Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T18:11:41.871Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Impaired self-recognition in individuals with no full-blown psychotic symptoms represented across the continuum of psychosis: a meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 May 2020

Layla Lavallé
Affiliation:
Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, PSYR2 Team, Lyon, F-69000, France Lyon University, F-69000, France Centre Hospitalier le Vinatier, Batiment 416, 95 Bd Pinel, 69678Bron, France
Clément Dondé
Affiliation:
Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, PSYR2 Team, Lyon, F-69000, France Lyon University, F-69000, France Centre Hospitalier le Vinatier, Batiment 416, 95 Bd Pinel, 69678Bron, France
Łukasz Gawęda
Affiliation:
Experimental Psychopathology Lab, Institute of Psychology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland
Jérome Brunelin*
Affiliation:
Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, PSYR2 Team, Lyon, F-69000, France Lyon University, F-69000, France Centre Hospitalier le Vinatier, Batiment 416, 95 Bd Pinel, 69678Bron, France
Marine Mondino
Affiliation:
Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, INSERM U1028, CNRS UMR5292, PSYR2 Team, Lyon, F-69000, France Lyon University, F-69000, France Experimental Psychopathology Lab, Institute of Psychology, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland
*
Author for correspondence: Jérome Brunelin, E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Background

Impairments in self-recognition (i.e. recognition of own thoughts and actions) have been repeatedly shown in individuals with schizophrenia. According to classical clinical characterizations, schizophrenia is included in a continuum encompassing a large range of genetic statuses, psychotic states and symptoms. The current meta-analysis aims to determine whether self-recognition is affected by individuals within the psychosis continuum.

Method

Three populations were considered: people with an at-risk mental state for psychosis (ARMS), hallucination-prone individuals and unaffected relatives of patients with schizophrenia. Eleven studies contrasted self-recognition between these three populations (n = 386) and healthy controls (n = 315) and four studies used correlational analysis to estimate comparable effects (n = 629). Eligible studies used experimental paradigms including source-monitoring and self-monitoring.

Results

We observed significantly reduced self-recognition accuracy in these populations [g = −0.44 (−0.71 to −0.17), p = 0.002] compared to controls. No influence of the type of population, experimental paradigm or study design was observed.

Conclusion

The present analysis argues for self-recognition deficits in populations with no full-blown psychotic symptoms represented across the continuum of psychosis.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aldebot Sacks, S., Weisman de Mamani, A. G., & Garcia, C. P. (2012). Associations between cognitive biases and domains of schizotypy in a non-clinical sample. Psychiatry Research, 196(1), 115122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.09.019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alderson-Day, B., Smailes, D., Moffatt, J., Mitrenga, K., Moseley, P., & Fernyhough, C. (2019). Intentional inhibition but not source memory is related to hallucination-proneness and intrusive thoughts in a university sample. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 113, 267278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.12.020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, P., Amaro, E., Fu, C. H. Y., Williams, S. C. R., Brammer, M. J., Johns, L. C., & McGuire, P. K. (2007). Neural correlates of the misattribution of speech in schizophrenia. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 190, 162169. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.106.025700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, P., Freeman, D., Johns, L., & McGuire, P. (2006). Misattribution of self-generated speech in relation to hallucinatory proneness and delusional ideation in healthy volunteers. Schizophrenia Research, 84(2–3), 281288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2006.01.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bentall, R. P. (1990). The illusion of reality: A review and integration of psychological research on hallucinations. Psychological Bulletin, 107(1), 8295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bora, E., & Murray, R. M. (2014). Meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in ultra-high risk to psychosis and first-episode psychosis: Do the cognitive deficits progress over, or after, the onset of psychosis? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40(4), 744755. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbt085.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2010). A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Research Synthesis Methods, 1(2), 97111. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brébion, G., Gorman, J. M., Amador, X., Malaspina, D., & Sharif, Z. (2002). Source monitoring impairments in schizophrenia: Characterisation and associations with positive and negative symptomatology. Psychiatry Research, 112(1), 2739. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1781(02)00187-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brébion, G., Larøi, F., & Van der Linden, M. (2010). Associations of hallucination proneness with free-recall intrusions and response bias in a nonclinical sample. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(8), 847854. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803391003596397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brébion, G., Ohlsen, R. I., Bressan, R. A., & David, A. S. (2012). Source memory errors in schizophrenia, hallucinations and negative symptoms: A synthesis of research findings. Psychological Medicine, 42(12), 25432554. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171200075X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brookwell, M. L., Bentall, R. P., & Varese, F. (2013). Externalizing biases and hallucinations in source-monitoring, self-monitoring and signal detection studies: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Medicine, 43(12), 24652475. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291712002760.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunelin, J., Combris, M., Poulet, E., Kallel, L., D'Amato, T., Dalery, J., & Saoud, M. (2006a). Source monitoring deficits in hallucinating compared to non-hallucinating patients with schizophrenia. European Psychiatry: The Journal of the Association of European Psychiatrists, 21(4), 259261. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2006.01.015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunelin, J., d'Amato, T., Brun, P., Bediou, B., Kallel, L., Senn, M., … Saoud, M. (2007). Impaired verbal source monitoring in schizophrenia: An intermediate trait vulnerability marker? Schizophrenia Research, 89(1–3), 287292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2006.08.028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunelin, J., Poulet, E., Bediou, B., Kallel, L., Dalery, J., D'amato, T., & Saoud, M. (2006b). Low frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation improves source monitoring deficit in hallucinating patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 81(1), 4145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2005.10.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chien, P. F., Khan, K. S., & Siassakos, D. (2012). Registration of systematic reviews: PROSPERO: Editorial. BJOG: An International Journal of Obstetrics & Gynaecology, 119(8), 903905. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-0528.2011.03242.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (2009). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.)). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum associates.Google Scholar
Cooper, H., Hedges, L. V., & Valentine, J. C. (2009). The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Eisenacher, S., Rausch, F., Ainser, F., Englisch, S., Becker, A., Mier, D., … Zink, M. (2018). Early cognitive basic symptoms are accompanied by neurocognitive impairment in patients with an “at-risk mental state” for psychosis. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 12(4), 586595. https://doi.org/10.1111/eip.12350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faraone, S. V., Seidman, L. J., Kremen, W. S., Pepple, J. R., Lyons, M. J., & Tsuang, M. T. (1995). Neuropsychological functioning among the nonpsychotic relatives of schizophrenic patients: A diagnostic efficiency analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104(2), 286304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ford, J. M., & Mathalon, D. H. (2005). Corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia: Can it explain auditory hallucinations? International Journal of Psychophysiology: Official Journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology, 58(2–3), 179189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.01.014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, C. D. (1996). Neuropsychology of schizophrenia, what are the implications of intellectual and experiential abnormalities for the neurobiology of schizophrenia? British Medical Bulletin, 52(3), 618626. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a011571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S. J., & Wolpert, D. M. (2000). Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 355(1404), 17711788. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2000.0734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, C. D., & Done, D. J. (1988). Towards a neuropsychology of schizophrenia. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 153, 437443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fusar-Poli, P., Borgwardt, S., Bechdolf, A., Addington, J., Riecher-Rössler, A., Schultze-Lutter, F., … Yung, A. (2013). The psychosis high-risk state: A comprehensive state-of-the-art review. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(1), 107120. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fusar-Poli, P., Deste, G., Smieskova, R., Barlati, S., Yung, A. R., Howes, O., … Borgwardt, S. (2012). Cognitive functioning in prodromal psychosis: A meta-analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(6), 562571. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrison, J. R., Moseley, P., Alderson-Day, B., Smailes, D., Fernyhough, C., & Simons, J. S. (2017). Testing continuum models of psychosis: No reduction in source monitoring ability in healthy individuals prone to auditory hallucinations. Cortex, 91, 197207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.11.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gawęda, Ł., Li, E., Lavoie, S., Whitford, T. J., Moritz, S., & Nelson, B. (2018). Impaired action self-monitoring and cognitive confidence among ultra-high risk for psychosis and first-episode psychosis patients. European Psychiatry, 47, 6775. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.09.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilpin, A. R. (1993). Table for conversion of Kendall'S Tau to Spearman'S Rho within the context of measures of magnitude of effect for meta-analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53(1), 8792. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164493053001007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giuliano, A. J., Li, H., Mesholam-Gately, R. I., Sorenson, S. M., Woodberry, K. A., & Seidman, L. J. (2012). Neurocognition in the psychosis risk syndrome: A quantitative and qualitative review. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 18(4), 399415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, M. F., Nuechterlein, K. H., & Breitmeyer, B. (1997). Backward masking performance in unaffected siblings of schizophrenic patients. Evidence for a vulnerability indicator. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54(5), 465472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gupta, T., DeVylder, J. E., Auerbach, R. P., Schiffman, J., & Mittal, V. A. (2018). Speech illusions and working memory performance in non-clinical psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 195, 391395. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2017.10.031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henkel, L. A., Johnson, M. K., & De Leonardis, D. M. (1998). Aging and source monitoring: Cognitive processes and neuropsychological correlates. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127(3), 251268. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.127.3.251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, J. P. T., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 327(7414), 557560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoyt, W. T., & Del Re, A. C. (2018). Effect size calculation in meta-analyses of psychotherapy outcome research. Psychotherapy Research: Journal of the Society for Psychotherapy Research, 28(3), 379388. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2017.1405171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humpston, C. S., Linden, D. E. J., & Evans, L. H. (2017). Deficits in reality and internal source monitoring of actions are associated with the positive dimension of schizotypy. Psychiatry Research, 250, 4449. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.01.063.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jardri, R., Pouchet, A., Pins, D., & Thomas, P. (2011). Cortical activations during auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia: A coordinate-based meta-analysis. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(1), 7381. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09101522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johns, L. C., Allen, P., Valli, I., Winton-Brown, T., Broome, M., Woolley, J., … McGuire, P. (2010). Impaired verbal self-monitoring in individuals at high risk of psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 40(9), 14331442. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291709991991.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johns, L. C., & van Os, J. (2001). The continuity of psychotic experiences in the general population. Clinical Psychology Review, 21(8), 11251141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114(1), 328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keefe, R. S. E., Poe, M. P., McEvoy, J. P., & Vaughan, A. (2003). Source monitoring improvement in patients with schizophrenia receiving antipsychotic medications. Psychopharmacology, 169(3–4), 383389. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-003-1476-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kmet, L. M., Lee, R. C., & Cook, L. S. (2004) Alberta Heritage Foundation for Medical Research, University of Calgary, Faculty of Medicine, & Calgary Health Region. Standard quality assessment criteria for evaluating primary research papers from a variety of fields. Alberta Heritage Foundation for Medical Research.Google Scholar
Lagioia, A., Eliez, S., Schneider, M., Simons, J. S., Van der Linden, M., & Debbané, M. (2011). Neural correlates of reality monitoring during adolescence. NeuroImage, 55(3), 13931400. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.058.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larøi, F., Collignon, O., & Van der Linden, M. (2005). Source monitoring for actions in hallucination proneness. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 10(2), 105123. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546800344000354.Google ScholarPubMed
Larøi, F., Van der Linden, M., & Marczewski, P. (2004). The effects of emotional salience, cognitive effort and meta-cognitive beliefs on a reality monitoring task in hallucination-prone subjects. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43(Pt 3), 221233. https://doi.org/10.1348/0144665031752970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marjoram, D., Miller, P., McIntosh, A. M., Cunningham Owens, D. G., Johnstone, E. C., & Lawrie, S. (2006). A neuropsychological investigation into “theory of mind” and enhanced risk of schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 144(1), 2937. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2006.01.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGorry, P. D., Hartmann, J. A., Spooner, R., & Nelson, B. (2018). Beyond the “at risk mental state” concept: Transitioning to transdiagnostic psychiatry. World Psychiatry: Official Journal of the World Psychiatric Association (WPA), 17(2), 133142. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGorry, P. D., & Killackey, E. J. (2002). Early intervention in psychosis: A new evidence based paradigm. Epidemiologia E Psichiatria Sociale, 11(4), 237247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, K. J., & Johnson, M. K. (2009). Source monitoring 15 years later: What have we learned from fMRI about the neural mechanisms of source memory? Psychological Bulletin, 135(4), 638677. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., & Altman, D. G., & PRISMA Group. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine, 6(7), e1000097. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mondino, M., Haesebaert, F., Poulet, E., Suaud-Chagny, M.-F., & Brunelin, J. (2015). Fronto-temporal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) reduces source-monitoring deficits and auditory hallucinations in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 161(2–3), 515516. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2014.10.054.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrison, A. P., French, P., Walford, L., Lewis, S. W., Kilcommons, A., Green, J., … Bentall, R. P. (2004). Cognitive therapy for the prevention of psychosis in people at ultra-high risk: Randomised controlled trial. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 185, 291297. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.185.4.291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrison, A. P., Wells, A., & Nothard, S. (2000). Cognitive factors in predisposition to auditory and visual hallucinations. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39(Pt 1), 6778.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, B., Yuen, K., & Yung, A. R. (2011). Ultra high risk (UHR) for psychosis criteria: Are there different levels of risk for transition to psychosis? Schizophrenia Research, 125(1), 6268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2010.10.017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ohmuro, N., Katsura, M., Obara, C., Kikuchi, T., Hamaie, Y., Sakuma, A., … Matsumoto, K. (2018). The relationship between cognitive insight and cognitive performance among individuals with at-risk mental state for developing psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 192, 281286. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2017.04.031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owen, M. J., Sawa, A., & Mortensen, P. B. (2016). Schizophrenia. Lancet (London, England), 388(10039), 8697. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(15)01121-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, M. J. V., Smeets, T., Giesbrecht, T., Jelicic, M., & Merckelbach, H. (2007). Confusing action and imagination: Action source monitoring in individuals with schizotypal traits. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 195(9), 752757. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e318142cc02.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polanin, JR, & Snilstveit, B. (2016). Campbell Methods Policy Note on Converting Between Effect Sizes. Oslo: The Campbell Collaboration. https://doi.org/10.4073/ cmpn.2016.3Google Scholar
R Core Team. (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Retrieved from https://www.r-project.org/.Google Scholar
Saoud, M., d'Amato, T., Gutknecht, C., Triboulet, P., Bertaud, J. P., Marie-Cardine, M., … Rochet, T. (2000). Neuropsychological deficit in siblings discordant for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26(4), 893902. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simons, J. S., Davis, S. W., Gilbert, S. J., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2006). Discriminating imagined from perceived information engages brain areas implicated in schizophrenia. NeuroImage, 32(2), 696703. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.04.209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugimori, E., Mitchell, K. J., Raye, C. L., Greene, E. J., & Johnson, M. K. (2014). Brain mechanisms underlying reality monitoring for heard and imagined words. Psychological Science, 25(2), 403413. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613505776.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szöke, A., Méary, A., Ferchiou, A., Trandafir, A., Leboyer, M., & Schürhoff, F. (2009). Correlations between cognitive performances and psychotic or schizotypal dimensions. European Psychiatry: The Journal of the Association of European Psychiatrists, 24(4), 244250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2008.10.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verdoux, H., & van Os, J. (2002). Psychotic symptoms in non-clinical populations and the continuum of psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 54(1–2), 5965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Versmissen, D., Janssen, I., Johns, L., McGuire, P., Drukker, M., Campo, J. À, … Krabbendam, L. (2007a). Verbal self-monitoring in psychosis: A non-replication. Psychological Medicine, 37(4), 569576. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291706009780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Versmissen, D., Myin-Germeys, I., Janssen, I., Franck, N., Georgieff, N., Campo, J. A., … Krabbendam, L. (2007b). Impairment of self-monitoring: Part of the endophenotypic risk for psychosis. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 51 (Suppl), s58s62. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.191.51.s58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wampold, B. E., Mondin, G. W., Moody, M., Stich, F., Benson, K., & Ahn, H. (1997). A meta-analysis of outcome studies comparing bona fide psychotherapies: Empiricially, “all must have prizes.” Psychological Bulletin, 122(3), 203215. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.122.3.203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waters, F., Woodward, T., Allen, P., Aleman, A., & Sommer, I. (2012). Self-recognition deficits in schizophrenia patients with auditory hallucinations: A meta-analysis of the literature. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(4), 741750. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbq144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodward, T. S., Menon, M., & Whitman, J. C. (2007). Source monitoring biases and auditory hallucinations. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 12(6), 477494. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546800701307198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: File

Lavallé et al. supplementary material

Lavallé et al. supplementary material 1

Download Lavallé et al. supplementary material(File)
File 15.2 KB
Supplementary material: File

Lavallé et al. supplementary material

Lavallé et al. supplementary material 2

Download Lavallé et al. supplementary material(File)
File 21.4 KB
Supplementary material: File

Lavallé et al. supplementary material

Lavallé et al. supplementary material 3

Download Lavallé et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.2 MB