Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-vmclg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-22T13:57:01.300Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Loneliness and cognition in older adults: A meta-analysis of harmonized studies from the United States, England, India, China, South Africa, Mexico, and Chile

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 February 2025

Ji Hyun Lee
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Community Health, Montana State University, Bozeman, MT, USA
Angelina R. Sutin
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioral Sciences and Social Medicine, College of Medicine, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA
André Hajek
Affiliation:
Department of Health Economics and Health Services Research, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany
Selin Karakose
Affiliation:
Department of Geriatrics, College of Medicine, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA
Damaris Aschwanden
Affiliation:
Department of Geriatrics, College of Medicine, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA Center for the Interdisciplinary Study of Gerontology and Vulnerability, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
Páraic S. O’Súilleabháin
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Limerick, Limerick, Ireland
Yannick Stephan
Affiliation:
EuroMov, University of Montpellier, Montpellier, France
Antonio Terracciano
Affiliation:
Department of Geriatrics, College of Medicine, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA
Martina Luchetti*
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioral Sciences and Social Medicine, College of Medicine, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA
*
Corresponding author: Martina Luchetti; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Loneliness is a risk factor for late-life dementia. There is less consistent evidence of its association with cognitive performance. This study examined the replicability of the association between loneliness and overall and domain-specific cognitive function and informant-rated cognitive decline in cohorts from seven countries: the United States, England, India, China, South Africa, Mexico, and Chile.

Methods

Data were from the Harmonized Cognitive Assessment Protocol administered in seven population-based studies (total N > 20,000). Participants reported their loneliness, completed a battery of cognitive tests, and nominated a knowledgeable informant to rate their cognitive decline. Random-effect meta-analyses were used to summarize the associations from each cohort.

Results

Loneliness was associated with poor overall cognitive performance and informant-rated cognitive decline controlling for sociodemographic factors (meta-analytic correlation for overall cognition = −.10 [95% CI = −.13, −.06] and informant-rated decline = .16 [95% CI = .14, .17]). Despite some heterogeneity, the associations were significant across samples from Africa, Asia, Europe, North, Central, and South America. The meta-analysis also indicated an association with specific cognitive domains: episodic memory, speed-attention, visuospatial abilities, numeric reasoning, and verbal fluency. The associations were attenuated but persisted when depressive symptoms were added as a covariate. Depression, cognitive impairment, and sociodemographic factors did not consistently moderate the associations across samples.

Conclusions

Loneliness is associated with poor performance across multiple domains of cognition and observer-rated cognitive decline, associations that replicated across diverse world regions and cultures.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

The World Health Organization (WHO) have called attention to the public health crisis of loneliness (WHO, 2021, 2023). About three in ten older adults feel lonely worldwide (Susanty et al., Reference Susanty, Nadirawati, Setiawan, Haroen, Pebrianti, Harun and Banda2025), with widespread, documented consequences for their health and well-being (National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine, 2020). Feeling lonely is the subjective perception that one does not have their desired social connections, regardless of whether alone or not (Hawkley & Cacioppo, Reference Hawkley and Cacioppo2010). As such, it is related but distinct from being isolated or alone. Such feelings are associated with premature mortality (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Gao, Han, Yu, Long, Jiang and Zhao2023) and increased risk for cardiovascular problems, immune dysfunctions, impaired sleep, and depressive symptoms (Cacioppo, Hawkley, & Thisted, Reference Cacioppo, Hawkley and Thisted2010; Christiansen, Larsen, & Lasgaard, Reference Christiansen, Larsen and Lasgaard2016; Steptoe, Owen, Kunz-Ebrecht, & Brydon, Reference Steptoe, Owen, Kunz-Ebrecht and Brydon2004), all factors important for cognitive health. Feeling lonely is associated with an increased risk of all-cause dementia, including Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia, and cognitive impairment (Luchetti et al., Reference Luchetti, Aschwanden, Sesker, Zhu, O’Súilleabháin, Stephan and Sutin2024). There is less consistent evidence for the relation between loneliness and cognitive function, which impacts everyday life, even before the onset of impairment.

In a recent meta-analysis, Harrington et al. (Reference Harrington, Vasan, Kang, Sliwinski and Lim2023) pooled data from six independent studies (n = 10,954; age > 60 years) and identified a significant cross-sectional association between loneliness and global cognitive function (r = − .08, 95% CI = −.14, −.02). The authors noted large heterogeneity across studies (I2 = 84) and a lack of sufficient data to examine associations with specific cognitive domains in a meta-analysis. In studies that have examined episodic memory (e.g., word recall), the association has been inconsistent. Some researchers find a negative association, both cross-sectionally and longitudinally (Desai et al., Reference Desai, Krueger, De Leon, Wilson, Evans and Rajan2023; Estrella et al., Reference Estrella, Durazo-Arvizu, Gallo, Tarraf, Isasi, Perreira and Lamar2021; Kang et al., Reference Kang, Oremus, Dubin, Tyas, Oga-Omenka and Golberg2024; Souza et al., Reference Souza, Farias-Itao, Aliberti, Bertola, De Andrade, Lima-Costa and Suemoto2023; Tao et al., Reference Tao, Akhter-Khan, Ang, DeCarli, Alosco, Mez and Qiu2022), whereas others have not found an association between loneliness and memory (Samtani et al., Reference Samtani, Mahalingam, Lam, Lipnicki, Lima-Costa, Blay and Brodaty2022; Sol, Sharifian, Manly, Brickman, & Zahodne, Reference Sol, Sharifian, Manly, Brickman and Zahodne2021; Solé-Padullés et al., Reference Solé-Padullés, Macià, Andersson, Stiernstedt, Pudas, Düzel and Bartrés-Faz2022), or reported a significant association only for some participants (e.g., adults over 65) but not others (Cachón-Alonso, Hakulinen, Jokela, Komulainen, & Elovainio, Reference Cachón-Alonso, Hakulinen, Jokela, Komulainen and Elovainio2023). Results are also mixed in studies that examined executive function or performance on speed-attention tasks: Some studies reported a negative association (Desai et al., Reference Desai, Krueger, De Leon, Wilson, Evans and Rajan2023; Estrella et al., Reference Estrella, Durazo-Arvizu, Gallo, Tarraf, Isasi, Perreira and Lamar2021; Samtani et al., Reference Samtani, Mahalingam, Lam, Lipnicki, Lima-Costa, Blay and Brodaty2022; Tao et al., Reference Tao, Akhter-Khan, Ang, DeCarli, Alosco, Mez and Qiu2022), but others not (Kyröläinen & Kuperman, Reference Kyröläinen and Kuperman2021; McVeigh et al., Reference McVeigh, Mehl, Polsinelli, Moseley, Sbarra, Glisky and Grilli2024; Windsor, Ghisletta, & Gerstorf, Reference Windsor, Ghisletta and Gerstorf2020). Similarly, mixed associations are observed with poor verbal fluency, a marker of subsequent cognitive impairment (Sutin, Stephan, & Terracciano, Reference Sutin, Stephan and Terracciano2019b), with studies reporting both significant (Cachón-Alonso et al., Reference Cachón-Alonso, Hakulinen, Jokela, Komulainen and Elovainio2023; Estrella et al., Reference Estrella, Durazo-Arvizu, Gallo, Tarraf, Isasi, Perreira and Lamar2021; Souza et al., Reference Souza, Farias-Itao, Aliberti, Bertola, De Andrade, Lima-Costa and Suemoto2023) and non-significant associations (Kyröläinen & Kuperman, Reference Kyröläinen and Kuperman2021; Windsor et al., Reference Windsor, Ghisletta and Gerstorf2020).

These findings suggest that loneliness may have a negative association with cognitive function across multiple domains. There are methodological differences—from the different tasks administered, to sample size, location, and composition, to selection of covariates—that limit comparability across studies. The present study expands the literature on loneliness and cognitive health in several ways. First, it uses data from the Harmonized Cognitive Assessment Protocol (HCAP) administered in the Health and Retirement Study (HRS) and six HRS-sister studies. The use of HCAP increases comparability across studies to examine the association between loneliness and cognitive function across five domains (Sutin, Stephan, Luchetti, & Terracciano, Reference Sutin, Stephan, Luchetti and Terracciano2019a; Weir, Langa, & Ryan, Reference Weir, Langa and Ryan2016): episodic memory; attention and processing speed; visuospatial abilities; numeric reasoning; and verbal fluency. Second, HCAP also collects information on cognitive decline reported by a knowledgeable informant. While there is evidence of an association between loneliness and self-reported cognitive decline (Reynolds et al., Reference Reynolds, Manning, Kirn, Klein, Hampton, Burke and Amariglio2022), to our knowledge, no study has examined whether this association extends to cognitive decline observed by knowledgeable others. This measure is important because close others may be able to detect cognitive deficits that occur in daily life that cannot be detected with standardized cognitive tests (Pérez-Blanco et al., Reference Pérez-Blanco, Felpete, Patten, Mallo, Pereiro, Campos-Magdaleno and Juncos-Rabadán2022). Third, with few exceptions (Foong, Ibrahim, Abdullah, & Bagat, Reference Foong, Ibrahim, Abdullah and Bagat2024; Kyaw & Levine, Reference Kyaw and Levine2023; Souza et al., Reference Souza, Farias-Itao, Aliberti, Bertola, De Andrade, Lima-Costa and Suemoto2023), most published work has tested samples from North America and Europe, which limits generalizability of the associations to other countries and cultures. With the use of seven samples with harmonized measures across the world, this study addresses the lack of data from geographic areas, particularly from Africa and South America.

Based on prior research, we expect loneliness to be associated with poor overall cognitive performance and informant-rated cognitive decline. We do not make specific hypotheses for the cognitive domains because of the mixed associations reported in the literature. In addition, we examined whether the associations were moderated by cognitive status (impaired versus unimpaired) and depressive symptomatology because both factors are relevant for loneliness (Carrasco et al., Reference Carrasco, Crespo, García, Ibáñez, Rubio and Montenegro-Peña2024; Susanty et al., Reference Susanty, Nadirawati, Setiawan, Haroen, Pebrianti, Harun and Banda2025) and impair performance on cognitive tasks (Henry, Crawford, & Phillips, Reference Henry, Crawford and Phillips2004; Semkovska et al., Reference Semkovska, Quinlivan, O’Grady, Johnson, Collins, O’Connor and Gload2019). We also examined whether the association varied by sociodemographic factors (age, sex, education, marital status, living arrangements, and race and/or ethnicity [where possible]) to examine generalizability of the associations across sociodemographic groups. We performed the analysis separately in each HCAP study and then combined the results in a meta-analysis.

Methods

Participants

Participants were from seven cohort studies that administered HCAP: the HRS, the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA), the Diagnostic Assessment of Dementia for the Longitudinal Aging Study in India (LASI-DAD), the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS), the Dementia Study of the Health and Aging in Africa (DS-HAALSI), the Mexican Cognitive Aging Ancillary Study (Mex-Cog) of the Mexican Health and Aging Study (MHAS), and the Chile Cognitive Aging Study (Chile-Cog). Data were from the wave that the HCAP protocol was administered for the first time, except for Mex-Cog, for which we used the wave in which HCAP was administered in the same year as of the regular assessment. Detailed information about measures, administration, and scoring can be found in Weir et al. (Reference Weir, Langa and Ryan2016), Sutin and colleagues (Sutin, Luchetti, Stephan, & Terracciano, Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Stephan and Terracciano2024; Sutin, Stephan, Luchetti, et al., 2019) and Supplementary Material (Table S1-S3).

Of the HRS participants, those who completed the 2016 interview and were 65 and over were eligible for HCAP. Of the eligible participants, a random subset was invited to participate in HCAP (n = 5,500). Of those, 3,496 completed at least part of the protocol between June 2016 and October 2017. Participants could have the HCAP assessment administered in either English or Spanish. Loneliness was measured in the 2014 or 2016 regular assessment; the sociodemographic covariates were from the same assessment as loneliness. The analyses included 2,829 individuals (60.1% females; age at the regular assessment M = 74.46, SD = 7.39) with data on loneliness, age, sex, education, race, ethnicity, and marital status, and at least one HCAP cognitive measure; informant ratings were available for 2,587 participants.

ELSA participants were eligible for the HCAP assessment if they were 65 or older by January 2018 and completed an in-person interview in Wave 8 (2016/2017) or Wave 7 (2014/2015). Of the eligible participants (n = 5,715), only a subset was randomly invited to complete the protocol (n = 1,778). A total of 1,273 participants completed at least part of the protocol between January 2018 and April 2018. Loneliness was from the 2016/2017 regular assessment; the covariates were from the same assessment as loneliness. The analyses included 1,064 individuals (54.0% females; age at the regular assessment M = 74.50, SD = 7.03) with data on loneliness, age, sex, education, race, and marital status, and at least one HCAP cognitive measure; informant ratings were available for 901 participants.

LASI-DAD assessed a sub-sample of 4,096 respondents from the baseline LASI sample at age 60 or older. HCAP was administered between October 2017 and March 2020 after the regular LASI interview and included a question on loneliness. The analyses included 4,021 individuals (53.9% females; age M = 69.63, SD = 7.47) with data on loneliness, age, sex, education, and marital status, and at least one HCAP cognitive measure; informant ratings were available for 3,963 participants.

CHARLS used HCAP to pilot cognitive tasks to include in their regular interview. Participants aged 60 and older (n = 11,021) and their informants were selected to complete part of the HCAP in Wave 4 (2018). The analyses included 9,588 individuals (50.3% females; age M = 68.07, SD = 6.48) with data on loneliness, age, sex, education, and marital status, and at least one HCAP cognitive measure; informant ratings were available for 8,837 participants.

DS assessed a subsample of HAALSI participants aged 50 and older. HCAP was administered to 635 respondents between September 2019 and January 2020 and included a question on loneliness; covariates were from the main HAALSI survey administered in 2018–19. The analyses included 630 individuals (61.7% females; age at the main survey, M = 69.17, SD = 11.56) with data on loneliness, age, sex, education, and marital status, and at least one HCAP cognitive measure; informant ratings were available for 625 participants.

Mex-Cog assessed cognitive function in a subsample of MHAS participants aged 58 and older in 2021. For the current analysis, information on loneliness was from the regular interview conducted in the same year as the HCAP assessment. Of the 4,066 participants selected, 3,575 completed at least part of the assessment. The analyses included 3,198 individuals (57.2% females; age M = 72.19, SD = 7.49) with data on loneliness, age, sex, education, and marital status, and at least one HCAP measure; informant ratings were available for 3,172 participants.

Chile-Cog assessed cognitive function among Chilean adults aged 60 and older as part of the Chilean Social Protection Survey. A total of 2,033 participants were tested in 2019. The analyses were based on 1,967 individuals (57.1% females; age M = 70.89, SD = 8.11) with data on loneliness, age, sex, and at least one HCAP cognitive measure (we were unable to retrieve information on other sociodemographic factors at the time of analysis); informant ratings were available for 1,712 participants.

Measures

Loneliness. HRS, ELSA, Mex-Cog, and Chile-Cog included three items from the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA) Loneliness Scale (Hughes, Waite, Hawkley, & Cacioppo, Reference Hughes, Waite, Hawkley and Cacioppo2004). Respondents were asked how often they “lack companionship,” “felt left out/ignored,” and “isolated from others.” Responses were on a 3-point scale, from often to hardly ever or never. Chile-Cog used a 4-point scale (never, rarely, sometimes, and always). Items were reverse scored in the direction of loneliness when appropriate and the average taken across items (alphas ranged from .73 in Chile-Cog to .84 in ELSA). Except for Chile-cog, the scale was administered in the main interview; the pattern of results was the same controlling for time between the loneliness assessment and HCAP administration. For CHARLS, LASI-DAD, and HAALSI, loneliness was assessed with a single item, “In the past week… I felt lonely,” from the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CES-D) scale (e.g., Karim, Weisz, Bibi, & ur Rehman, Reference Karim, Weisz, Bibi and ur Rehman2015), administered with the HCAP measures; responses were on a 4-point scale, from rarely or none (<1 day) to most of the time (5–7 days).

Cognitive performance. The cognitive tasks in HCAP were grouped into five domains (Sutin, Stephan, Luchetti, et al., 2019; Weir et al., Reference Weir, Langa and Ryan2016): (1) Episodic memory was measured in most samples with three tasks: the CERAD Word List Learning and Recall Task (immediate, delayed, recognition) and a version of the Wechsler Memory Scale – Logical Memory I, Long Story (immediate, delayed, recognition), and/or the Brave Man, Short Story (immediate, delayed). (2) Speed-attention was measured with the following tests: Letter or Symbol Cancellation, the Symbol-Digit Modalities Test, Backward Naming or Counting, and/or Trail Making A and B. (3) Visuospatial abilities were measured with Constructional Praxis (immediate and recall) and/or Raven’s matrices. (4) Numeric reasoning was measured with the HRS Number Series. (5) A verbal fluency task (animal category). In addition, global cognitive function was measured with the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein, Folstein, & Fanjiang, Reference Folstein, Folstein and Fanjiang2001). Each task was scored and standardized. For domains with multiple tasks, the mean was taken across standardized scores. Trails A and B were reversed (multiplied by −1) to match the scoring direction of other speed-attention measures. Supplementary Table S1 describes task variations across studies.

Informant-rated cognitive decline. For each participant, one person who knew them well completed informant-rated measures of cognitive decline. Studies included at least one of the following measures: the Informant Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly (IQ-CODE; Jorm, Reference Jorm1994), the Community Screening Instrument for Dementia (CSI-D; Hall et al., Reference Hall, Gao, Emsley, Ogunniyi, Morgan and Hendrie2000), the Blessed Dementia Rating Scale Part I (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Heyman, Mohs, Hughes, Van Belle, Fillenbaum and Clark1989), and the 10/66 (Prince et al., Reference Prince, Acosta, Ferri, Guerra, Huang and Jacob2011). These scales were scored and standardized, and the mean taken in the direction of greater informant-perceived decline. Each scale and variation across studies are described in Supplementary Table S2.

Covariates and moderators. Covariates were age (years), sex (1 = female, 0 = male), education (reported in years or as the highest level of education), marital status (1 = married or cohabitating with a partner, 0 = single, separated, divorced or widowed), race (1 = Black/other or non-White, 0 = White for HRS and ELSA) and ethnicity (1 = Hispanic/Latinx, 0 = non-Hispanic/Latinx for HRS). Administration could be in more than one language within a country (e.g., HRS) or with the help of a translator in others (e.g., CHARLS). For informant-rated cognitive decline, we controlled for characteristics of the informant and their relationship with the respondent: informant age, sex (1 = female, 0 = male), education, whether the informant was the spouse (1 = yes, 0 = no), and whether they lived with the respondent (1 = yes, 0 = no). Additional analysis further controlled for depressive symptoms, measured with the CES-D (excluding the loneliness item), and living alone, as a proxy for isolation (1 = living alone, 0 = with others). Depressive symptoms and severe cognitive impairment, defined as 1.5 SD below the sample mean on the MMSE, were tested as moderators, as both are associated with loneliness (Susanty et al., Reference Susanty, Nadirawati, Setiawan, Haroen, Pebrianti, Harun and Banda2025) and cognition (Henry et al., Reference Henry, Crawford and Phillips2004; Semkovska et al., Reference Semkovska, Quinlivan, O’Grady, Johnson, Collins, O’Connor and Gload2019). Covariates and moderators are described in Supplementary Table S3.

Statistical analysis

Linear regressions were used to examine the association between loneliness and the cognitive measures in each sample using SPSS (Version 29). The score of each cognitive domain was predicted by loneliness, controlling for age, sex, education, marital status, race and ethnicity (where available). All continuous predictors were standardized, and dichotomous predictors were coded 0/1 to facilitate comparison across samples. Language of administration and use of a translator were also assessed as covariates in HRS, LASI-DAD, and CHARLS, but were not included in the final models because they did not impact the results. For scores based on performance on multiple cognitive tasks, supplemental analyses examined the association between loneliness and each task to determine whether a specific task was driving domain-level associations. Follow-up analyses tested whether associations held controlling for depressive symptoms and living alone, respectively. We repeated the same strategy for the informant ratings of cognitive decline, controlling for both participant characteristics and informant and relationship characteristics.

Moderation. Additional analyses tested if associations varied by depressive symptoms or cognitive impairment, examining interactions between loneliness and these factors. Depressive symptomatology was tested as a continuous moderator, and cognitive impairment was tested as a dichotomous moderator. We also tested whether age, sex, education, marital status, living alone, and race/ethnicity (where possible) moderated the associations in each sample.

Meta-analysis. Results from the individual samples were summarized with random-effect meta-analyses using STATA (StataNow 18.5 SE). Sub-group analyses were used to test whether the association between loneliness and each cognitive domain and informant-rated decline varied by type of loneliness measure (1-item versus 3-item scale) and country (United States/England versus others). Interaction terms for the moderation analyses were also meta-analyzed to summarize their effect across samples.

All associations were reported with standardized beta coefficients, which can be interpreted as an effect size. For interpretation, we focused on p-values <.01 due to the large number of analyses.

Results

Descriptive statistics for each sample are in Table 1 and Supplementary Table S4. As shown in Figures 1 and 2, loneliness was consistently associated with poor overall performance and with informant ratings of cognitive decline in each of the seven countries. The meta-analytic effects were − .10 (95%CI = −.13, −.06; N = 23,266) for overall cognition and .16 (95% CI = .14, .17; N = 21,797) for informant-rated decline. The associations were similar in cohorts where loneliness was measured with 1-item (n = 3) versus 3-item (n = 4); there were no statistically significant differences between the United States/England versus other countries (see Supplementary Figures S1 and S2). There was significant heterogeneity in the association between loneliness and overall cognition (Q = 29.32, p < .01; I2 = 83.83), which suggested differences in the magnitude of the association across studies, which ranged from −.05 in the United States and Mexico to −.18 in Chile. No significant heterogeneity was observed for the association with informant-rated decline (Q = 9.17, p > .05; I2 = 3.57). When considering results from models that accounted for depressive symptoms, the meta-analytic effect of loneliness remained significant, but it was reduced by about 60% (Supplementary Table S5)—i.e., −.04 (95% CI = −.05, −.03) for overall cognitive performance (3 out of 7 studies with an association at p < .01) and .07 (95% CI = .03, .10) for informant ratings of decline (4 out of 7 studies with an association at p < .01). The associations held across samples when additionally controlling for living arrangements (Table S6).

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for each sample

Note: Studies are the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA), the Diagnostic Assessment of Dementia for the Longitudinal Aging Study in India (LASI-DAD), China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS), Dementia Study of the Health and Aging in Africa (DS-HAALSI), the Mexican Cognitive Aging Ancillary Study (Mex-Cog) of the Mexican Health and Aging Study, and the Chile Cognitive Aging Study (Chile-Cog). Values represent means (standard deviations) or percent (number); range. a Level corresponds to the percentage of participants with no formal/low education. b Cognitive impairment was defined as a score 1.5 SD below the sample mean on the MMSE. Cognitive scores and zero-order correlations for each measure and cognitive domain within each sample are reported in Supplementary Table S4.

Figure 1. Loneliness association with overall cognitive function.

Studies are the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA), the Diagnostic Assessment of Dementia for the Longitudinal Aging Study in India (LASI-DAD), China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS), Dementia Study of the Health and Aging in Africa (DS-HAALSI), the Mexican Cognitive Aging Ancillary Study (Mex-Cog) of the Mexican Health and Aging Study, and the Chile Cognitive Aging Study (Chile-Cog). Black boxes represent the correlation for each study; the size of each box indicates the influence of the correlation on the model. The solid gray line indicates a correlation of zero. The dotted line and the diamond indicate the meta-analytic association. The analysis accounted for age, sex, education, marital status, and race and ethnicity (where possible). Supplementary Table S5 and S6 report results when further accounting for depressive symptoms and living arrangements.

Figure 2. Loneliness association with informant ratings of cognitive decline.

Studies are the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA), the Diagnostic Assessment of Dementia for the Longitudinal Aging Study in India (LASI-DAD), China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS), Dementia Study of the Health and Aging in Africa (DS-HAALSI), the Mexican Cognitive Aging Ancillary Study (Mex-Cog) of the Mexican Health and Aging Study, and the Chile Cognitive Aging Study (Chile-Cog). Black boxes represent the correlation for each study; the size of each box indicates the influence of the correlation on the model. The solid gray line indicates a correlation of zero. The dotted line and the diamond indicate the meta-analytic association. The analysis accounted for age, sex, education, marital status, and race and ethnicity (where possible). Supplementary Table S5 and S6 report results when further accounting for depressive symptoms and living arrangements.

The meta-analysis also indicated an association between loneliness and poor performance in specific cognitive domains (Table 2): episodic memory (3/7 studies with a negative association at p < .01; meta-analytic estimate = −.07 [95% CI = −.11, −.04]), speed-attention (6/6 studies with a negative association at p < .01; meta-analytic estimate = −.11 [95% CI = −.15, −.06]), visuospatial abilities (4/6 studies with a negative association at p < .01; meta-analytic estimate = −.08 [95% CI = −.12, −.05]), numeric reasoning (2/2 studies with a negative association at p < .01; meta-analytic estimate = −.10 [95% CI = −.13, −.06]) and verbal fluency (5/7 studies with a negative association at p < .01; meta-analytic estimate = −.06 [95% CI = −.09, −.04]). Subgroup analyses indicated no statistically significant differences related to measures of loneliness (1-item versus 3-item) or between the United States/England versus other countries in the association of loneliness with the different domains (Supplementary Material, Figures S3-S6). Except for numeric reasoning (assessed only in the United States/England), there was significant heterogeneity across studies, with I2 values >50, which indicated differences in the magnitude of the association across samples for most domains. The domain-level associations were generally not driven by the specific tasks used to compute the composite scores. For episodic memory, however, loneliness was more consistently related to the CERAD Word List Learning and Recall Task than other tasks (see Supplementary Table S7). The meta-analytic effect of loneliness was reduced in size but remained significant (ps < .01) when accounting for depressive symptoms (Supplementary Table S8), except for the association with verbal fluency (p = .099). The associations with each domain were similar when further controlling for living alone (Table S9).

Table 2. Loneliness associations with five cognitive domains

Note: Studies are the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA), the Diagnostic Assessment of Dementia for the Longitudinal Aging Study in India (LASI-DAD), China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS), Dementia Study of the Health and Aging in Africa (DS-HAALSI), the Mexican Cognitive Aging Ancillary Study (Mex-Cog) of the Mexican Health and Aging Study, and the Chile Cognitive Aging Study (Chile-Cog). N is the number of participants with data for each cognitive domain. βs are standardized coefficients from regressions within each sample. We further report Fisher’s z (95% Confidence Intervals) and weights for each study included in the meta-analysis. The analysis accounted for age, sex, education, marital status, and race and ethnicity (where possible). Supplementary Table S8 and S9 reports results when further accounting for depressive symptoms and living arrangements.

Moderation. The meta-analysis of the interactions indicated that only a few associations between loneliness and HCAP measures were moderated by depressive symptoms and cognitive impairment (Supplementary Table S10). The loneliness × depression interaction was significant for speed-attention (meta-analytic estimate = .04 [95% CI = .02, .06]) and verbal fluency (meta-analytic estimate = .03 [95% CI = .02, .05]), with an association observed for participants with lower depressive symptoms in India, China, and Chile (Figure S7). For episodic memory, but not other domains, the meta-analysis indicated a significant loneliness × cognitive impairment interaction (meta-analytic estimate = .02 [95% CI = .01, .03]); the association was significant among individuals without cognitive impairment in the Chinese sample (Figure S7), but the interaction did not replicate in other samples. The meta-analysis also indicated that for most measures the observed associations did not vary by sociodemographic characteristics (Table S10 and Figure S7). The exception was a significant loneliness × education interaction predicting overall cognition (meta-analytic estimate = .04 [95% CI = .02, .05]), with a slightly stronger association for individuals with lower education in samples from India, China and Mexico. There was also a significant loneliness × sex interaction predicting informant ratings (meta-analytic estimate = .05 [95% CI = .01, .08]); the association with informant-rated decline was stronger for females in the Chilean sample, but the interaction term was non-significant in the other samples.

Discussion

This study examined the association between loneliness and multiple measures of cognitive function in seven HCAP studies and combined the results in a meta-analysis (total N > 20,000). Consistent with the literature on loneliness and risk for dementia (Luchetti et al., Reference Luchetti, Aschwanden, Sesker, Zhu, O’Súilleabháin, Stephan and Sutin2024), the meta-analysis found an association between loneliness and poor performance across cognitive tasks measuring overall cognitive function (MMSE), and specific cognitive domains (episodic memory, speed-attention, visuospatial abilities, numeric reasoning, verbal fluency). Loneliness was also associated with informant-rated cognitive decline. The observed associations were significant controlling for depressive symptoms and living alone. The findings were generally consistent when loneliness was measured with 1-item or 3-item UCLA scale, were similar in subgroups that scored in the range of cognitive impairment and did not differ across age, sex, and other sociodemographic groups. Furthermore, the harmonized approach revealed replicable associations across samples from different world regions, including the United States, England, India, China, South Africa, Mexico, and Chile.

Previous research has found a negative association between loneliness and overall cognitive function (Harrington et al., Reference Harrington, Vasan, Kang, Sliwinski and Lim2023). The association between loneliness and other cognitive domains has been inconsistent, with studies reporting negative associations (Desai et al., Reference Desai, Krueger, De Leon, Wilson, Evans and Rajan2023; Estrella et al., Reference Estrella, Durazo-Arvizu, Gallo, Tarraf, Isasi, Perreira and Lamar2021) and others no associations (Kyröläinen & Kuperman, Reference Kyröläinen and Kuperman2021; Windsor et al., Reference Windsor, Ghisletta and Gerstorf2020). The present meta-analysis found that loneliness was associated with worse performance in every domain measured, as well as overall cognitive function. For certain domains, like speed-attention, the association was significant (p < .01) and in the same direction in every sample that assessed the domain. The meta-analysis noted high heterogeneity for all domains assessed (except numeric reasoning), indicating that the effect sizes varied across samples, possibly due to sample composition, language, socioeconomic, or cultural factors. For instance, the samples from India, China, and South Africa included participants with lower education levels as compared to the samples from the United States and England. In addition to education, there might be other social and cultural factors not assessed in the current work that explains the heterogeneity. There are often concerns that patterns of associations found in the United States or other wealthy countries might not generalize to samples from countries with fewer economic resources (e.g., Smith et al., Reference Smith, Bloska, Jacob, Barnett, Butler, Trott and Koyanagi2021). In such context, it was surprising to observe that loneliness had the weakest association with MMSE in the sample from the United States, but the effect size was stronger than average in the sample from England. Even with potential socio-cultural differences in loneliness (Akhter-Khan et al., Reference Akhter-Khan, Van Es, Prina, Lawrence, Piri, Rokach and Mayston2024), its association with cognition function is broadly generalizable. These findings underscore the universality of loneliness and its harmful association with cognitive health.

Importantly, the current study links loneliness to cognitive function observed by a knowledgeable informant. That is, participants who reported higher levels of loneliness not only performed worse on the cognitive tasks, but also functioned worse in daily life, as observed by their informant. Informant ratings of cognitive function are critical to the diagnosis of dementia (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Such ratings provide information about cognitive changes in everyday life that may not be detected on standardized cognitive tests and that might be noticeable only by close others (Pérez-Blanco et al., Reference Pérez-Blanco, Felpete, Patten, Mallo, Pereiro, Campos-Magdaleno and Juncos-Rabadán2022).

Loneliness may affect cognition in multiple ways (Cacioppo & Hawkley, Reference Cacioppo and Hawkley2009). For instance, feeling lonely may reduce engagement in social activities, with a consequent reduction of cognitive stimulation, leaving individuals who experience loneliness more vulnerable to cognitive decline (Cacioppo & Hawkley, Reference Cacioppo and Hawkley2009; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Beam, Greenberg and Burke2020). Further, loneliness is associated with depressive symptomatology (Cacioppo et al., Reference Cacioppo, Hawkley and Thisted2010), unhealthy behaviors (e.g., physical inactivity), heightened stress, and poor sleep (Christiansen et al., Reference Christiansen, Larsen and Lasgaard2016; Hawkley, Thisted, & Cacioppo, Reference Hawkley, Thisted and Cacioppo2009), which may impair cognition over time (Dabiri, Mwendwa, & Campbell, Reference Dabiri, Mwendwa and Campbell2024; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Beam, Greenberg and Burke2020). Feeling lonely, however, is not the same as being alone and, more importantly, cannot be considered as a simple symptom of depression. Loneliness, for example, has an independent relation with cognitive impairment controlling for depressive symptoms, social isolation (e.g., participation in social activities) and health-related limitations (Luchetti et al., Reference Luchetti, Terracciano, Aschwanden, Lee, Stephan and Sutin2020). In the current analysis, the associations held controlling for depression and indices of isolation (i.e., living alone). Unfortunately, a harmonized measure of social participation/contact across the selected studies was not possible.

There was little evidence of moderation by depression or cognitive impairment. For speed-attention and verbal fluency, the association with loneliness was observed among individuals without depression, and for memory, among individuals without cognitive impairment. This result suggests that feelings of loneliness might be detrimental to cognition, particularly before the onset of clinical symptoms that characterize severe impairment. There was also limited evidence of moderation for most sociodemographic factors suggesting generalizability of the negative association between loneliness and cognitive-related outcomes across sociodemographic groups. In the samples from India, China, and Mexico, the negative association of loneliness with performance on the MMSE was stronger among individuals with lower education, who generally tend to be at greater risk for poor cognitive outcomes. Still, the observed moderations should be interpreted cautiously because interactions are difficult to replicate. For example, the meta-analysis pointed to a loneliness × sex interaction for informant-rated cognitive decline, but the interaction term was significant only in the Chilean sample and not in the other samples.

This study has several strengths, including the coordinated analysis and meta-analytic synthesis of the association between loneliness and harmonized cognitive measures across seven studies around the world. There were, however, limitations. First, this study is cross-sectional and does not consider the dynamic nature of loneliness. The frequency and intensity of loneliness may change over time (Hawkley & Kocherginsky, Reference Hawkley and Kocherginsky2018) and have differential consequences for cognitive health (Li et al., Reference Li, Wang, Guo, Zhu, Shi, Wang and Lu2022; Zhong, Chen, & Conwell, Reference Zhong, Chen and Conwell2016). Moreover, there might be bidirectional associations between loneliness and cognitive function, with loneliness predicting cognitive decline and poor cognitive performance predicting increases in loneliness (Cachón-Alonso et al., Reference Cachón-Alonso, Hakulinen, Jokela, Komulainen and Elovainio2023; Yin, Lassale, Steptoe, & Cadar, Reference Yin, Lassale, Steptoe and Cadar2019; Zhong, Chen, Tu, & Conwell, Reference Zhong, Chen, Tu and Conwell2017). Second, while the current analysis indicates loneliness is an important predictor of cognition independent of the measure used (1-item versus 3-item scale), some evidence suggests the association may vary based on loneliness scale and content of the items (Camacho et al., Reference Camacho, Pacheco, Moxley, Aranda, Reid and Wethington2024; Luchetti et al., Reference Luchetti, Aschwanden, Sesker, Zhu, O’Súilleabháin, Stephan and Sutin2024; Shibata et al., Reference Shibata, Ohara, Hosoi, Hata, Yoshida, Hirabayashi and Ninomiya2021). More work is needed to understand how loneliness, including its temporal (transient versus persistent) and emotional and social facets, relate to cognition and specific cognitive domains. Third, our analysis accounted for possible mediators of the association between loneliness and cognition, such as depression (Dabiri et al., Reference Dabiri, Mwendwa and Campbell2024). Future work could consider additional social and health-related covariates (e.g., social participation/contact; Luchetti et al., Reference Luchetti, Terracciano, Aschwanden, Lee, Stephan and Sutin2020). Fourth, this study used the MMSE to identify participants with cognitive impairment. MMSE, however, might not identify early stages of impairment (Salis, Costaggiu, & Mandas, Reference Salis, Costaggiu and Mandas2023) and future work should investigate whether cognitive status moderates the association between loneliness and cognitive domains using a clinical classification of impairment or dementia. Lastly, the current work is one of the few studies that examined loneliness and cognition in samples from different world regions. Even though the observed associations did not depend on a specific country or region, it remains important to examine loneliness and cognitive health among other low- and middle-income countries, which have greater numbers of individuals living with dementia than high-income countries (Guerchet, Prince, & Prina, Reference Guerchet, Prince and Prina2020).

In summary, the present research supports the literature that links loneliness and risk of dementia (Luchetti et al., Reference Luchetti, Aschwanden, Sesker, Zhu, O’Súilleabháin, Stephan and Sutin2024). Interventions that address loneliness might be effective in supporting cognition, particularly before the onset of clinical symptoms that characterize severe impairment (Joshi et al., Reference Joshi, Hendrie, Jester, Dasarathy, Lavretsky, Ku and Tampi2024). While interventions might need cultural adaptations, the findings of this study suggest that the association between loneliness and cognitive function is not limited to the Western cultural context; it is evident across samples from diverse world regions.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172500011X.

Acknowledgments

The present study includes a coordinated data analysis from seven public cohort studies: HRS, ELSA, LASI-DAD, CHARLS, DS-HAALSI, Mex-Cog, and Chile-Cog. Data from these studies can be accessed after registration at each study data portal. We thank all participants, national and international agencies that support each cohort analyzed in this work: HRS-HCAP was supported by the US National Institute on Aging, grant numbers U01AG009740 and U01AG058499, and curated by the University of Michigan (see: https://hrs.isr.umich.edu/data-products/hcap); ELSA-HCAP was supported by US National Institute on Aging, grant number R01AG017644, and performed at the Institute of Epidemiology and Health Care, University College London (see: https://www.elsa-project.ac.uk/hcap); LASI-DAD was curated and distributed by the University of Southern California with funding from the National Institute on Aging, grant numbers R01AG051125, RF1AG055273, U01AG065958 (see: https://lasi-dad.org/access-data); CHARLS-HCAP was supported by the US National Institute on Aging, grant numbers NIA R01AG037031 and R01AG053228, the Management Science Division of the National Natural Science Foundation of China, the Development Research Group-Human Development and Public Services Team of the World Bank, the China Medical Board and Peking University (see: https://charls.charlsdata.com/pages/Data/2018-charls-wave4/en.html); DS-HAALSI was supported by US National Institute on Aging, grant number NIA P01 AG041710, and nested within the Agincourt Health and Demographic Surveillance System, supported by the Department of Science and Innovation, the University of the Witwatersrand, and the Medical Research Council, South Africa, and previously by the Wellcome Trust, UK (see: https://haalsi.org/data); Mex-Cog was supported by the US National Institute on Aging, grant numbers R01AG051158 and R56AG059756, and the Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Geografía in Mexico (see: https://www.mhasweb.org/); Chile-Cog was conducted by researchers from the Catholic University of Chile, University of Pennsylvania, grant number NIA P30 AG012836-25S1, and the Subsecretaría de Prevision Social, the Chilean public institution that funds the SPS (see: https://web.sas.upenn.edu/harmonized-cognitive-assessment-protocol-chile/). For each study, data were collected with the informed consent of participants; all procedures and materials were approved by the institutional review boards of their respective institutions.

Funding

The analysis pertaining this publication was supported by the National Institutes of Health, National Institute on Aging, award numbers RF1AG053297 (PI Sutin) and R01AG068093 (PI Terracciano). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. Ji Hyun Lee was supported by Alzheimer’s Association (AARFD-22-970207).

Competing Interest

The authors declare no competing interest.

Footnotes

*

Ji Hyun Lee and Angelina R. Sutin contributed equally to the manuscript.

References

Akhter-Khan, S. C., Van Es, W., Prina, M., Lawrence, V., Piri, I., Rokach, A., … Mayston, R. (2024). Experiences of loneliness in lower- and middle-income countries: A systematic review of qualitative studies. Social Science & Medicine, 340, 116438. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2023.116438CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5TM (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.Google Scholar
Cachón-Alonso, L., Hakulinen, C., Jokela, M., Komulainen, K., & Elovainio, M. (2023). Loneliness and cognitive function in older adults: Longitudinal analysis in 15 countries. Psychology and Aging, 38(8), 778789. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000777CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, J. T., & Hawkley, L. C. (2009). Perceived social isolation and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(10), 447454. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., & Thisted, R. A. (2010). Perceived social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study. Psychology and Aging, 25(2), 453463. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017216CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camacho, D., Pacheco, K., Moxley, J., Aranda, M. P., Reid, M. C., & Wethington, E. (2024). Loneliness and global cognitive functioning in racially and ethnically diverse US midlife and older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 15, 1344044. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1344044CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carrasco, P. M., Crespo, D. P., García, A. I. R., Ibáñez, M. L., Rubio, B. M., & Montenegro-Peña, M. (2024). Predictive factors and risk and protection groups for loneliness in older adults: A population-based study. BMC Psychology, 12(1), 238. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01708-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christiansen, J., Larsen, F. B., & Lasgaard, M. (2016). Do stress, health behavior, and sleep mediate the association between loneliness and adverse health conditions among older people? Social Science & Medicine, 152, 8086. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.01.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dabiri, S., Mwendwa, D. T., & Campbell, A. (2024). Psychological and neurobiological mechanisms involved in the relationship between loneliness and cognitive decline in older adults. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 116, 1021. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2023.11.034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desai, P., Krueger, K. R., De Leon, C. M., Wilson, R. S., Evans, D. A., & Rajan, K. B. (2023). Race and apolipoprotein E-e4 allele status differences in the association between loneliness and cognitive decline. Psychosomatic Medicine, 85(3), 231237. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000001168CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Estrella, M. L., Durazo-Arvizu, R. A., Gallo, L. C., Tarraf, W., Isasi, C. R., Perreira, K. M., … Lamar, M. (2021). Psychosocial factors associated with cognitive function among middle-aged and older Hispanics/Latinos: The Hispanic Community Health Study/Study of Latinos and its Sociocultural Ancillary Study. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 79(1), 433449. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-200612CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., Fanjiang, G. (2001). Mini-mental state examination: Clinical guide and user’s guide. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.Google Scholar
Foong, H. F., Ibrahim, R., Abdullah, S. F. Z., & Bagat, M. F. (2024). Longitudinal relationship between loneliness and cognitive function among unmarried older adults in Malaysia: Exploring gender disparities. Psychogeriatrics, 24(6), 13131323. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyg.13196CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guerchet, M., Prince, M., Prina, M. (2020). Numbers of people with dementia worldwide: An update to the estimates in the World Alzheimer Report 2015. https://www.alzint.org/resource/numbers-of-people-with-dementia-worldwide/Google Scholar
Hall, K. S., Gao, S., Emsley, C. L., Ogunniyi, A. O., Morgan, O., & Hendrie, H. C. (2000). Community screening interview for dementia (CSI-D?); performance in five disparate study sites. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 15(6), 521531. https://doi.org/10.1002/1099-1166(200006)15:6<521::AID-GPS182>3.0.CO;2-F3.0.CO;2-F>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrington, K. D., Vasan, S., Kang, J. E., Sliwinski, M. J., & Lim, M. H. (2023). Loneliness and cognitive function in older adults without dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 91(4), 12431259. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-220832CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness matters: A theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 40(2), 218227. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-010-9210-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkley, L. C., & Kocherginsky, M. (2018). transitions in loneliness among older adults: A 5-year follow-up in the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project. Research on Aging, 40(4), 365387. https://doi.org/10.1177/0164027517698965CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkley, L. C., Thisted, R. A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2009). Loneliness predicts reduced physical activity: Cross-sectional & longitudinal analyses. Health Psychology, 28(3), 354363. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014400CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henry, J. D., Crawford, J. R., & Phillips, L. H. (2004). Verbal fluency performance in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia, 42(9), 12121222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2004). A short scale for measuring loneliness in large surveys: Results from two population-based studies. Research on Aging, 26(6), 655672. https://doi.org/10.1177/0164027504268574CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jorm, A. F. (1994). A short form of the informant questionnaire on cognitive decline in the elderly (IQCODE): Development and cross-validation. Psychological Medicine, 24(1), 145153. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329170002691XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joshi, P., Hendrie, K., Jester, D. J., Dasarathy, D., Lavretsky, H., Ku, B. S., … Tampi, R. R. (2024). Social connections as determinants of cognitive health and as targets for social interventions in persons with or at risk of Alzheimer’s disease and related disorders: A scoping review. International Psychogeriatrics, 36(2), 92118. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610223000923CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kang, J. W., Oremus, M., Dubin, J., Tyas, S. L., Oga-Omenka, C., & Golberg, M. (2024). Exploring the differential impacts of social isolation, loneliness, and their combination on the memory of an aging population: A 6-year longitudinal study of the CLSA. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 125, 105483. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2024.105483CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karim, J., Weisz, R., Bibi, Z., & ur Rehman, S. (2015). Validation of the eight-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) among older adults. Current Psychology, 34(4), 681692. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-014-9281-yCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, A. J., Beam, C. R., Greenberg, N. E., & Burke, S. L. (2020). Health factors as potential mediators of the longitudinal effect of loneliness on general cognitive ability. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 28(12), 12721283. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jagp.2020.07.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kyaw, K. T., & Levine, A. (2023). Association of loneliness with cognitive functions. The Journal of Prevention of Alzheimer’s Disease, 10(4), 903908. https://doi.org/10.14283/jpad.2023.60CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kyröläinen, A.-J., & Kuperman, V. (2021). The effect of loneliness on cognitive functioning among healthy individuals in mid- and late-adulthood: Evidence from the Canadian Longitudinal Study on Aging (CLSA). Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 701305. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.701305CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, Y., Wang, X., Guo, L., Zhu, L., Shi, J., Wang, W., & Lu, C. (2022). Eight-year trajectories of late-life loneliness and incident dementia: A nationally representative cohort study. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 31(7), 475486. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jagp.2022.12.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luchetti, M., Aschwanden, D., Sesker, A. A., Zhu, X., O’Súilleabháin, P. S., Stephan, Y., … & Sutin, A. R. (2024). A meta-analysis of loneliness and risk of dementia using longitudinal data from> 600,000 individuals. Nature Mental Health, 2, 13501361. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44220-024-00328-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luchetti, M., Terracciano, A., Aschwanden, D., Lee, J. H., Stephan, Y., & Sutin, A. R. (2020). Loneliness is associated with risk of cognitive impairment in the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 35, 794801 https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.5304CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McVeigh, K. S., Mehl, M. R., Polsinelli, A. J., Moseley, S. A., Sbarra, D. A., Glisky, E. L., & Grilli, M. D. (2024). Loneliness and social isolation are not associated with executive functioning in a cross-sectional study of cognitively healthy older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 31(5), 777794. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2023.2270208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, J. C., Heyman, A., Mohs, R. C., Hughes, J. P., Van Belle, G., Fillenbaum, G., … Clark, C. (1989). The consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assesment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 39(9), 11591159. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.39.9.1159Google Scholar
National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. (2020). Social isolation and loneliness in older adults: Opportunities for the health care system. Washinton (DC): National Academies Press. https://doi.org/10.17226/25663Google Scholar
Pérez-Blanco, L., Felpete, A., Patten, S. B., Mallo, S. C., Pereiro, A. X., Campos-Magdaleno, M., & Juncos-Rabadán, O. (2022). Do informant-reported subjective cognitive complaints predict progression to mild cognitive impairment and dementia better than self-reported complaints in old adults? A meta-analytical study. Ageing Research Reviews, 82, 101772. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2022.101772CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prince, M., Acosta, D., Ferri, C. P., Guerra, M., Huang, Y., Jacob, K. S10/66 Dementia Group. (2011). A brief dementia screener suitable for use by non-specialists in resource poor settings—The cross-cultural derivation and validation of the brief Community Screening Instrument for Dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 26(9), 899907. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.2622CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reynolds, G. O., Manning, L., Kirn, D., Klein, H., Hampton, O., Burke, O. Jr., … Amariglio, R. E. (2022). Subjective cognitive decline in a registry sample: Relation to psychiatric history, loneliness, and personality. The Journal of Prevention of Alzheimer’s Disease, 9(3), 435440. https://doi.org/10.14283/jpad.2022.31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salis, F., Costaggiu, D., & Mandas, A. (2023). Mini-mental state examination: Optimal cut-off levels for mild and severe cognitive impairment. Geriatrics, 8(1), 12. https://doi.org/10.3390/geriatrics8010012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samtani, S., Mahalingam, G., Lam, B. C. P., Lipnicki, D. M., Lima-Costa, M. F., Blay, S. L., … Brodaty, H. (2022). Associations between social connections and cognition: A global collaborative individual participant data meta-analysis. The Lancet Healthy Longevity, 3(11), e740e753. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2666-7568(22)00199-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Semkovska, M., Quinlivan, L., O’Grady, T., Johnson, R., Collins, A., O’Connor, J., … Gload, T. (2019). Cognitive function following a major depressive episode: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 6(10), 851861. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30291-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shibata, M., Ohara, T., Hosoi, M., Hata, J., Yoshida, D., Hirabayashi, N., … Ninomiya, T. (2021). Emotional loneliness is associated with a risk of dementia in a general Japanese older population: The Hisayama Study. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 76(9), 17561766. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbaa196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L., Bloska, J., Jacob, L., Barnett, Y., Butler, L., Trott, M., … Koyanagi, A. (2021). Is loneliness associated with mild cognitive impairment in low- and middle-income countries? International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 36(9), 13451353. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.5524CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sol, K., Sharifian, N., Manly, J. J., Brickman, A. M., & Zahodne, L. B. (2021). Associations between loneliness, reading ability and episodic memory in non-Hispanic Black and White older adults. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 36(6), 10031011. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acab001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solé-Padullés, C., Macià, D., Andersson, M., Stiernstedt, M., Pudas, S., Düzel, S., … Bartrés-Faz, D. (2022). No association between loneliness, episodic memory and hippocampal volume change in young and healthy older Adults: A longitudinal european multicenter study. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 14, 795764. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2022.795764CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Souza, J. G., Farias-Itao, D. S., Aliberti, M. J. R., Bertola, L., De Andrade, F. B., Lima-Costa, M. F., … Suemoto, C. K. (2023). Social isolation, loneliness, and cognitive performance in older adults: Evidence from the ELSI-Brazil Study. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 31(8), 610620. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jagp.2023.03.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steptoe, A., Owen, N., Kunz-Ebrecht, S. R., & Brydon, L. (2004). Loneliness and neuroendocrine, cardiovascular, and inflammatory stress responses in middle-aged men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29(5), 593611. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00086-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Susanty, S., Nadirawati, N., Setiawan, A., Haroen, H., Pebrianti, S., Harun, H., … Banda, K. J. (2025). Overview of the prevalence of loneliness and associated risk factors among older adults across six continents: A meta-analysis. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 128, 105627. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2024.105627CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutin, A. R., Luchetti, M., Stephan, Y., & Terracciano, A. (2024). Purpose in life and cognitive performance and informant ratings of cognitive decline, affect, and activities. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 30(3), 244252. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617723000516CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutin, A. R., Stephan, Y., Luchetti, M., & Terracciano, A. (2019a). Five-factor model personality traits and cognitive function in five domains in older adulthood. BMC Geriatrics, 19, 343. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-019-1362-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutin, A. R., Stephan, Y., & Terracciano, A. (2019b). Verbal fluency and risk of dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 34(6), 863867. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.5081CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tao, Q., Akhter-Khan, S. C., Ang, T. F. A., DeCarli, C., Alosco, M. L., Mez, J., … Qiu, W. Q. (2022). Different loneliness types, cognitive function, and brain structure in midlife: Findings from the Framingham Heart Study. EClinicalMedicine, 53, 101643. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eclinm.2022.101643CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, F., Gao, Y., Han, Z., Yu, Y., Long, Z., Jiang, X., … & Zhao, Y. (2023). A systematic review and meta-analysis of 90 cohort studies of social isolation, loneliness and mortality. Nature Human Behaviour, 7(8), 13071319. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-023-01617-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weir, D. R., Langa, K. M., & Ryan, L. H. (2016). 2016 Harmonized cognitive assessment Protocol (HCAP): Study protocol summary. https://hrs.isr.umich.edu/publications/biblio/9950Google Scholar
Windsor, T. D., Ghisletta, P., & Gerstorf, D. (2020). Social resources as compensatory cognitive reserve? Interactions of social resources with education in predicting late-life cognition. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 75(7), 14511461. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gby143CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization. (2021). Social isolation and loneliness among older people: Advocacy brief. https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/9789240030749Google Scholar
World Health Organization. (2023). WHO launches commission to foster social connection. https://www.who.int/news/item/15-11-2023-who-launches-commission-to-foster-social-connectionGoogle Scholar
Yin, J., Lassale, C., Steptoe, A., & Cadar, D. (2019). Exploring the bidirectional associations between loneliness and cognitive functioning over 10 years: The English longitudinal study of ageing. International Journal of Epidemiology, 48(6), 19371948. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyz085CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhong, B.-L., Chen, S.-L., & Conwell, Y. (2016). Effects of transient versus chronic loneliness on cognitive function in older adults: Findings from the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 24(5), 389398. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jagp.2015.12.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhong, B.-L., Chen, S.-L., Tu, X., & Conwell, Y. (2017). Loneliness and cognitive function in older adults: Findings from the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 72(1), https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbw037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for each sample

Figure 1

Figure 1. Loneliness association with overall cognitive function.Studies are the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA), the Diagnostic Assessment of Dementia for the Longitudinal Aging Study in India (LASI-DAD), China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS), Dementia Study of the Health and Aging in Africa (DS-HAALSI), the Mexican Cognitive Aging Ancillary Study (Mex-Cog) of the Mexican Health and Aging Study, and the Chile Cognitive Aging Study (Chile-Cog). Black boxes represent the correlation for each study; the size of each box indicates the influence of the correlation on the model. The solid gray line indicates a correlation of zero. The dotted line and the diamond indicate the meta-analytic association. The analysis accounted for age, sex, education, marital status, and race and ethnicity (where possible). Supplementary Table S5 and S6 report results when further accounting for depressive symptoms and living arrangements.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Loneliness association with informant ratings of cognitive decline.Studies are the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA), the Diagnostic Assessment of Dementia for the Longitudinal Aging Study in India (LASI-DAD), China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS), Dementia Study of the Health and Aging in Africa (DS-HAALSI), the Mexican Cognitive Aging Ancillary Study (Mex-Cog) of the Mexican Health and Aging Study, and the Chile Cognitive Aging Study (Chile-Cog). Black boxes represent the correlation for each study; the size of each box indicates the influence of the correlation on the model. The solid gray line indicates a correlation of zero. The dotted line and the diamond indicate the meta-analytic association. The analysis accounted for age, sex, education, marital status, and race and ethnicity (where possible). Supplementary Table S5 and S6 report results when further accounting for depressive symptoms and living arrangements.

Figure 3

Table 2. Loneliness associations with five cognitive domains

Supplementary material: File

Lee et al. supplementary material

Lee et al. supplementary material
Download Lee et al. supplementary material(File)
File 733.3 KB