HCs had more years of education (therefore, all group comparisons were covaried for years of education). At baseline, UHR subjects had higher scores on CAARMS positive and negative symptoms and lower scores on the GAF. Over time, the UHR group showed significant improvements in CAARMS positive and negative symptoms, but not in GAF scores (Table 1).

Table 1. Demographical and clinical characteristics of the study sample

s.d., Standard deviation; NART FSIQ, National Adult Reading Test full-scale intelligence quotient; GAF, Global Assessment of Functioning; CAARMS, Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental States.

a Two-sample t tests and χ2 tests between groups, respectively.

b Paired tests and χ2 tests within ultra-high-risk subjects between baseline and follow-up assessment.

c CAARMS positive symptoms were the sum of severity scores for unusual thought content (abnormal belief), non-bizarre ideas, perceptual abnormalities and disorganized speed; negative symptoms were the sum of severity scores for alogia, avolition/apathy and anhedonia.

Behavioural data

Aberrant attribution of salience

Across both visits, UHR subjects showed significantly higher implicit aberrant salience than HC subjects (F 1,34 = 6.718, p = 0.014), and there was a trend for a group × time interaction (F 1,34 = 3.225, p = 0.081). There was also a trend for a group × time interaction for explicit aberrant salience (F 1,34 = 3.325, p = 0.077). Based on our a priori hypotheses we constructed linear contrasts at each time point to test for the predicted group differences in aberrant salience.

At baseline, UHR subjects were more likely than HCs to attribute salience to irrelevant cue features (explicit aberrant salience) (F 1,34 = 4.732, p = 0.037), but did not exhibit greater implicit aberrant salience than HCs (F 1,34 = 0.964, p = 0.333). At follow-up the group difference in explicit aberrant salience was no longer significant (F 1,34 = 0.061, p = 0.806), but HCs had significantly lower implicit aberrant scores than the UHR group (F 1,34 = 12.296, p = 0.001) due to a reduction in this measure over time (Figs. 1a and b ).

Fig. 1. (a) Implicit (reaction times, ms) and (b) explicit (visual analogue scale) scores for aberrant motivational salience processing in healthy controls and subjects at ultra-high risk (UHR) for psychosis. Values are means, with standard errors represented by vertical bars.

Within the UHR group we detected no significant correlations between aberrant salience responses and psychotic symptoms (baseline, follow-up, change over time).

Adaptive attribution of salience

Across both visits, the UHR group had lower implicit adaptive salience scores than HCs (F 1,34 = 11.472, p = 0.002), as well as lower explicit adaptive salience scores (F 1,34 = 5.493, p = 0.035). There was also a significant group x time interaction for explicit adaptive salience (F 1,34 = 4.157, p = 0.049).

At baseline, UHR subjects had significantly lower implicit adaptive salience than HCs (F 1,34 = 13.866, p = 0.001) and also exhibited significantly lower explicit adaptive salience (F 1,34 = 9.043, p = 0.005). Both of these group differences were no longer significant at follow-up (implicit adaptive salience: F 1,34 = 3.733, p = 0.062; explicit adaptive salience: F 1,34 = 1.360, p = 0.252), due to improved scores in the UHR group together with relatively stable performance in HCs (Figs. 2a and b ).

Fig. 2. (a) Implicit (reaction times, ms) and (b) explicit (visual analogue scale) scores for adaptive motivational salience processing in healthy controls and subjects at ultra-high risk (UHR) for psychosis. Values are means, with standard errors represented by vertical bars.

Within the UHR group, explicit adaptive salience scores at follow-up were negatively correlated with the severity of abnormal beliefs (r = −0.674, p < 0.001) (online Supplementary Fig. S1A) and of positive symptoms (r = −0.653, p < 0.001) (online Supplementary Fig. S1B), and positively correlated with the level of global functioning (r = 0.497, p = 0.014) (online Supplementary Fig. S1C).

All behavioural results remained after excluding the UHR subject with a later transition to psychosis (online Supplementary information 2A).

Activation during salience processing

Aberrant reward prediction

There were no significant effects of group or time, and no group x time interactions. There were also no significant group differences in responses to irrelevant cues at either baseline or follow-up.

Adaptive reward prediction

Across both time points, UHR subjects showed less activation than HCs in the VS, calcarine sulcus and midbrain bilaterally and in the left cuneus and middle temporal gyrus (main effect of group: Fig. 3a , online Supplementary Table S1). Across both groups, activation during adaptive reward prediction was greater at follow-up than at baseline in the bilateral VS and right thalamus (main effect of time: Fig. 3b , online Supplementary Table S2). No significant group × time interactions were found for adaptive reward prediction.

Fig. 3. (a) Greater activation during adaptive reward prediction in healthy controls (HC) compared with ultra-high-risk (UHR) subjects across both visits. (b) Greater activation at follow-up relative to baseline during adaptive reward prediction across both groups. Images are displayed at a cluster-forming threshold of p < 0.001 uncorrected, with an extent threshold of 20 voxels. Colour bars indicate t values.

At baseline, the UHR group showed significantly less activation than HCs in the VS bilaterally and the left parahippocampal and middle temporal gyrus, and cerebellum during adaptive reward prediction (online Supplementary Table S3). At follow-up, the UHR group continued to show significantly less activation in the VS bilaterally (online Supplementary Table S4). All results remained after excluding the UHR subject with a later transition to psychosis (online Supplementary information S2B).

There were no significant relationships between neural responses from the aberrant and adaptive reward prediction contrast and behavioural scores on the SAT (baseline, follow-up, change over time).

Relationship between longitudinal changes in clinical features and brain activation

Aberrant salience

There were no significant relationships between changes in clinical features and longitudinal changes in brain activation during aberrant reward prediction.

Adaptive salience

In the UHR group, there was a trend (t 22 = 1.775, p = 0.09) for the mean severity of abnormal beliefs to improve between presentation and follow-up (Fig. 4a ). The degree of improvement in abnormal beliefs over time was associated with the longitudinal increase in activation during adaptive reward prediction in the right VS and in the supplementary motor cortex bilaterally (Figs. 4b and c , online Supplementary Table S5). This relationship remained after excluding the UHR subject with a later transition to psychosis (online Supplementary information S2B).

Fig. 4. (a) Unusual thought content (abnormal beliefs) at baseline (mean: 3.04) and follow-up (mean: 2.09) in ultra-high-risk (UHR) subjects (t 22 = 1.775, p = 0.09). (b) Negative correlation between changes in brain activation during adaptive reward prediction and changes in abnormal beliefs from baseline to follow-up in UHR subjects. The image is displayed at a cluster-forming threshold of p < 0.001 uncorrected, with an extent threshold of 20 voxels. The colour bar indicates t values. (c) Scatterplot of negative relationship between change in right ventral striatum (VS) activation during adaptive salience processing, taken from the peak voxel in (b) and change in abnormal beliefs [Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental States (CAARMS) item unusual thought content] from baseline to follow-up in UHR subjects (r = −0.702).

There were no significant correlations between longitudinal changes in negative symptoms and in neural responses during motivational salience processing.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first longitudinal investigation of salience processing in subjects with psychotic symptoms. We explored the relationship between changes in the clinical features of people at UHR for psychosis after they had presented to clinical services and longitudinal changes in their behavioural and neural responses during aberrant and adaptive salience processing.

Aberrant salience

Consistent with the aberrant salience model (Heinz, Reference Heinz2002; Kapur, Reference Kapur2003; Howes & Kapur, Reference Howes and Kapur2009), we found that UHR subjects were more likely to attribute salience to irrelevant stimuli than HCs at clinical presentation. These data are consistent with a previous report of increased explicit aberrant salience in an independent UHR sample (Roiser et al. Reference Roiser, Howes, Chaddock, Joyce and McGuire2013). A study using the SAT in first-episode schizophrenia did not find a difference in the patient sample overall, but found that aberrant salience was related to the severity of delusions and negative symptoms within the patient group (Roiser et al. Reference Roiser, Stephan, den Ouden, Barnes, Friston and Joyce2009). However, it should be noted that another study found no significant differences between UHR subjects, first-episode patients and controls in aberrant salience attribution (Smieskova et al. Reference Smieskova, Roiser, Chaddock, Schmidt, Harrisberger, Bendfeldt, Simon, Walter, Fusar-Poli, McGuire, Lang, Riecher-Rössler and Borgwardt2015).

During the 17-month follow-up period, there was a reduction in explicit aberrant salience in UHR subjects, such that there was no longer a significant group difference relative to HCs. On the basis that abnormal salience processing is proposed to underlie the generation of psychotic symptoms (Roiser et al. Reference Roiser, Stephan, den Ouden, Barnes, Friston and Joyce2009, Reference Roiser, Howes, Chaddock, Joyce and McGuire2013), we tested whether longitudinal changes in aberrant salience processing were related to changes in clinical features in the UHR subjects during the follow-up period. Although UHR subjects showed improvements clinically, there were no significant correlations between changes in these variables and longitudinal changes in behavioural measures of aberrant salience processing. It has been proposed that the link between aberrant salience and symptoms is moderated by cognitive biases (Howes & Murray, Reference Howes and Murray2014), which may account for the lack of direct relationship between aberrant salience and symptom change in our data.

Adaptive salience

The aberrant salience model proposed that adaptive salience is intact in patients with psychosis, but may become impaired as a result of treatment with antipsychotic medication (Heinz, Reference Heinz2002; Kapur, Reference Kapur2003). The first experimental study of salience processing in first-episode psychosis using the SAT found that patients showed impaired adaptive salience, and this was attributed to be an effect of antipsychotic treatment (Roiser et al. Reference Roiser, Stephan, den Ouden, Barnes, Friston and Joyce2009). However, a subsequent study of largely medication-naive UHR subjects also found a trend for reduced implicit adaptive salience (Roiser et al. Reference Roiser, Howes, Chaddock, Joyce and McGuire2013). In the present study, which involved a larger patient sample, at presentation, UHR subjects showed significantly reduced adaptive salience responses. As all of our UHR subjects were naive to antipsychotic medication at this stage, these data not only suggest that adaptive salience is impaired in UHR subjects, but that this is not secondary to antipsychotic treatment. Consistent with this interpretation, a recent study found that adaptive salience processing was numerically (though not significantly) impaired in first-episode psychosis patients, but this impairment was if anything less marked in antipsychotic-treated than untreated patients (Smieskova et al. Reference Smieskova, Roiser, Chaddock, Schmidt, Harrisberger, Bendfeldt, Simon, Walter, Fusar-Poli, McGuire, Lang, Riecher-Rössler and Borgwardt2015).

Although significant behavioural differences in adaptive salience processing were only present at baseline, group differences in activation during adaptive salience processing were seen at both presentation and follow-up time points. At both time points, UHR subjects showed reduced activation relative to HCs in the VS. This is consistent with a recent meta-analysis demonstrating reduced VS activity in response to reward-predicting cues in schizophrenia spectrum disorders (Radua et al. Reference Radua, Schmidt, Borgwardt, Heinz, Schlagenhauf, McGuire and Fusar-Poli2015), and reports of altered VS activation in patients with psychosis during reward prediction error tasks (Murray et al. Reference Murray, Corlett, Clark, Pessiglione, Blackwell, Honey, Jones, Bullmore, Robbins and Fletcher2008; Gradin et al. Reference Gradin, Kumar, Waiter, Ahearn, Stickle, Milders, Reid, Hall and Steele2011). Furthermore, within the UHR group, improvement in abnormal beliefs over the follow-up period was correlated with the degree to which VS activation increased over time during adaptive reward prediction. This finding is in line with data from unmedicated first-episode patients demonstrating a negative correlation between the severity of delusional symptoms and reward prediction signals in the VS (Esslinger et al. Reference Esslinger, Englisch, Inta, Rausch, Schirmbeck, Mier, Kirsch, Meyer-Lindenberg and Zink2012). Taken with longitudinal positron emission tomography imaging findings that changes in dopamine synthesis capacity in the dorsal (associative) striatum are associated with change in clinical state (Howes et al. Reference Howes, Bose, Turkheimer, Valli, Egerton, Stahl, Valmaggia, Allen, Murray and McGuire2011a ), our findings suggest that alterations in both the VS and dorsal striatum are linked to symptom change. A possible mechanism could be that hyperactive inputs from the hippocampus to the VS in psychosis may have an impact on dopaminergic neurons that project to more dorsal (associative) striatal areas and thereby affect dorsal striatum-related salience processing (Haber, Reference Haber2003; Lodge & Grace, Reference Lodge and Grace2011, Reference Lodge and Grace2012; Modinos et al. Reference Modinos, Allen, Grace and McGuire2015).

The amelioration of abnormal beliefs in UHR subjects was also associated with longitudinal increases in activation in the supplementary motor cortex to reward predicting cue features. The latter finding was not predicted, as the supplementary motor cortex is not specifically implicated in motivational salience processing. However, the SAT is a complex task that also involves sustained attention, maintaining stimulus information in memory, decision-making and response selection (Roiser et al. Reference Roiser, Howes, Chaddock, Joyce and McGuire2013), and the UHR state is associated with a broad range of cognitive impairments (Fusar-Poli et al. Reference Fusar-Poli, Deste, Smieskova, Barlati, Yung, Howes, Stieglitz, Vita, McGuire and Borgwardt2012). We therefore speculate that this finding in the supplementary motor cortex may be related to alterations in one or more of these processes, possibly secondary to changes in striatal function. Furthermore, UHR subjects also showed reduced activation in the calcarine sulcus, cuneus, midbrain and middle temporal gyrus across both visits during the attribution of salience to relevant stimuli, as well as reduced activation in the parahippocampal gyrus, cerebellum, midbrain, middle temporal gyrus, middle and anterior cingulate cortex, inferior frontal gyrus and insula at baseline and/or follow-up (see online Supplementary information S1 and S2B for more details). Together with the striatum, integration of these regions is important to sustain emotion and cognition, especially during the detection and processing of salient information (Seeley et al. Reference Seeley, Menon, Schatzberg, Keller, Glover, Kenna, Reiss and Greicius2007; Menon & Uddin, Reference Menon and Uddin2010). Dysfunction of this network and abnormal network switching when dealing with a relevant task at hand has been proposed to underlie the formation of psychotic symptoms (Palaniyappan & Liddle, Reference Palaniyappan and Liddle2012; Palaniyappan et al. Reference Palaniyappan, Simmonite, White, Liddle and Liddle2013; Schmidt et al. Reference Schmidt, Palaniyappan, Smieskova, Simon, Riecher-Rössler, Lang, Fusar-Poli, McGuire and Borgwardt2016).

Some limitations of our study merit comment. The sample sizes were modest, largely because inclusion required that participants completed multi-modal neuroimaging assessments at both baseline and follow-up. The modest group sizes may thus have accounted for the absence of significant group differences in activation during aberrant salience processing. A further consideration is that at the time of writing, only one UHR subject had developed a psychotic disorder (all results remained after excluding this subject; see Supplementary information S2A and B for details), precluding any examination of the relationship between abnormal salience processing and the risk of transition to psychosis. In this regard, it is possible that the low conversion rate in our UHR sample may explain the lack of alterations in brain activation during aberrant salience processing. Future large-scale studies with a meaningful ratio between converters and non-converters are required to test if functional brain alterations during aberrant reward prediction are evident in UHR subjects who later develop psychosis or if the risk of transition to psychosis is more related to impaired activation when dealing with a relevant task at hand (i.e. adaptive reward prediction). Furthermore, in accordance with the aberrant salience model (Kapur, Reference Kapur2003), the SAT has been designed to measure abnormal motivational (reward) salience processing in psychosis and its relation to dopamine dysregulation in the VS. However, motivation is not the only form of salience (Winton-Brown et al. Reference Winton-Brown, Fusar-Poli, Ungless and Howes2014), and it would be important to test ventral and dorsal (associative) striatal activation in psychosis during other forms of salience processing that are not measured using speeded response tasks. Finally, subsequent to presentation, some of the UHR subjects received CBT or low doses of antidepressants, which may have influenced our findings. In this study, the numbers of subjects receiving different forms of treatment were too small to allow for meaningful subgroup analyses and this issue would be better addressed in longitudinal studies that were explicitly linked to a clinical trial of an intervention that might be expected to improve motivational salience processing.

In summary, this study shows that UHR subjects exhibit behavioural deficits in both adaptive and aberrant salience processing at clinical presentation, which disappeared along with the remission of APS over the follow-up period. Our results further indicate VS hypoactivation in UHR subjects during adaptive reward prediction at baseline and follow-up and that the amelioration of abnormal beliefs over the follow-up period is linked to a longitudinal increase in VS activation during adaptive reward prediction.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716002439

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant no. P2ZHP3_155184 to A.S.) and the Medical Research Council (MRC) UK (research grant no. G0700995 to P.A., O.H. and P.M.). O.H. has received investigator-initiated research funding from and/or participated in advisory/speaker meetings organized by Astra-Zeneca, Autifony, BMS, Eli Lilly, Heptares, Jansenn, Lundbeck, Lyden-Delta, Otsuka, Servier, Sunovion, Rand and Roche.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Abi-Dargham, A, Rodenhiser, J, Printz, D, Zea-Ponce, Y, Gil, R, Kegeles, LS, Weiss, R, Cooper, TB, Mann, JJ, Van Heertum, RL, Gorman, JM, Laruelle, M (2000). Increased baseline occupancy of D2 receptors by dopamine in schizophrenia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 97, 81048109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Boehme, R, Deserno, L, Gleich, T, Katthagen, T, Pankow, A, Behr, J, Buchert, R, Roiser, JP, Heinz, A, Schlagenhauf, F (2015). Aberrant salience is related to reduced reinforcement learning signals and elevated dopamine synthesis capacity in healthy adults. Journal of Neuroscience 35, 1010310111.Google Scholar
Breier, A, Su, TP, Saunders, R, Carson, RE, Kolachana, BS, de Bartolomeis, A, Weinberger, DR, Weisenfeld, N, Malhotra, AK, Eckelman, WC, Pickar, D (1997). Schizophrenia is associated with elevated amphetamine-induced synaptic dopamine concentrations: evidence from a novel positron emission tomography method. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 94, 25692574.Google Scholar
Ceaser, AE, Barch, DM (2015). Striatal activity is associated with deficits of cognitive control and aberrant salience for patients with schizophrenia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9, 687.Google ScholarPubMed
de Leeuw, M, Kahn, RS, Vink, M (2015). Fronto-striatal dysfunction during reward processing in unaffected siblings of schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Bulletin 41, 94103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egerton, A, Chaddock, CA, Winton-Brown, TT, Bloomfield, MA, Bhattacharyya, S, Allen, P, McGuire, PK, Howes, OD (2013). Presynaptic striatal dopamine dysfunction in people at ultra-high risk for psychosis: findings in a second cohort. Biological Psychiatry 74, 106112.Google Scholar
Esslinger, C, Englisch, S, Inta, D, Rausch, F, Schirmbeck, F, Mier, D, Kirsch, P, Meyer-Lindenberg, A, Zink, M (2012). Ventral striatal activation during attribution of stimulus saliency and reward anticipation is correlated in unmedicated first episode schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Research 140, 114121.Google Scholar
Fusar-Poli, P, Borgwardt, S, Bechdolf, A, Addington, J, Riecher-Rossler, A, Schultze-Lutter, F, Keshavan, M, Wood, S, Ruhrmann, S, Seidman, LJ, Valmaggia, L, Cannon, T, Velthorst, E, De Haan, L, Cornblatt, B, Bonoldi, I, Birchwood, M, McGlashan, T, Carpenter, W, McGorry, P, Klosterkotter, J, McGuire, P, Yung, A (2013 a). The psychosis high-risk state a comprehensive state-of-the-art review. JAMA Psychiatry 70, 107120.Google Scholar
Fusar-Poli, P, Byrne, M, Badger, S, Valmaggia, LR, McGuire, PK (2013 b). Outreach and Support in South London (OASIS), 2001–2011: ten years of early diagnosis and treatment for young individuals at high clinical risk for psychosis. European Psychiatry 28, 315326.Google Scholar
Fusar-Poli, P, Deste, G, Smieskova, R, Barlati, S, Yung, AR, Howes, O, Stieglitz, RD, Vita, A, McGuire, P, Borgwardt, S (2012). Cognitive functioning in prodromal psychosis: a meta-analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry 69, 562571.Google Scholar
Fusar-Poli, P, Frascarelli, M, Valmaggia, L, Byrne, M, Stahl, D, Rocchetti, M, Codjoe, L, Weinberg, L, Tognin, S, Xenaki, L, McGuire, P (2015). Antidepressant, antipsychotic and psychological interventions in subjects at high clinical risk for psychosis: OASIS 6-year naturalistic study. Psychological Medicine 45, 13271339.Google Scholar
Goto, Y, Grace, AA (2005). Dopaminergic modulation of limbic and cortical drive of nucleus accumbens in goal-directed behavior. Nature Neuroscience 8, 805812.Google Scholar
Gradin, VB, Kumar, P, Waiter, G, Ahearn, T, Stickle, C, Milders, M, Reid, I, Hall, J, Steele, JD (2011). Expected value and prediction error abnormalities in depression and schizophrenia. Brain 134, 17511764.Google Scholar
Haber, SN (2003). The primate basal ganglia: parallel and integrative networks. Journal of Chemical Neuroanatomy 26, 317330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heinz, A (2002). Dopaminergic dysfunction in alcoholism and schizophrenia – psychopathological and behavioral correlates. European Psychiatry 17, 916.Google Scholar
Heinz, A, Schlagenhauf, F (2010). Dopaminergic dysfunction in schizophrenia: salience attribution revisited. Schizophrenia Bulletin 36, 472485.Google Scholar
Howes, O, Bose, S, Turkheimer, F, Valli, I, Egerton, A, Stahl, D, Valmaggia, L, Allen, P, Murray, R, McGuire, P (2011 a). Progressive increase in striatal dopamine synthesis capacity as patients develop psychosis: a PET study. Molecular Psychiatry 16, 885886.Google Scholar
Howes, OD, Bose, SK, Turkheimer, F, Valli, I, Egerton, A, Valmaggia, LR, Murray, RM, McGuire, P (2011 b). Dopamine synthesis capacity before onset of psychosis: a prospective [18F]-DOPA PET imaging study. American Journal of Psychiatry 168, 13111317.Google Scholar
Howes, OD, Egerton, A, Allan, V, McGuire, P, Stokes, P, Kapur, S (2009 a). Mechanisms underlying psychosis and antipsychotic treatment response in schizophrenia: insights from PET and SPECT imaging. Current Pharmaceutical Design 15, 25502559.Google Scholar
Howes, OD, Kambeitz, J, Kim, E, Stahl, D, Slifstein, M, Abi-Dargham, A, Kapur, S (2012). The nature of dopamine dysfunction in schizophrenia and what this means for treatment. Archives of General Psychiatry 69, 776786.Google Scholar
Howes, OD, Kapur, S (2009). The dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia: version III – the final common pathway. Schizophrenia Bulletin 35, 549562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howes, OD, Montgomery, AJ, Asselin, MC, Murray, RM, Valli, I, Tabraham, P, Bramon-Bosch, E, Valmaggia, L, Johns, L, Broome, M, McGuire, PK, Grasby, PM (2009 b). Elevated striatal dopamine function linked to prodromal signs of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 66, 1320.Google Scholar
Howes, OD, Murray, RM (2014). Schizophrenia: an integrated sociodevelopmental–cognitive model. Lancet 383, 16771687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kapur, S (2003). Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 1323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kumakura, Y, Cumming, P, Vernaleken, I, Buchholz, HG, Siessmeier, T, Heinz, A, Kienast, T, Bartenstein, P, Gründer, G (2007). Elevated [18F]fluorodopamine turnover in brain of patients with schizophrenia: an [18F]fluoroDOPA/positron emission tomography study. Journal of Neuroscience 27, 80808087.Google Scholar
Laruelle, M, Abi-Dargham, A, Gil, R, Kegeles, L, Innis, R (1999). Increased dopamine transmission in schizophrenia: relationship to illness phases. Biological Psychiatry 46, 5672.Google Scholar
Laruelle, M, Abi-Dargham, A, van Dyck, CH, Gil, R, D'Souza, CD, Erdos, J, McCance, E, Rosenblatt, W, Fingado, C, Zoghbi, SS, Baldwin, RM, Seibyl, JP, Krystal, JH, Charney, DS, Innis, RB (1996). Single photon emission computerized tomography imaging of amphetamine-induced dopamine release in drug-free schizophrenic subjects. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 93, 92359240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lodge, DJ, Grace, AA (2011). Hippocampal dysregulation of dopamine system function and the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences 32, 507513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lodge, DJ, Grace, AA (2012). Divergent activation of ventromedial and ventrolateral dopamine systems in animal models of amphetamine sensitization and schizophrenia. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 15, 6976.Google Scholar
Menon, V, Uddin, LQ (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Structure and Function 214, 655667.Google Scholar
Mizrahi, R, Kenk, M, Suridjan, I, Boileau, I, George, TP, McKenzie, K, Wilson, AA, Houle, S, Rusjan, P (2014). Stress-induced dopamine response in subjects at clinical high risk for schizophrenia with and without concurrent cannabis use. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 14791489.Google Scholar
Modinos, G, Allen, P, Grace, AA, McGuire, P (2015). Translating the MAM model of psychosis to humans. Trends in Neurosciences 38, 129138.Google Scholar
Murray, GK, Corlett, PR, Clark, L, Pessiglione, M, Blackwell, AD, Honey, G, Jones, PB, Bullmore, ET, Robbins, TW, Fletcher, PC (2008). Substantia nigra/ventral tegmental reward prediction error disruption in psychosis. Molecular Psychiatry 13, 267276.Google Scholar
Nielsen, M, Rostrup, E, Wulff, S, Bak, N, Lublin, H, Kapur, S, Glenthøj, B (2012 a). Alterations of the brain reward system in antipsychotic naïve schizophrenia patients. Biological Psychiatry 71, 898905.Google Scholar
Nielsen, MO, Rostrup, E, Wulff, S, Bak, N, Broberg, BV, Lublin, H, Kapur, S, Glenthoj, B (2012 b). Improvement of brain reward abnormalities by antipsychotic monotherapy in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 69, 11951204.Google Scholar
Palaniyappan, L, Liddle, PF (2012). Does the salience network play a cardinal role in psychosis? An emerging hypothesis of insular dysfunction. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 37, 1727.Google Scholar
Palaniyappan, L, Simmonite, M, White, TP, Liddle, EB, Liddle, PF (2013). Neural primacy of the salience processing system in schizophrenia. Neuron 79, 814828.Google Scholar
Pankow, A, Katthagen, T, Diner, S, Deserno, L, Boehme, R, Kathmann, N, Gleich, T, Gaebler, M, Walter, H, Heinz, A, Schlagenhauf, F (2016). Aberrant salience is related to dysfunctional self-referential processing in psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 42, 6776.Google Scholar
Petersson, KM, Nichols, TE, Poline, JB, Holmes, AP (1999). Statistical limitations in functional neuroimaging. II. Signal detection and statistical inference. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 354, 12611281.Google Scholar
Radua, J, Schmidt, A, Borgwardt, S, Heinz, A, Schlagenhauf, F, McGuire, P, Fusar-Poli, P (2015). Ventral striatal activation during reward processing in psychosis: a neurofunctional meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 12431251.Google Scholar
Reith, J, Benkelfat, C, Sherwin, A, Yasuhara, Y, Kuwabara, H, Andermann, F, Bachneff, S, Cumming, P, Diksic, M, Dyve, SE, Etienne, P, Evans, AC, Lal, S, Shevell, M, Savard, G, Wong, DF, Chouinard, G, Gjedde, A (1994). Elevated DOPA decarboxylase activity in living brain of patients with psychosis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 91, 1165111654.Google Scholar
Roiser, JP, Howes, OD, Chaddock, CA, Joyce, EM, McGuire, P (2013). Neural and behavioral correlates of aberrant salience in individuals at risk for psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 39, 13281336.Google Scholar
Roiser, JP, Stephan, KE, den Ouden, HE, Barnes, TR, Friston, KJ, Joyce, EM (2009). Do patients with schizophrenia exhibit aberrant salience? Psychological Medicine 39, 199209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roiser, JP, Stephan, KE, den Ouden, HE, Friston, KJ, Joyce, EM (2010). Adaptive and aberrant reward prediction signals in the human brain. NeuroImage 50, 657664.Google Scholar
Schmidt, A, Palaniyappan, L, Smieskova, R, Simon, A, Riecher-Rössler, A, Lang, UE, Fusar-Poli, P, McGuire, P, Borgwardt, SJ (2016). Dysfunctional insular connectivity during reward prediction in patients with first-episode psychosis. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 41, 150234.Google Scholar
Schultz, W, Dayan, P, Montague, PR (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275, 15931599.Google Scholar
Seeley, WW, Menon, V, Schatzberg, AF, Keller, J, Glover, GH, Kenna, H, Reiss, AL, Greicius, MD (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. Journal of Neuroscience 27, 23492356.Google Scholar
Sheehan, D, Lecrubier, Y, Sheehan, K, Amorim, P, Janavs, J, Weiller, E, Hergueta, T, Baker, R, Dunbar, G (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 59, 2233.Google ScholarPubMed
Simon, JJ, Biller, A, Walther, S, Roesch-Ely, D, Stippich, C, Weisbrod, M, Kaiser, S (2010). Neural correlates of reward processing in schizophrenia – relationship to apathy and depression. Schizophrenia Research 118, 154161.Google Scholar
Smieskova, R, Roiser, JP, Chaddock, CA, Schmidt, A, Harrisberger, F, Bendfeldt, K, Simon, A, Walter, A, Fusar-Poli, P, McGuire, PK, Lang, UE, Riecher-Rössler, A, Borgwardt, S (2015). Modulation of motivational salience processing during the early stages of psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 166, 1723.Google Scholar
Winton-Brown, TT, Fusar-Poli, P, Ungless, MA, Howes, OD (2014). Dopaminergic basis of salience dysregulation in psychosis. Trends in Neurosciences 37, 8594.Google Scholar
Woo, CW, Krishnan, A, Wager, TD (2014). Cluster-extent based thresholding in fMRI analyses: pitfalls and recommendations. NeuroImage 91, 412419.Google Scholar
Wotruba, D, Heekeren, K, Michels, L, Buechler, R, Simon, JJ, Theodoridou, A, Kollias, S, Rössler, W, Kaiser, S (2014). Symptom dimensions are associated with reward processing in unmedicated persons at risk for psychosis. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 8, 382.Google Scholar
Yung, AR, Yuen, HP, McGorry, PD, Phillips, LJ, Kelly, D, Dell'Olio, M, Francey, SM, Cosgrave, EM, Killackey, E, Stanford, C, Godfrey, K, Buckby, J (2005). Mapping the onset of psychosis: the Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental States. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 39, 964971.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographical and clinical characteristics of the study sample

Figure 1

Fig. 1. (a) Implicit (reaction times, ms) and (b) explicit (visual analogue scale) scores for aberrant motivational salience processing in healthy controls and subjects at ultra-high risk (UHR) for psychosis. Values are means, with standard errors represented by vertical bars.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. (a) Implicit (reaction times, ms) and (b) explicit (visual analogue scale) scores for adaptive motivational salience processing in healthy controls and subjects at ultra-high risk (UHR) for psychosis. Values are means, with standard errors represented by vertical bars.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. (a) Greater activation during adaptive reward prediction in healthy controls (HC) compared with ultra-high-risk (UHR) subjects across both visits. (b) Greater activation at follow-up relative to baseline during adaptive reward prediction across both groups. Images are displayed at a cluster-forming threshold of p < 0.001 uncorrected, with an extent threshold of 20 voxels. Colour bars indicate t values.

Figure 4

Fig. 4. (a) Unusual thought content (abnormal beliefs) at baseline (mean: 3.04) and follow-up (mean: 2.09) in ultra-high-risk (UHR) subjects (t22 = 1.775, p = 0.09). (b) Negative correlation between changes in brain activation during adaptive reward prediction and changes in abnormal beliefs from baseline to follow-up in UHR subjects. The image is displayed at a cluster-forming threshold of p < 0.001 uncorrected, with an extent threshold of 20 voxels. The colour bar indicates t values. (c) Scatterplot of negative relationship between change in right ventral striatum (VS) activation during adaptive salience processing, taken from the peak voxel in (b) and change in abnormal beliefs [Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental States (CAARMS) item unusual thought content] from baseline to follow-up in UHR subjects (r = −0.702).

Supplementary material: File

Schmidt supplementary material

Schmidt supplementary material

Download Schmidt supplementary material(File)
File 975.9 KB