Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T17:28:34.992Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Investigating the genetic and environmental aetiologies of non-suicidal and suicidal self-harm: a twin study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 February 2021

Kai Xiang Lim*
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
Georgina Krebs
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK National and Specialist OCD and Related Disorders Clinic for Young People, South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, London, UK
Kaili Rimfeld
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
Jean-Baptiste Pingault
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London, London, UK
Frühling Vesta Rijsdijk
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Kai Xiang Lim, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Self-harm is a major health concern, not only as a signal of distress but also as a strong predictor of later suicide. Self-harm can be further refined into suicidal self-harm (SSH, i.e. suicide attempt) and non-suicidal self-harm (NSSH). Understanding the aetiologies of NSSH and SSH can help inform suicide prevention strategies. Using a twin design, we investigated the phenotypic and aetiological relationships between NSSH and SSH, and their aetiological overlap with mental health problems.

Methods

We analysed data from the Twins Early Development Study using structural equation modelling. At age 21 years, 9063 twins (62.4% female) answered questions related to self-harm. At age 16 years, 19 self- or parent-reported mental health measures were administered, including measures of internalising and externalising problems, psychotic-like experiences and substance abuse.

Results

Prevalences for NSSH and SSH were 21.9% and 10.5%, respectively. Additive genetic factors explained half of the variance in NSSH (55%) and SSH (50%), with the rest explained by non-shared environmental factors. Phenotypically, NSSH and SSH were strongly correlated (r = 0.87) with their correlation explained by genetic (57%) and non-shared environmental (43%) factors. We found no evidence that NSSH and SSH differed in their phenotypic and aetiological relationships with mental health measures.

Conclusion

Our findings suggest no aetiological difference between NSSH and SSH. NSSH and SSH should be regarded as two different ends of a continuum, rather than as two distinct categories.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Self-harm can be defined as any act of self-injury and self-poisoning carried out by an individual, regardless of intention or motivation (Hawton et al., Reference Hawton, Zahl and Weatherall2003). It is a major public health concern. Not only distressing and impairing, it is also predictive of suicide (Hawton, Zahl, & Weatherall, Reference Hawton, Zahl and Weatherall2003b), which is the second leading cause of death in young adults (World Health Organization, 2014).

Self-harm can be separated into non-suicidal self-harm (NSSH) and suicidal self-harm (SSH), i.e. a self-harm episode without suicidal intention, or suicide attempt. In the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5), self-harm was suggested to be divided along the dimension of suicidal intention as two separate conditions, i.e. non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI) and suicidal behaviour disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

There is no consensus yet whether NSSH and SSH should be viewed as two distinct entities (Butler & Malone, Reference Butler and Malone2013; Kapur, Cooper, O'Connor, & Hawton, Reference Kapur, Cooper, O'Connor and Hawton2013). At the phenotypic level, NSSH and SSH co-occur frequently (Klonsky, May, & Glenn, Reference Klonsky, May and Glenn2013; Nock, Joiner, Gordon, Lloyd-Richardson, & Prinstein, Reference Nock, Joiner, Gordon, Lloyd-Richardson and Prinstein2006). Studies have also shown that SSH is associated with higher levels of psychological and psychosocial impairment than NSSH (Bjureberg et al., Reference Bjureberg, Ohlis, Ljótsson, D'Onofrio, Hedman-Lagerlöf, Jokinen and Hellner2019; Jacobson, Muehlenkamp, Miller, & Turner, Reference Jacobson, Muehlenkamp, Miller and Turner2008). Hence, it was proposed that self-harm should be regarded as opposite ends of the same continuum of self-harm behaviours (Brausch & Gutierrez, Reference Brausch and Gutierrez2010; Linehan, Reference Linehan1986; Stanley, Winchel, Molcho, Simeon, & Stanley, Reference Stanley, Winchel, Molcho, Simeon and Stanley1992), with NSSH considered as a gateway towards more severe form of self-harm (i.e. SSH). Another theory, the interpersonal theory of suicide, proposes that NSSH increases one's acquired capability for suicide through repeated exposure to painful and/or fearful experiences, and when this is joined with perceived burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness, it will lead towards lethal SSH (Joiner, Reference Joiner2007; Van Orden et al., Reference Van Orden, Witte, Cukrowicz, Braithwaite, Selby and Joiner2010).

Nonetheless, these two facets of self-harm are also different in terms of prevalence, frequency, lethality of methods, and attitudes towards life and death (Muehlenkamp & Gutierrez, Reference Muehlenkamp and Gutierrez2007). Indeed, a recent study in an UK adolescent sample suggests that although NSSH and SSH may share some common risk factors such as mental health problems, these risk factors are more strongly associated with SSH, and there are risk factors that appear to be specific to NSSH (higher IQ and maternal education) and SSH (lower IQ and parental self-harm; Mars et al., Reference Mars, Heron, Crane, Hawton, Kidger, Lewis and Gunnell2014).

Examining their aetiological differences and similarities may offer insights into why NSSH and SSH co-occur and yet differ from each other. Twin studies may offer insights into the potential aetiological difference between NSSH and SSH. In a systematic review of twin studies, based on four large-scale epidemiological twin datasets (Cho, Guo, Iritani, & Hallfors, Reference Cho, Guo, Iritani and Hallfors2006; Fu et al., Reference Fu, Heath, Bucholz, Nelson, Glowinski, Goldberg and Eisen2002; Glowinski et al., Reference Glowinski, Bucholz, Nelson, Fu, Madden, Reich and Heath2001; Statham et al., Reference Statham, Heath, Madden, Bucholz, Bierut, Dinwiddie and Martin1998), Voracek and Loibl (Reference Voracek and Loibl2007) reported that heritability estimates for a broader phenotype of suicidal behaviour (suicide thoughts, plans and attempts) ranged from 30% to 55%. A more recent population based twin study not included in the systematic review found that heritability for thoughts of self-harm and suicide ranged from 45% to 74% (Althoff et al., Reference Althoff, Hudziak, Willemsen, Hudziak, Bartels and Boomsma2012).

However, little empirical attention has been given to the aetiological overlap between NSSH and SSH. One study, based on an Australian twin sample, investigated the shared aetiology between NSSH and suicidal ideation, and found that their heritabilities ranged from 37% to 59%, and the correlation between these two traits was substantially explained by overlapping genetic influences (62–76%; Maciejewski et al., Reference Maciejewski, Creemers, Lynskey, Madden, Heath, Statham and Verweij2014). An unpublished doctoral research was conducted among an American female twin sample, and found that both NSSH and SSH were heritable, with a genetic correlation of 0.45, and a non-shared environmental correlation of 0.09 (Durrett, Reference Durrett2006). However, this twin sample was entirely female, hence the overall heritability estimates of NSSH and SSH, and their genetic correlations in a population with both males and females are still unclear.

The interpersonal theory of suicide also posits that mental health conditions may elevate individuals' risks of developing thwarted belongingness and perceived burdensomeness, subsequently increasing the risk of suicide (Joiner, Reference Joiner2007; Van Orden et al., Reference Van Orden, Witte, Cukrowicz, Braithwaite, Selby and Joiner2010). Apart from NSSH, experiencing a mental health condition may also increase the risk of acquiring the capability for suicide, e.g. painful experiences of self-starvation for anorexia nervosa (Joiner, Reference Joiner2007; Van Orden et al., Reference Van Orden, Witte, Cukrowicz, Braithwaite, Selby and Joiner2010). Several twin studies have investigated the aetiological relationships between self-harm-related phenotypes and mental health conditions. For example, shared genetic risk factors were found to explain substantially the comorbidity of suicide attempt with anorexia nervosa and major depressive disorder (MDD) in a Swedish female twin sample, with non-shared environmental factors responsible for the rest of the comorbidity (Thornton, Welch, Munn-Chernoff, Lichtenstein, & Bulik, Reference Thornton, Welch, Munn-Chernoff, Lichtenstein and Bulik2016). A similar pattern was seen among female twins in an Australian twin sample when using a more broadly defined suicidality and eating disorder phenotypes with MDD (Wade, Fairweather-Schmidt, Zhu, & Martin, Reference Wade, Fairweather-Schmidt, Zhu and Martin2015). In the same Australian twin sample, overlapping genetic and non-shared environmental factors were found to account for the associations between cannabis involvement and suicidal ideation (Delforterie et al., Reference Delforterie, Lynskey, Huizink, Creemers, Grant, Few and Agrawal2015). In an American twin sample, a large proportion of genetic variance was suggested to be shared among suicidal ideation, MDD and conduct disorder, with lower shared environmental influences (Linker, Gillespie, Maes, Eaves, & Silberg, Reference Linker, Gillespie, Maes, Eaves and Silberg2012). In the aforementioned Australian twin sample, correlations between high-risk trauma exposure and self-injurious thoughts and behaviours were also found to be influenced by genetic factors in both men and women, and non-shared environmental factors in women only (Richmond-Rakerd et al., Reference Richmond-Rakerd, Trull, Gizer, McLaughlin, Scheiderer, Nelson and Martin2019).

Among the aforementioned studies which investigated the aetiological relationships between self-harm–related phenotypes and mental health conditions, most of them studied suicidal ideation or suicide attempt only, or a broadly defined suicidality phenotype. One exception is the high-risk trauma exposure study, which included both NSSH and SSH, but these two variables were tied together with suicidal ideation in a common pathway model, and was termed ‘self-injurious thoughts and behaviours’ (Richmond-Rakerd et al., Reference Richmond-Rakerd, Trull, Gizer, McLaughlin, Scheiderer, Nelson and Martin2019). Hence, it was impossible to know how high-risk trauma exposure was correlated with NSSH and SSH differently, and whether their genetic and environmental correlations were different. This calls for the need to investigate aetiological relationships between a range of mental health problems and both NSSH and SSH, as well as to verify whether such aetiological relationships differ for NSSH v. SSH.

To our knowledge, no previous study has investigated the aetiological difference between NSSH and SSH in a population-based twin sample. Hence, using an UK population-based sample of over 9000 twins, the current study (pre-registered: https://osf.io/dy25a) was conducted with the following aims:

  1. (1) To investigate to what extent SSH and NSSH share the same genetic and environmental aetiologies; and

  2. (2) To investigate the genetic and environmental overlap between a range of mental health problems in adolescence and both lifetime SSH and NSSH reported at age 21 years, as well as to establish whether the patterns of these relationships differ between NSSH and SSH (i.e. to test whether covariances show a differential decomposition in terms of genetic and environmental factors for NSSH v. SSH).

We hypothesised that:

  1. (1) SSH and NSSH are substantially heritable. There are genetic and environmental correlations between SSH and NSSH, but also unique genetic and environmental components contributing to each facet of self-harm.

  2. (2) There are distinct patterns of aetiological relationships between mental health problems and each of NSSH and SSH, e.g. unequal genetic correlations between SSH and mental health problems v. NSSH and mental health problems.

Methods

The preregistration for this study and the code is available on the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/dy25a

Participants

Participants were from the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS), a longitudinal twin study following a population sample of twins born between 1994 and 1996 in England and Wales, who were contacted via parents after birth and followed-up across development. At the start of data collection, sample characteristics were close to the characteristics of the UK population as assessed with census data, which remains reasonably the case despite some attrition (Rimfeld et al., Reference Rimfeld, Malanchini, Spargo, Spickernell, Selzam, McMillan and Plomin2019). The data were collected in multiple waves across development, by posting questionnaires and test booklets, by telephone, by web-based platforms and at age 21, by a smartphone app. For those preferring questionnaires and test booklets, they have been made available across all data collections. More details about the recruitment process and the sample are described in Rimfeld et al. (Reference Rimfeld, Malanchini, Spargo, Spickernell, Selzam, McMillan and Plomin2019).

Data used in this study were collected when the twins were 16 (for mental health measures) and 21 years old (for self-harm measures). We applied the standard exclusion criteria in TEDS before carrying out the analyses. Twin pairs with serious medical conditions, missing zygosity and gender information, missing first contact data and extreme perinatal conditions were excluded from the analyses.

After applying exclusion criteria, there are a total of 9063 twins (62.4% female) who answered the self-harm-related questions. The average age of the twins when the questionnaire booklets were completed is 22.3 years (s.d. = 0.91 years), ranging from 20.5 to 25.3 years.

Measures

Self-harm measures

Three questions related to self-harm were used. They were taken and adapted from the Child & Adolescent Self-harm in Europe (CASE) study (Madge et al., Reference Madge, Hewitt, Hawton, de Wilde, Corcoran, Fekete and Ystgaard2008). The first question was ‘In your lifetime, have you ever hurt yourself on purpose in any way (e.g. by taking an overdose of pills, or by cutting yourself?)’. Participants chose their responses out of five options, ranging from ‘No’, ‘Yes, once or twice’, ‘Yes, 3–5 times’, ‘Yes, 6–10 times’ to ‘Yes, more than 10 times’.

If the participants answered ‘Yes’ in the first question, they were asked to answer other follow-up questions, which included two questions related to NSSH and SSH. For NSSH, participants were asked, ‘In your lifetime, have you ever hurt yourself on purpose without intending to kill yourself?’. For SSH, participants were asked, ‘In your lifetime, on any of the occasions you have hurt yourself on purpose, have you ever seriously wanted to kill yourself?’. The options available for these two questions were the same as the first question. Of note, participants who did not endorse a history of self-harm in the first question were coded as ‘No’ in the subsequent NSSH and SSH questions.

Mental health measures

A total of 19 measures related to mental health were collected at age 16 for this study. They were either reported by the parents, or by the twin themselves. These included internalising problems (anxiety, depression, anxiety sensitivity, insomnia and eating disorder), externalising problems [attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity, and emotional lability], psychotic-like experiences (negative symptoms of schizophrenia, hallucinations, paranoia, hedonia, grandiosity and anhedonia), substance abuse (alcohol abuse, smoking and cannabis use) and others (total difficulties and autistic traits). Details of the measures are summarised in Table 1.

Table 1. Measures related to mental health collected at age 16

Note: Substance abuse questions are from a questionnaire on alcohol, tobacco and cannabis use in TEDS, which was adapted from the ‘Life of a 16 + Teenager’ questionnaire from Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents And Child (ALSPAC; Boyd et al., Reference Boyd, Golding, Macleod, Lawlor, Fraser, Henderson and Davey Smith2013). More details about each measure can be found on TEDS’ data dictionary: https://www.teds.ac.uk/datadictionary/home.htm.

Analyses

Structural equation modelling

Statistical analyses were carried out using the structural equation modelling R package OpenMx (Boker et al., Reference Boker, Neale, Maes, Wilde, Spiegel, Brick and Fox2011) in the R version 3.6.2 environment (R Core Team, 2017). The classical twin design was employed to decompose the variance of the phenotypes into additive genetic (A), shared environmental (C) and non-shared environmental (E) influences. Additive genetic influences assume the allelic effects are additive but not interactive. Shared environmental influences are environmental factors shared by the twin pairs that make them more similar to each other, whereas non-shared environmental influences include environmental factors unique to each twin and also measurement error. Decomposing the variance into genetic and environmental components is possible due to the shared segregated genes among monozygotic (MZ) twins (100%) and also dizygotic (DZ) twins (50% on average).

Analyses were carried out using the liability threshold model (Falconer, Reference Falconer1965). The ordinal variables are regarded as expressions of underlying continuous liabilities for NSSH and SSH which are normally distributed. For NSSH and SSH, four thresholds were estimated for the continuums of liability, resulting in five observed categories corresponding to the five levels on the ordinal scales. In all models, for ordinal variables, sex and age were included as covariates in the threshold equations. For continuous variables, we adjusted for age and sex by individually regressing each variable on both covariates, and used the residuals in subsequent analyses. Whenever necessary, the residuals of these continuous variables were either log-transformed or cube-rooted to normalise the distributions for compatibility with parametric methods. The 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were computed using the maximum likelihood method. Model comparison was done by means of likelihood ratio chi-squared testing.

Bivariate model

To explore the first aim of the study, we firstly fitted a bivariate constrained correlation model, with constraints applied whereby the within-twin correlations and the liability thresholds are the same across birth order and zygosity. The constrained correlation model enabled us to derive the phenotypic correlation and also cross-twin cross-trait correlation between NSSH and SSH.

To decompose the phenotypic correlation between NSSH and SSH into genetic and environmental components, we fitted a bivariate ACE model using Cholesky decomposition, which is then transformed into a correlated factors model. These correlations indicate to what extent the genetic and environmental influences on NSSH are the same as the genetic and environmental influences on SSH. To formally test whether these two variables have different aetiologies, we constrained the genetic, shared environmental and non-shared environmental influences of one variable to be the same as another, and tested the goodness of fit of this constrained bivariate ACE model against the unconstrained bivariate ACE model. A graphical representation of the constrained and non-constrained bivariate ACE models is shown in online Supplementary Fig. S1. We also tested for quantitative sex differences in the aetiologies for NSSH and SSH (see online Supplementary Method S1).

Trivariate model

For the second aim of the study, trivariate analyses were used to model NSSH, SSH and a mental health measure together. This is similar to the bivariate ACE model but extended to include one extra variable. The cross-twin cross-trait correlations among three variables were estimated to partition the phenotypic correlations into genetic and environmental correlations. In total, we fitted 24 trivariate models out of 19 mental health measures (see Table 1 for details of each variable, in which some are subscales from the same measure). For each trivariate ACE model, we tested whether the pairs of genetic correlations (i.e. r g between a measure and NSSH, and r g between the measure and SSH), shared and non-shared environmental correlations can be constrained to be the same. This tested whether NSSH and SSH share similar aetiologies with the mental health measures. A graphical representation of the constrained and non-constrained bivariate ACE models is shown in online Supplementary Fig. S2. Due to multiple testing, the p values from the comparison tests were corrected with the false discovery rate method, which controls for the expected proportion of false positives among the rejected hypotheses. We set the threshold for statistical significance at a q value <0.05 after correcting for multiple testing.

Results

Descriptive statistics

Table 2 shows the descriptive statistics for NSSH and SSH in the twin sample. The prevalences for NSSH and SSH in the full sample are 21.9% and 10.5% respectively, with higher prevalences among the female twins (p < 0.001 in both cases). The estimated phenotypic correlation between NSSH and SSH in the twin sample is 0.87, as shown in online Supplementary Table S1. Descriptive statistics for the mental health measures are presented in online Supplementary Table S2.

Table 2. Descriptive statistics for NSSH and SSH

Bivariate models

As shown in online Supplementary Table S1, in the bivariate constrained correlation models, both NSSH and SSH have similar MZ correlations and also DZ correlations. In the bivariate ACE model, both NSSH and SSH have similar genetic architecture. Both NSSH and SSH are under substantial additive genetic influences (55% for NSSH, 50% for SSH) and non-shared environmental influences (45% for NSSH, 50% for SSH), and there was no shared environmental influence, as shown in Fig. 1. The estimates for NSSH and SSH in univariate ACE models are similar to what were estimated in the bivariate ACE model (see online Supplementary Method S2, Note S1 and Table S3).

Fig. 1. Path diagram of the unconstrained bivariate ACE model for NSSH and SSH.

In terms of the bivariate relationship, the genetic correlation between NSSH and SSH was 0.94. Based on Fig. 1, using path tracing we can derive the phenotypic correlation. This is done by tracing the paths from NSSH to SSH via the correlated latent A or E factors and summing up the products of these chains, i.e. (√0.55 × 0.94 × √0.50) + (√0.45 × 0.80 × √0.50), giving us a phenotypic correlation of 0.87. This means that ~57% of the phenotypic correlation is due to correlated genetic effects i.e. (√0.55 × 0.94 × √0.50)/0.87 = 0.57), and the rest of the correlation (~43%) accounted for by the non-shared environmental influences. As shown in online Supplementary Table S4, in the likelihood ratio test, the constrained bivariate ACE model was not a significantly worse fit than the non-constrained bivariate ACE model (p = 0.47), suggesting no aetiological difference between NSSH and SSH. We also found no evidence of quantitative sex differences in the aetiologies of NSSH and SSH (see online Supplementary Note S2, Tables S4–S6).

Trivariate models

Figure 2 shows the phenotypic correlations between each mental health measure with both NSSH and SSH. For NSSH, the measure that is positively correlated with it the most is child-rated MFQ for depressive symptoms, r = 0.38, whereas the least positively correlated measure is parent-rated CPRS hyperactivity subscale, r = 0.05. The measure that is negatively correlated with NSSH the most is the child-rated TEPS for hedonia, r = −0.21, whereas the least negatively correlated measure is child-rated grandiosity subscale from SPEQ, r = −0.05.

Fig. 2. Phenotypic correlations between each mental health measure and NSSH and SSH, and the contributions of A, C and E in the correlations. Measures beginning with letter c = child-rated, p = parent-rated. CONN = ADHD; EMOL = emotional lability; INAT = inattention; HYPER = hyperactivity/impulsivity; SWAN = ADHD symptoms (reversed); SMOK = smoking; CANN = cannabis use; ALC = alcohol; SDQ = total difficulties; AUT = autistic traits; MFQ = depression; INSOM = insomnia; EAT = eating disorder; ANX = anxiety; PRND = paranoia; CAPS = hallucinations; ANHE = anhedonia; SANS = negative symptoms; GRAND = grandiosity; TEPS = hedonia.

For SSH, the measure that is most positively correlated with it is also child-rated MFQ, r = 0.40, and the least positively correlated measure is parent-rated AQ for autism, r = 0.09. The mental health measure that is most negatively correlated with it is the child-rated SWAN for ADHD symptoms, r = −0.21, in which a higher SWAN score reflects fewer ADHD symptoms, whereas the least negatively correlated measure is child-rated grandiosity subscale from SPEQ, r = −0.03. The strengths of phenotypic correlations between different mental health measures with both NSSH and SSH are visualised in Fig. 2. Numerical results of the phenotypic correlations with 95% CIs, together with the contributions of genetic and environmental factors to the correlations are presented in online Supplementary Table S7. In general, a strikingly similar pattern of results was observed for NSSH v. SSH. For explanation for negative contribution to positive phenotypic correlations, see online Supplementary Note S3.

In terms of the genetic correlations (r g), as shown in Fig. 3, for NSSH, the most positive genetic correlation is with the child-rated Insomnia Severity Index, r g = 0.53. The measure with the most negative genetic correlation with NSSH is the child-rated TEPS for hedonia, r g = −0.36. For SSH, the most positive genetic correlation is with the child-rated MFQ for depressive symptoms, r g = 0.67, whereas the most negative genetic correlation is also with the child-rated TEPS, r g = −0.28.

Fig. 3. Genetic (R g) and non-shared environmental (R e) correlations between each mental health measure and both NSSH and SSH. Measures beginning with letter c = child-rated, p = parent-rated. HYPER = hyperactivity/impulsivity; CONN = ADHD; EMOL = emotional lability; INAT = inattention; SWAN = ADHD symptoms (reversed); ANX = anxiety; MFQ = depression; EAT = eating disorder; INSOM = insomnia; SDQ = total difficulties; AUT = autistic traits; SANS = negative symptoms; CAPS = hallucinations; ANHE = anhedonia; PRND = paranoia; GRAND = grandiosity; TEPS = hedonia; SMOK = smoking; CANN = cannabis use; ALC = alcohol.

For shared environmental correlations (r c), since both NSSH and SSH are under little shared environmental influences, most of the shared environmental correlations between the mental health measures were not significant, mostly with the pattern of r c = 0.99, and 95% CI ranging from −1 to 1.

For non-shared environmental correlations (r e), for NSSH, the most positive non-shared environmental correlation was with child-reported number of cigarettes used, r e = 0.28, whereas the most negative non-shared environmental correlation is with child-rated SWAN scores, r e = −0.10. Similarly, for SSH, the most positive and most negative non-shared environmental correlations were with the child-reported number of cigarettes used, r e = 0.32, and child-rated SWAN scores, r e = −0.16, respectively.

In general, the aetiological relationships between the mental health measures with NSSH do not differ substantially from that of SSH. The numerical results for the r g, r c and r e together with their estimated 95% CIs are presented in online Supplementary Table S8. In the likelihood ratio tests, three measures (parent-rated inattention, parent-rated depression and child-rated anhedonia) had their constrained trivariate models being worse than the non-constrained counterparts at nominal significance (p = 0.04, p = 0.02 and p = 0.03 respectively). However, after multiple testing correction, none of the constrained trivariate models was significantly worse than their non-constrained counterparts (see online Supplementary Table S9), suggesting that r g, r c and r e values between each mental health measure and self-harm do not vary depending on the type of self-harm, either NSSH or SSH.

Discussion

No evidence of aetiological difference between NSSH and SSH

In this study, we found that NSSH and SSH are strongly correlated. They have similar aetiological architectures, with substantial genetic and non-shared environmental influences, but no significant shared environmental influences. The high genetic and non-shared environmental correlations between NSSH and SSH imply that they are influenced by similar biological mechanisms and unique environmental factors, whereas there are very little unique genetic and environmental influences that contribute to each of them specifically. Hence, the first hypothesis is only partially supported. We found no evidence that NSSH and SSH differ in terms of their shared genetic and environmental aetiologies with a range of mental health problems. Hence, the second hypothesis was not supported. There were also no quantitative sex differences in the aetiologies of NSSH and SSH.

Our findings are in line with previous study, in which about half of the variance for SSH (or a more broadly defined suicidality) is explained by genetic influences, with approximately another half explained by non-shared environmental influences, and shared-environmental influences playing little to no role (Althoff et al., Reference Althoff, Hudziak, Willemsen, Hudziak, Bartels and Boomsma2012; Glowinski et al., Reference Glowinski, Bucholz, Nelson, Fu, Madden, Reich and Heath2001). An AE aetiological architecture also underlies NSSH (Durrett, Reference Durrett2006; Maciejewski et al., Reference Maciejewski, Creemers, Lynskey, Madden, Heath, Statham and Verweij2014). Previous study focusing on SSH only demonstrated significant genetic and non-shared environmental correlations between self-harm-related phenotypes and eating disorders, MDD (Thornton et al., Reference Thornton, Welch, Munn-Chernoff, Lichtenstein and Bulik2016; Wade et al., Reference Wade, Fairweather-Schmidt, Zhu and Martin2015), cannabis use (Delforterie et al., Reference Delforterie, Lynskey, Huizink, Creemers, Grant, Few and Agrawal2015) and high-risk trauma exposure (Richmond-Rakerd et al., Reference Richmond-Rakerd, Trull, Gizer, McLaughlin, Scheiderer, Nelson and Martin2019). Our study extended those findings by considering SSH and NSSH together with a range of mental health problems.

Self-harm as a continuum

As we found no evidence of aetiological difference between NSSH and SSH, our findings support the view that the distinction between NSSH and SSH may be a false dichotomy (Kapur et al., Reference Kapur, Cooper, O'Connor and Hawton2013), and should be regarded as a continuum, with NSSH and SSH at the opposite ends, and NSSH being a gateway towards SSH (Brausch & Gutierrez, Reference Brausch and Gutierrez2010; Linehan, Reference Linehan1986; Stanley et al., Reference Stanley, Winchel, Molcho, Simeon and Stanley1992). We also showed that NSSH is more prevalent than SSH, which fits the theory, as SSH is at the more severe end of the self-harm continuum and hence is less prevalent. The results also fit the interpersonal theory of suicide, in which NSSH may elevate one's acquired capability for lethal SSH (Joiner, Reference Joiner2007; Van Orden et al., Reference Van Orden, Witte, Cukrowicz, Braithwaite, Selby and Joiner2010). In addition, previous research found that most adolescents engage in self-harm for more than one reason (Scoliers et al., Reference Scoliers, Portzky, Madge, Hewitt, Hawton, Wilde and Heeringen2009). The intention of dying during self-harm can be ambivalent (Hawton, Cole, O'Grady, & Osborn, Reference Hawton, Cole, O'Grady and Osborn1982). It can change from episode to episode, or even within a single episode of self-harm (Cooper et al., Reference Cooper, Hunter, Owen-Smith, Gunnell, Donovan, Hawton and Kapur2011). Together, these findings question the utility of creating new diagnoses for NSSH and SSH in the DSM-5. However, it is important to note that the phenotypes used in the current study do not entirely fit into the definitions for NSSI and suicidal behaviour disorder in the DSM-5. Specifically, in our study, NSSH includes non-suicidal self-poisoning, but NSSI in DSM-5 is defined as ‘self-inflicted damage to the surface of the body’, which excludes non-suicidal self-poisoning (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

Clinical and research implications

The high r g and r e between NSSH and SSH suggest that for adolescents or young adults who engage in NSSH and progress to SSH, the underlying factors are likely to be influenced by similar biological mechanisms and unique environmental influences, suggesting opportunities to prevent both of them with similar preventative interventions. These shared underlying factors are also likely to be linked with the aetiologies of mental health problems ranging from internalising problems to externalising problems, substance use and psychotic-like experiences. Hence, interventions for these mental health problems may be helpful to address NSSH and SSH too.

The phenotypic correlations estimated between the mental health measures with both NSSH and SSH also suggest the role of mental health problems as risk factors for both NSSH and SSH. The mental health problems were measured at age 16, which is 5 years before lifetime NSSH and SSH questions were asked at age 21, suggesting longitudinal associations between these mental health problems with both NSSH and SSH. Nonetheless, it is also important to be cautious in interpreting these associations, as NSSH and SSH were measured using lifetime-ever items. Participants could therefore have engaged in self-harm prior to age 16.

The current study suggests that conducting research by regarding self-harm as a continuum and regardless of suicide intent may be helpful to acquire novel insights, particularly in investigating its biological mechanisms. This is supported by previous studies, for example, a genome-wide association study (GWAS) that regarded suicidality as a continuum, identified three novel genome-wide significant loci associated with suicidality (Strawbridge et al., Reference Strawbridge, Ward, Ferguson, Graham, Shaw, Cullen and Smith2019). Another GWAS that investigated broad sense self-harm also found a novel genome-wide significant locus associated with self-harm (Campos et al., Reference Campos, Verweij, Statham, Madden, Maciejewski, Davis and Rentería2020). This shows that investigating self-harm regardless of motivation could be valuable to gain biological insights into the liability of both self-harm and suicidality. The current study also shows that self-harm is a complex dimensional trait associated with a range of mental health problems. This is consistent with the Research Domain Criteria approach with emphasises transdiagnostic dimensions (Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn and Wang2010).

Strengths and limitations

One of the strengths in this study is that the self-harm measures are ordinal variables with multiple levels, instead of being binary variables with ‘Yes’ or ‘No’ responses. This yielded more statistical power. Another strength is the inclusion of parent-reported mental health measures, and that findings appear to be robust across informants. Having both male and female twins in this sample also enabled us to obtain the overall A, C and E estimates in the population, rather than only for a specific gender.

The limitations in the current study are mostly related to the classical twin design assumptions. One of them is the generalisability of twin research findings to the general population which are mostly singletons. However, the prevalence of lifetime NSSH (21.9%) and SSH (10.5%) in the current study seem to be comparable with the prevalence of lifetime NSSH and SSH in the 16–24 years age group in the Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey (17.5% and 9.0% respectively) conducted in England (Mcmanus et al., Reference Mcmanus, Hassiotis, Jenkins, Dennis, Aznar, Appleby, McManus, Bebbington, Jenkins and Brugha2016). Another assumption in the classical twin design is the absence of gene–environment interaction and correlation (Rijsdijk & Sham, Reference Rijsdijk and Sham2002), which may have led to bias of estimates in the current study.

Moreover, NSSH and SSH in this study were not measured at two separate times, hence their temporal relationship should be interpreted with caution. The stronger associations between self-rated mental health measures and self-reported self-harm may be due to shared-method variance (i.e. variance attributed to the measurement method rather than to the constructs represented by the measures; Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, Reference Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee and Podsakoff2003). Whenever possible, we included parental reports of similar mental health measure, so that we could compare the estimates derived from parent-rated and self-rated measures. However, this was not possible for all mental health measures, and is hence a limitation of the current study.

Using classical twin design, we only estimated correlations but not causal estimates. Hence, the causal relationships between the mental health problems with self-harm could not be determined here. However, recent findings using polygenic scores and Mendelian randomisation have shown that some of these mental health problems likely play a causal role in the aetiology of self-harm (Lim et al., Reference Lim, Rijsdijk, Hagenaars, Socrates, Choi, Coleman and Pingault2020). Future genetically informed studies that integrate polygenic scores and family-based designs can further strengthen causal inference and our understanding of the aetiology of self-harm (Pingault et al., Reference Pingault, O'Reilly, Schoeler, Ploubidis, Rijsdijk and Dudbridge2018). Furthermore, it is important to note that the study sample consisted of young adults, hence generalisation to older population should be done with caution, as suicidal behaviour and self-harm are manifested differently in different age groups (Mcmanus et al., Reference Mcmanus, Hassiotis, Jenkins, Dennis, Aznar, Appleby, McManus, Bebbington, Jenkins and Brugha2016, Reference McManus, Gunnell, Cooper, Bebbington, Howard, Brugha and Appleby2019). For the final limitation, we found that among those who reported to have self-harmed (regardless of intention), 11% of them reported to have never engaged in NSSH or SSH. As such, these individuals were not included in our analyses as we could not identify them to either have engaged in NSSH or SSH. This possibly contributed to a loss of statistical power due to the reduction in sample size. The reason for this may be due to the wording in the SSH question (‘Have you ever seriously wanted to kill yourself?’) as the word ‘seriously’ may have influenced those with ambivalent suicidal intent to answer ‘No’ in both NSSH and SSH questions. This also suggests that suicidal intent is fluid and difficult to be captured, and hence categorising self-harm into NSSH and SSH may be a potential threat in clinical practice as it may accidentally exclude those with ambivalent suicidal intent.

Conclusion

In conclusion, using a UK population-based twin sample, we found evidence for substantial overlap between lifetime NSSH and SSH at both phenotypic and aetiological levels among young adults. Among these young people, both NSSH and SSH also share similar aetiologies with a range of mental health problems. Together, the evidence suggests that among young adults at least, NSSH and SSH should be conceptualised as different ends of the same continuum, rather than two distinct categories.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721000040

Acknowledgements

TEDS is supported by a programme grant to Professor Robert Plomin from the UK Medical Research Council (MR/M021475/1 and previously G0901245), with additional support from the US National Institutes of Health (AG046938). We gratefully acknowledge the ongoing contribution of the participants in the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS) and their families.

Financial support

KXL is supported by King's International Postgraduate Research Scholarship. GK is funded by an MRC Clinical Research Training Fellowship (MR/N001400/1). JBP is a fellow of MQ: Transforming Mental Health (MQ16IP16) and is supported by the Medical Research Foundation 2018 Emerging Leaders 1st Prize in Adolescent Mental Health (MRF-160-0002-ELP-PINGA).

Conflict of interest

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Althoff, R. R., Hudziak, J. J., Willemsen, G., Hudziak, V., Bartels, M., & Boomsma, D. I. (2012). Genetic and environmental contributions to self-reported thoughts of self-harm and suicide. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 159 B(1), 120127. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.32010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Andreasen, N. C. (1989). The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS): Conceptual and theoretical foundations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 155(S7), 4952. https://doi.org/10.1192/S0007125000291496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angold, A., Costello, E. J., Messer, S. C., & Pickles, A. (1995). Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 5(4), 237249.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 517. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005653411471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bastien, C. H., Vallières, A., & Morin, C. M. (2001). Validation of the insomnia severity index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Medicine, 2(4), 297307. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1389-9457(00)00065-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, V., Halligan, P. W., & Ellis, H. D. (2006). The Cardiff Anomalous Perceptions Scale (CAPS): A new validated measure of anomalous perceptual experience. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32(2), 366377. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbj014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjureberg, J., Ohlis, A., Ljótsson, B., D'Onofrio, B. M., Hedman-Lagerlöf, E., Jokinen, J., … Hellner, C. (2019). Adolescent self-harm with and without suicidality: Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses of a Swedish regional register. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 60(3), 295304. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boker, S., Neale, M., Maes, H., Wilde, M., Spiegel, M., Brick, T., … Fox, J. (2011). OpenMx: An open source extended structural equation modeling framework. Psychometrika, 76(2), 306317. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-010-9200-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyd, A., Golding, J., Macleod, J., Lawlor, D. A., Fraser, A., Henderson, J., … Davey Smith, G. (2013). Cohort profile: The ‘children of the 90s’ – the index offspring of the Avon longitudinal study of parents and children. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42(1), 111127. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dys064.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brausch, A. M., & Gutierrez, P. M. (2010). Differences in non-suicidal self-injury and suicide attempts in adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39(3), 233242. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-009-9482-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, A. M., & Malone, K. (2013). Attempted suicide v. non-suicidal self-injury: Behaviour syndrome or diagnosis? British Journal of Psychiatry, 202(5), 324325. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.112.113506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campos, A. I., Verweij, K. J. H., Statham, D. J., Madden, P. A. F., Maciejewski, D. F., Davis, K. A. S., … Rentería, M. E. (2020). Genetic aetiology of self-harm ideation and behaviour. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 9713. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-66737-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cho, H., Guo, G., Iritani, B. J., & Hallfors, D. D. (2006). Genetic contribution to suicidal behaviors and associated risk factors among adolescents in the U.S. Prevention Science, 7(3), 303311. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-006-0042-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conners, C. K. (2001). CRS-R: Conners’ rating scales-revised: Technical manual. North Tonawanda, N.Y.: Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Cooper, J., Hunter, C., Owen-Smith, A., Gunnell, D., Donovan, J., Hawton, K., & Kapur, N. (2011). ‘Well it's like someone at the other end cares about you’. A qualitative study exploring the views of users and providers of care of contact-based interventions following self-harm. General Hospital Psychiatry, 33(2), 166176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2011.01.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delforterie, M. J., Lynskey, M. T., Huizink, A. C., Creemers, H. E., Grant, J. D., Few, L. R., … Agrawal, A. (2015). The relationship between cannabis involvement and suicidal thoughts and behaviors. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 150, 98104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.02.019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durrett, C. (2006). A behavior genetic study of self-harm, suicidality, and personality in African American and white women. University of Missouri-Columbia. Retrieved from https://mospace.umsystem.edu/xmlui/bitstream/handle/10355/4424/research.pdf?sequence=3.Google Scholar
Eley, T. C., Bolton, D., O'Connor, T. G., Perrin, S., Smith, P., & Plomin, R. (2003). A twin study of anxiety-related behaviours in pre-school children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(7), 945960. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falconer, D. S. (1965). The inheritance of liability to certain diseases, estimated from the incidence among relatives. Annals of Human Genetics, 29(1), 5176. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-1809.1965.tb00500.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, D., Garety, P. A., Bebbington, P. E., Smith, B., Rollinson, R., Fowler, D., … Dunn, G. (2005). Psychological investigation of the structure of paranoia in a non-clinical population. British Journal of Psychiatry, 186(MAY), 427435. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.186.5.427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fu, Q., Heath, A. C., Bucholz, K. K., Nelson, E. C., Glowinski, A. L., Goldberg, J., … Eisen, S. A. (2002). A twin study of genetic and environmental influences on suicidality in men. Psychological Medicine, 32(1), 1124. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291701004846.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gard, D. E., Gard, M. G., Kring, A. M., & John, O. P. (2006). Anticipatory and consummatory components of the experience of pleasure: A scale development study. Journal of Research in Personality, 40(6), 10861102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2005.11.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glowinski, A. L., Bucholz, K. K., Nelson, E. C., Fu, Q., Madden, P. A. F., Reich, W., & Heath, A. C. (2001). Suicide attempts in an adolescent female twin sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(11), 13001307. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200111000-00010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 581586. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawton, K., Cole, D., O'Grady, J., & Osborn, M. (1982). Motivational aspects of deliberate self-poisoning in adolescents. British Journal of Psychiatry, 141(3), 286291. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.141.3.286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawton, K., Zahl, D., & Weatherall, R. (2003). Suicide following deliberate self-harm: Long-term follow-up of patients who presented to a general hospital. British Journal of Psychiatry, 182(JUNE), 537542. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.182.6.537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., … Wang, P. (2010). Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(7), 748751. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobson, C. M., Muehlenkamp, J. J., Miller, A. L., & Turner, J. B. (2008). Psychiatric impairment among adolescents engaging in different types of deliberate self-harm. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 37(2), 363375. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374410801955771.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joiner, T. (2007). Why people die by suicide. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kapur, N., Cooper, J., O'Connor, R. C., & Hawton, K. (2013). Non-suicidal self-injury v. attempted suicide: New diagnosis or false dichotomy? British Journal of Psychiatry, 202(5), 326328. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.112.116111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klonsky, E. D., May, A. M., & Glenn, C. R. (2013). The relationship between nonsuicidal self-injury and attempted suicide: Converging evidence from four samples. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(1), 231237. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lim, K. X., Rijsdijk, F., Hagenaars, S. P., Socrates, A., Choi, S. W., Coleman, J. R. I., … Pingault, J.-B. (2020). Studying individual risk factors for self-harm in the UK Biobank: A polygenic scoring and Mendelian randomisation study. PLoS Medicine, 17(6), e1003137. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1003137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linehan, M. (1986). Suicidal people. One population or two? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 487(5), 1633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linker, J., Gillespie, N. A., Maes, H., Eaves, L., & Silberg, J. L. (2012). Suicidal ideation, depression, and conduct disorder in a sample of adolescent and young adult twins. Suicide & Life-Threatening Behavior, 42(4), 426436. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1943-278X.2012.00101.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maciejewski, D. F., Creemers, H. E., Lynskey, M. T., Madden, P. A. F., Heath, A. C., Statham, D. J., … Verweij, K. J. H. (2014). Overlapping genetic and environmental influences on nonsuicidal self-injury and suicidal ideation: Different outcomes, same etiology? JAMA Psychiatry, 71(6), 699705. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madge, N., Hewitt, A., Hawton, K., de Wilde, E. J., Corcoran, P., Fekete, S., … Ystgaard, M. (2008). Deliberate self-harm within an international community sample of young people: Comparative findings from the Child & Adolescent Self-harm in Europe (CASE) Study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(6), 667677. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01879.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mars, B., Heron, J., Crane, C., Hawton, K., Kidger, J., Lewis, G., … Gunnell, D. (2014). Differences in risk factors for self-harm with and without suicidal intent: Findings from the ALSPAC cohort. Journal of Affective Disorders, 168, 407414. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.07.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mason, O., Linney, Y., & Claridge, G. (2005). Short scales for measuring schizotypy. Schizophrenia Research, 78(2–3), 293296. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.SCHRES.2005.06.020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McManus, S., Gunnell, D., Cooper, C., Bebbington, P. E., Howard, L. M., Brugha, T., … Appleby, L. (2019). Prevalence of non-suicidal self-harm and service contact in England, 2000–14: Repeated cross-sectional surveys of the general population. The Lancet Psychiatry, 6(7), 573581. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30188-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mcmanus, S., Hassiotis, A., Jenkins, R., Dennis, M., Aznar, C., & Appleby, L. (2016). Suicidal thoughts, suicide attempts, and self-harm. In McManus, S., Bebbington, P., Jenkins, R., & Brugha, T. (Eds.), Mental health and wellbeing in England: Adult psychiatric morbidity survey 2014 (pp. 294322). Leeds: NHS Digital.Google Scholar
Morin, C. M. (1993). Insomnia: Psychological assessment and management. Insomnia: Psychological Assessment and Management. New York, NY, USA: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Muehlenkamp, J. J., & Gutierrez, P. M. (2007). Risk for suicide attempts among adolescents who engage in non-suicidal self-injury. Archives of Suicide Research, 11(1), 6982. https://doi.org/10.1080/13811110600992902.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nock, M. K., Joiner, T. E., Gordon, K. H., Lloyd-Richardson, E., & Prinstein, M. J. (2006). Non-suicidal self-injury among adolescents: Diagnostic correlates and relation to suicide attempts. Psychiatry Research, 144(1), 6572. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2006.05.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pingault, J.-B., O'Reilly, P. F., Schoeler, T., Ploubidis, G. B., Rijsdijk, F., & Dudbridge, F. (2018). Using genetic data to strengthen causal inference in observational research. Nature Reviews Genetics, 19(9), 566580. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41576-018-0020-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879903. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.r-project.org/.Google Scholar
Richmond-Rakerd, L. S., Trull, T. J., Gizer, I. R., McLaughlin, K., Scheiderer, E. M., Nelson, E. C., … Martin, N. G. (2019). Common genetic contributions to high-risk trauma exposure and self-injurious thoughts and behaviors. Psychological Medicine, 49(3), 421430. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718001034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rijsdijk, F. V., & Sham, P. C. (2002). Analytic approaches to twin data using structural equation models. Briefings in Bioinformatics, 3(2), 119133. https://doi.org/10.1093/bib/3.2.119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rimfeld, K., Malanchini, M., Spargo, T., Spickernell, G., Selzam, S., McMillan, A., … Plomin, R. (2019). Twins early development study: A genetically sensitive investigation into behavioral and cognitive development from infancy to emerging adulthood. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 22(6), 508513. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2019.56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ronald, A., Sieradzka, D., Cardno, A. G., Haworth, C. M. A., McGuire, P., & Freeman, D. (2014). Characterization of psychotic experiences in adolescence using the specific psychotic experiences questionnaire: Findings from a study of 5000 16-year-old twins. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40(4), 868877. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbt106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scoliers, G., Portzky, G., Madge, N., Hewitt, A., Hawton, K., Wilde, E. J., … Heeringen, K. (2009). Reasons for adolescent deliberate self-harm: A cry of pain and/or a cry for help?: Findings from the child and adolescent self-harm in Europe (CASE) study. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 44(8), 601607. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-008-0469-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silverman, W. K., Fleisig, W., Rabian, B., & Peterson, R. A. (1991). Childhood anxiety sensitivity index. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 20(2), 162168. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp2002_7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanley, B., Winchel, R., Molcho, A., Simeon, D., & Stanley, M. (1992). Suicide and the self-harm continuum: Phenomenological and biochemical evidence. International Review of Psychiatry, 4(2), 149155. https://doi.org/10.3109/09540269209066312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Statham, D. J., Heath, A. C., Madden, P. A. F., Bucholz, K. K., Bierut, L., Dinwiddie, S. H., … Martin, N. G. (1998). Suicidal behaviour: An epidemiological and genetic study. Psychological Medicine, 28(4), 839855. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291798006916.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stice, E., Fisher, M., & Martinez, E. (2004). Eating disorder diagnostic scale: Additional evidence of reliability and validity. Psychological Assessment, 16(1), 6071. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.16.1.60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strawbridge, R. J., Ward, J., Ferguson, A., Graham, N., Shaw, R. J., Cullen, B., … Smith, D. J. (2019). Identification of novel genome-wide associations for suicidality in UK Biobank, genetic correlation with psychiatric disorders and polygenic association with completed suicide. EBioMedicine, 41, 517525. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ebiom.2019.02.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, J. M., Schuck, S., Porter, M. M., Carlson, C., Hartman, C. A., Sergeant, J. A., … Wigal, T. (2012). Categorical and dimensional definitions and evaluations of symptoms of ADHD: History of the SNAP and the SWAN rating scales. The International Journal of Educational and Psychological Assessment, 10(1), 51. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4618695/.Google ScholarPubMed
Thornton, L. M., Welch, E., Munn-Chernoff, M. A., Lichtenstein, P., & Bulik, C. M. (2016). Anorexia nervosa, major depression, and suicide attempts: Shared genetic factors. Suicide & Life-Threatening Behavior, 46(5), 525534. https://doi.org/10.1111/sltb.12235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Orden, K. A., Witte, T. K., Cukrowicz, K. C., Braithwaite, S. R., Selby, E. A., & Joiner, T. E. (2010). The interpersonal theory of suicide. Psychological Review, 117(2), 575600. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018697.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voracek, M., & Loibl, L. M. (2007). Genetics of suicide: A systematic review of twin studies. Wiener Klinische Wochenschrift, 119(15–16), 463475. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00508-007-0823-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wade, T. D., Fairweather-Schmidt, A. K., Zhu, G., & Martin, N. G. (2015). Does shared genetic risk contribute to the co-occurrence of eating disorders and suicidality? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 48(6), 684691. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Health Organization. (2014). Preventing suicide – A global imperative (Vol. 91). Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnucene.2016.04.018.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Measures related to mental health collected at age 16

Figure 1

Table 2. Descriptive statistics for NSSH and SSH

Figure 2

Fig. 1. Path diagram of the unconstrained bivariate ACE model for NSSH and SSH.

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Phenotypic correlations between each mental health measure and NSSH and SSH, and the contributions of A, C and E in the correlations. Measures beginning with letter c = child-rated, p = parent-rated. CONN = ADHD; EMOL = emotional lability; INAT = inattention; HYPER = hyperactivity/impulsivity; SWAN = ADHD symptoms (reversed); SMOK = smoking; CANN = cannabis use; ALC = alcohol; SDQ = total difficulties; AUT = autistic traits; MFQ = depression; INSOM = insomnia; EAT = eating disorder; ANX = anxiety; PRND = paranoia; CAPS = hallucinations; ANHE = anhedonia; SANS = negative symptoms; GRAND = grandiosity; TEPS = hedonia.

Figure 4

Fig. 3. Genetic (Rg) and non-shared environmental (Re) correlations between each mental health measure and both NSSH and SSH. Measures beginning with letter c = child-rated, p = parent-rated. HYPER = hyperactivity/impulsivity; CONN = ADHD; EMOL = emotional lability; INAT = inattention; SWAN = ADHD symptoms (reversed); ANX = anxiety; MFQ = depression; EAT = eating disorder; INSOM = insomnia; SDQ = total difficulties; AUT = autistic traits; SANS = negative symptoms; CAPS = hallucinations; ANHE = anhedonia; PRND = paranoia; GRAND = grandiosity; TEPS = hedonia; SMOK = smoking; CANN = cannabis use; ALC = alcohol.

Supplementary material: File

Lim et al. supplementary material

Lim et al. supplementary material

Download Lim et al. supplementary material(File)
File 462.7 KB