Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T00:28:11.991Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Higher emotion regulation flexibility predicts more stable negative emotions and faster affective recovery in early psychosis: an experience sampling study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 February 2024

Xu Li
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Central China Normal University, Wuhan, 430079, China Department of Neurosciences, Psychiatry Research Group, Center for Contextual Psychiatry, KU Leuven, Leuven, 3000, Belgium
Thomas Vaessen
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences, Psychiatry Research Group, Center for Contextual Psychiatry, KU Leuven, Leuven, 3000, Belgium Department of Psychology, Center for eHealth and Well-being Research, Health, and Technology, University of Twente, Twente, 7500 AE, The Netherlands
Ginette Lafit
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences, Psychiatry Research Group, Center for Contextual Psychiatry, KU Leuven, Leuven, 3000, Belgium Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Research Group of Quantitative Psychology and Individual Differences, KU Leuven, Leuven, 3000, Belgium
Evelyne van Aubel
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences, Psychiatry Research Group, Center for Contextual Psychiatry, KU Leuven, Leuven, 3000, Belgium
Anu P. Hiekkaranta
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences, Psychiatry Research Group, Center for Contextual Psychiatry, KU Leuven, Leuven, 3000, Belgium
Marlies Houben
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences, Psychiatry Research Group, Center for Contextual Psychiatry, KU Leuven, Leuven, 3000, Belgium Department of Medical and Clinical Psychology, Tilburg University, 5037 AB Tilburg, The Netherlands
Annelie Beijer-Klippel
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences, Psychiatry Research Group, Center for Contextual Psychiatry, KU Leuven, Leuven, 3000, Belgium Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, 6229 GT, The Netherlands Faculty of Psychology, Department of Lifespan Psychology, Open University, Heerlen, 6419 AT, The Netherlands
Lieuwe de Haan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam UMC, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, 1105 AZ, The Netherlands
Frederike Schirmbeck
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam UMC, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, 1105 AZ, The Netherlands Department of Public Mental Health, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, University of Heidelberg, Mannheim, 68167, Germany
Ulrich Reininghaus
Affiliation:
Department of Public Mental Health, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, University of Heidelberg, Mannheim, 68167, Germany Health Service and Population Research Department, ESRC Centre for Society and Mental Health and Centre for Epidemiology and Public Health, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, WC2R 2LS, UK
Inez Myin-Germeys*
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences, Psychiatry Research Group, Center for Contextual Psychiatry, KU Leuven, Leuven, 3000, Belgium
*
Corresponding author: Inez Myin-Germeys; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

While evidence shows that people with early psychosis are flexible in using different emotion regulation (ER) strategies to manage the varying contextual demands, no studies have examined the effectiveness of such regulatory flexibility in this population. We addressed this issue by investigating whether and how ER flexibility relate to different dynamic aspects (variability, instability, inertia, and recovery) of negative affect (NA) in a combined early psychosis sample, consisting of both individuals at high clinical risk for psychosis and those diagnosed with first-episode psychosis.

Methods

Participants were 148 individuals from the INTERACT project, a multi-center randomized controlled trial on the efficacy of acceptance and commitment therapy in early psychosis. We utilized data from the baseline assessment, during which all participants completed six days of experience sampling assessment of momentary NA, as well as end-of-day assessments of ER strategy use.

Results

Multilevel models of within-person associations showed that greater ER flexibility was associated with more stable NA, and quicker recovery of NA from stressors during the day. Linear regression analyses of between-person associations showed that people who had more variable and unstable NA reported greater ER flexibility generally. No evidence was found for associations with NA inertia.

Conclusions

The current study identified unique within-person and between-person links between ER flexibility and dynamics of NA in early psychosis. These findings further provide evidence for ER flexibility in early psychosis, emphasizing the adaptive nature of regulatory flexibility in relation to reduced instability in NA and faster recovery from NA in everyday life.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Emotion dysregulation, which refers to difficulty in regulating emotions, has been identified as a mechanism significantly contributing to the development and maintenance of many psychiatric disorders, including psychosis (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer2010; Lincoln, Schulze, & Renneberg, Reference Lincoln, Schulze and Renneberg2022). Problems in emotion regulation (ER) occur across the psychosis continuum, being present both in individuals at clinical high risk as well as in persons with a diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizo-affective disorder (Chapman et al., Reference Chapman, Visser, Mittal, Gibb, Coles and Strauss2020). ER dysfunctions are linked to more severe positive and negative symptoms in early as well as in chronic psychosis (Lincoln, Sundag, Schlier, & Karow, Reference Lincoln, Sundag, Schlier and Karow2018; Liu, Subramaniam, Chong, & Mahendran, Reference Liu, Subramaniam, Chong and Mahendran2020), thus constituting a vital area of study regarding risk for psychosis.

While previous research has largely examined how patients with psychosis differ from healthy participants in using different ER strategies with experimental (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Hwang, Park, Kim, Oh and Kwon2021; Strauss et al., Reference Strauss, Kappenman, Culbreth, Catalano, Lee and Gold2013) and self-report measures (Chapman et al., Reference Chapman, Visser, Mittal, Gibb, Coles and Strauss2020; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Subramaniam, Chong and Mahendran2020), the emerging dynamic perspective have motivated researchers to move beyond the focus on specific ER strategy use to a more comprehensive conceptualization emphasizing more dynamic features of ER (Murphy & Young, Reference Murphy and Young2018). Specifically, the ER flexibility framework proposed that regulatory flexibility reflecting the extent to which an individual dynamically varies their efforts in the use of different strategies to match the situational demands (Aldao, Sheppes, & Gross, Reference Aldao, Sheppes and Gross2015), is linked to more adaptive emotional and behavioral outcomes (Bonanno & Burton, Reference Bonanno and Burton2013), and as such, central to regulatory success. Indirect evidence supporting the adaptive functioning of ER flexibility comes from work showing that psychological inflexibility, which refers to the lack of psychological flexibility and is defined as the inflexible, rigid responses to internal experiences, relates to many forms of psychopathology (Kashdan & Rottenberg, Reference Kashdan and Rottenberg2010). For example, lower levels of psychological flexibility were found to be associated with higher stress levels and more severe anxiety and depressive symptoms (Grégoire, Chénier, Doucerain, Lachance, & Shankland, Reference Grégoire, Chénier, Doucerain, Lachance and Shankland2020; Hemi et al., Reference Hemi, Sopp, Bonanno, Michael, McGiffin and Levy-Gigi2023). An emerging body of research further suggests that a greater variety in ER strategies, a well-established indicator of ER flexibility, is associated with less negative affect (NA) and less severe psychopathology symptoms in healthy populations (Blanke et al., Reference Blanke, Brose, Kalokerinos, Erbas, Riediger and Kuppens2019; Wang, Blain, Meng, Liu, & Qiu, Reference Wang, Blain, Meng, Liu and Qiu2021; Wenzel, Blanke, Rowland, & Kubiak, Reference Wenzel, Blanke, Rowland and Kubiak2021), further supporting the position that ER flexibility represents an adaptive emotional functioning. Importantly, a recent study has reported that people with early psychosis are able to adapt their using of the different ER strategies in response to varying contextual demands (Li et al., Reference Li, Lafit, van Aubel, Vaessen, Hiekkaranta, Houben and Myin-Germeys2023), suggesting ER flexibility in early psychosis, yet whether they use these strategies effectively requires further empirical testing.

ER and the adoption of different ER strategies to regulate emotions influences the way emotions fluctuate and change over time (i.e. emotion dynamics) (Kuppens & Verduyn, Reference Kuppens and Verduyn2015). Emotional changes that are extreme and/or rigid may reflect failures in ER and are relevant for predicting negative mental health outcomes. For example, higher levels of emotional variability (i.e. the extent of emotional fluctuations), instability (i.e. the magnitude and temporal dependency of emotional fluctuations from moment to moment), and inertia (i.e. the resistance to emotional fluctuations) are found to be associated with more severe psychopathology symptoms (such as anxiety, depression, borderline personality disorder), over and above the mean levels of emotions (Houben, Van Den Noortgate, & Kuppens, Reference Houben, Van Den Noortgate and Kuppens2015; Houben & Kuppens, Reference Houben and Kuppens2020; Maciejewski et al., Reference Maciejewski, van Roekel, Ha, De France, Lin, Lennarz and Verhagen2023). Disruptions in emotional dynamics have been reported with greater variability and instability, and higher inertia in NA in people with psychosis (Myin-Germeys, Delespaul, & deVries, Reference Myin-Germeys, Delespaul and deVries2000; Nowak, Wood et al., Reference Nowak, Wood, Dinu, Wittkamp, Clamor, Oravecz and Lincoln2022b; Oorschot et al., Reference Oorschot, Lataster, Thewissen, Lardinois, Wichers, van Os and Myin-Germeys2013; Weermeijer et al., Reference Weermeijer, Lafit, Kiekens, Wampers, Eisele, Kasanova and Myin-Germeys2022). These alterations are already present in the early stages of psychosis (Hermans et al., Reference Hermans, Myin-Germeys, Gayer-Anderson, Kempton, Valmaggia, McGuire and Reininghaus2020) and are postulated to contribute to the exacerbation and maintenance of psychotic symptoms (Krkovic, Clamor, Schlier, & Lincoln, Reference Krkovic, Clamor, Schlier and Lincoln2020). Additionally, affective recovery, which refers to the degree to which affect returns to baseline after stress, also characterizes the temporal dynamics of emotions and is assumed to be dependent on the effectiveness of ER. The inability to regulate emotions can lead to delays in recovery. Notably, delayed recovery of NA from stressors has been uniquely related to early psychosis and not chronic psychosis (Vaessen et al., Reference Vaessen, Viechtbauer, van der Steen, Gayer-Anderson, Kempton, Valmaggia and Myin-Germeys2019), suggesting that the early stages of psychosis are particularly marked by slower NA recovery. However, despite the conceptual connections between ER flexibility and emotional dynamics, to date they have only been studied independently.

Our aim in the current study was to investigate the effectiveness of ER strategy use in early psychosis. To this end, we tested whether and how ER flexibility is related to temporal features of NA variability, instability, inertia, and recovery. Importantly, to fully capture ER strategy use and emotion dynamics in context and minimize recall biases, experience sampling methodology (ESM) was used to assess momentary fluctuations in NA, as well as ER strategy use in people with early psychosis. Acceptability and feasibility of ESM has already been demonstrated in clinical samples (Myin-Germeys et al., Reference Myin-Germeys, Kasanova, Vaessen, Vachon, Kirtley, Viechtbauer and Reininghaus2018). Importantly, one particular strength offered by the ESM approach is that it allows characterizing and disaggregating the within-person level associations (i.e. how ER flexibility relates to the different aspects of emotional dynamics within the same individual) from between-person level associations (i.e. are people with psychosis who have higher levels of ER flexibility different from those who have lower levels of ER flexibility in emotional dynamics). We hypothesized that when early psychosis individuals are more flexible in using different ER strategies, they would experience less fluctuations in NA (i.e. less variable, unstable, and inert) and recover quicker from stressors, and tested these hypotheses using both within- and between-person approaches.

Methods

Participants

One hundred and forty-eight individuals in the early stage of psychosis were recruited to participate in the INTERACT study, a multi-center randomized controlled trial investigating the efficacy of acceptance and commitment therapy in daily life (Myin-Germeys et al., Reference Myin-Germeys, van Aubel, Vaessen, Steinhart, Klippel, Lafit and Reininghaus2022). All participants aged between 15 and 65 were at high clinical risk for psychosis or had a first episode of psychosis, assessed with the Comprehensive Assessment of At Risk Mental State (Yung et al., Reference Yung, Yuen, McGorry, Phillips, Kelly, Dell'Olio and Buckby2005) and Nottingham Onset Schedule (Singh et al., Reference Singh, Cooper, Fisher, Tarrant, Lloyd, Banjo and Jones2005). For the purpose of the current study, we used data from the baseline assessment before intervention. The trial was approved by the Maastricht University Medical Centre (MUMC), the Netherlands (reference: NL46439.068.13) and the University Clinic Leuven, Belgium (reference: B322201629214). Written informed consents were obtained from participants at the beginning of the study. The full study methodology is detailed in the study protocol (Reininghaus et al., Reference Reininghaus, Klippel, Steinhart, Vaessen, van Nierop, Viechtbauer and Myin-Germeys2019).

Data collection

Experience sampling method

All participants participated in a 6-days ESM studies (Myin-Germeys et al., Reference Myin-Germeys, Kasanova, Vaessen, Vachon, Kirtley, Viechtbauer and Reininghaus2018; Myin-Germeys & Kuppens, Reference Myin-Germeys and Kuppens2021), in which they were prompted with 10 beeps using the PsyMate™ app (Myin-Germeys, Birchwood, & Kwapil, Reference Myin-Germeys, Birchwood and Kwapil2011) between 7:30 am to 10:30 pm in a 90-minute semi-random schedule, to assess momentary NA and stress. Specifically, NA was measured with five items: I feel lonely, anxious, down, guilty, and insecure, on a 1 to 7 Likert scale. The multilevel reliability (Omega coefficient) for these five NA items is 0.725 at the within-person level and 0.924 at the between-person level, indicating good reliability. Scores on these items were averaged to form a composite score representing NA at each beep. Stress was identified by asking participants at each ESM beep ‘Think of the most important event for you since the last beep, this event was____’, rated from −3 (very unpleasant) to 3 (very pleasant). In order to identify stressful events, responses to the item were excluded if they were 1 and higher (which indicated pleasant event). The item was relevant to the calculation of NA recovery only.

In addition, participants were also instructed to complete an evening questionnaire assessing ER strategy use, which was prompted between 8:00 PM and 11:00 PM throughout the ESM period. Specifically, each evening, participants reported on their use of six ER strategies (rumination, reappraisal, acceptance, expression, avoidance, situation modification), they were firstly asked to think about the most negative event of the day, then they rated each ER item on a 1–7 Likert scale (1 = ‘not’, 7 = “very much). Participants also indicated their experience of emotional intensity of the event: “This event was” (−3 = “very unpleasant”, 3 = “very pleasant”). Days where the most negative event of the day was rated 1 or higher (which indicated positive event) were excluded from further analysis.

Statistical analyses

Measures

ER flexibility and ER effort In accordance with the framework proposed by Aldao et al. (Reference Aldao, Sheppes and Gross2015) and previous evidence (Blanke et al. Reference Blanke, Brose, Kalokerinos, Erbas, Riediger and Kuppens2019; Wenzel et al. Reference Wenzel, Blanke, Rowland and Kubiak2021), we operationalized ER flexibility as levels of variations in using different strategies. Both within-person and between-person parameters of ER flexibility and ER effort were calculated. Specifically, for each participant, daily ER flexibility was calculated as the average amount of variability on the use of different ER strategies on each day, i.e. the standard deviation (s.d.) of scores across the six ER items indicated in the evening assessment. Daily mean ER effort was calculated as the average scores on the six ER items in the evening assessment. Then, to get the between-person parameters, daily ER flexibility (or daily mean ER effort) over the ESM period were averaged to obtain the person-level ER flexibility (or person-level ER effort).

NA dynamics Dynamics in NA were conceptualized as variability, instability, inertia, and recovery. Specifically, we firstly calculated emotional dynamics per day, which were used to estimate within-person effects. Daily NA variability was calculated as the s.d. of NA across the ten beeps on each day (Hermans et al., Reference Hermans, Myin-Germeys, Gayer-Anderson, Kempton, Valmaggia, McGuire and Reininghaus2020). Daily NA instability was calculated as root mean square of successive differences (RMSSD) between consecutive beeps, within days; daily NA inertia was calculated as the auto-regressive effect within days, i.e. NA at beep t was regressed onto NA at beep t-1. Difference scores between two observations overnight were excluded. Recovery was defined as the median time took for NA to be equal to or lower than the mean NA following the first stressor of the day. Additionally, daily mean NA was calculated as the average score across the 10 beeps of the day. Then, for each measure, scores were averaged over the ESM period, yielding a stable measure of the person-level mean of the individual, which allowed estimation of between-person effects.

Statistical models

Statistical analyses were performed using R software and R studio version 4.1.2 (R Core Team, 2021). Multilevel modeling was conducted with the lme4 (Bates, Mächler, Bolker, & Walker, Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015) and the lmerTest (Kuznetsova, Brockhoff, & Christensen, Reference Kuznetsova, Brockhoff and Christensen2017) packages, each model was fitted with restricted maximum likelihood estimation. The lm function in R was used to fit linear regression models. To estimate the median recovery time per individual, survival analyses were conducted with the R survival package (Therneau, Crowson, & Atkinson, Reference Therneau, Crowson and Atkinson2020), and we used the Weibull regression model to estimate the median survival time, indicative of the time till recovery (De Calheiros Velozo et al., Reference De Calheiros Velozo, Lafit, Viechtbauer, van Amelsvoort, Schruers, Marcelis and Vaessen2023) (details see the Supplementary Material).

We used multilevel modeling (MLM) to account for the hierarchical structure of the ESM data in studying the within-person associations between ER flexibility and NA dynamics, with daily NA variability, daily NA instability, daily NA inertia, and daily NA recovery as the outcome respectively, and daily ER flexibility as the primary predictor. Additionally, daily mean ER effort and daily mean NA were entered into the models as level 1 covariates because research indicated that dynamic measures could be significantly impacted by the average levels of the same variable (Dejonckheere et al., Reference Dejonckheere, Mestdagh, Houben, Rutten, Sels, Kuppens and Tuerlinckx2019). Level 1 predictors were person-mean centered before being included to models, thus removing between-person differences. Next, linear regression models were conducted to examine between-person associations between ER flexibility and NA dynamics, with person-level NA variability, NA instability, NA inertia and NA recovery as the outcome respectively, and person-level of ER flexibility as the primary predictor. Similarly, person-level ER effort and NA were entered as covariates. Age and sex were also included in all MLM and regression models as covariates.

The study and analyses plan were preregistered after data collection, at the open science framework (https://osf.io/xjsp7/). More details of statistical analyses and deviations from the registration were described in the Supplementary Material.

Results

Participant characteristics

A total of 119 participants (67 individuals at high clinical risk, 52 individuals with a first episode of psychosis) had data in the ESM and evening questionnaire and were included in the analysis (64 female, with a median age of 24). Among participants of the final sample, the average response rate to ESM beeps was 42%, and the average response rate to the evening questionnaire was 56% (see Table 1).

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of participants

Note. aThe severity of psychotic symptoms was measured with the 24-item Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS, Overall and GorhamReference Overall and Gorham1962); bThe number of beep responses was calculated based on responses to negative affect items; cThe number of responses to the evening questionnaire was calculated based on responses to emotion regulation items.

The mean, standard deviations, the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICCs), and the within-person and between-person correlation coefficients for the variables are presented in Table 2.

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and correlations among study variables

Note. ER, emotion regulation; NA, negative affect; s.d., standard deviation; ICC, intraclass correlation coefficient. * p < 0.05; ** p < 0 0.01; *** p < 0.001.

ER flexibility and emotion dynamics

Within-person associations

For daily variability in NA, no significant associations were found with daily ER flexibility, or daily mean ER effort. Higher levels of daily mean NA were related to greater variability in NA (Table 3), suggesting that diversity in ER strategy use did not account for any significant additional variance in variability in NA than the mean level of NA or mean ER effort during the day. Age was found to be associated with lower daily variability in NA, while being female was found to be associated with higher daily variability in NA.

Table 3. Results of multilevel models and linear regression models estimating associations between ER flexibility, emotional dynamic measures and affective recovery

Note. aIn the analsysis of NA recovery, mean NA was used as the indicator of baseline level of NA, and we have already controlled for age and gender in the survival regression. ER, emotion regulation; NA, negative affect; s.e., standard errors.

As predicted, lower daily instability in NA was associated with higher daily ER flexibility (Fig. 1). No significant associations between daily instability in NA and daily mean ER effort were found. Greater daily instability in NA was associated with higher levels of daily mean NA. The association between age or sex and daily instability in NA was not significant.

Figure 1. (a) Within-person associations between ER flexibility and instability in NA; (b) between-person associations between ER flexibility and instability in NA; (c) within-person associations between ER flexibility and NA recovery; (d) within-person associations between mean ER efforts and NA recovery.

For daily inertia in NA, no significant within-person associations were found for daily ER flexibility, or daily mean ER effort, or daily mean NA. The association between age or sex and daily inertia in NA was not significant.

For recovery speed in NA, results from the survival analyses showed that overall, after 103 min (ranging from 101 to 204 min, s.d. = 50.15) there was a 50% chance that the participant had not yet recovered, the effect of age or sex was not significant. Cumulative stress was associated with delayed recovery in NA, beta = 0.68, z = 3.44, p < 0.001. The MLM analysis showed that higher daily ER flexibility was associated with quicker NA recovery during the day, while greater daily mean ER effort was associated with slower NA recovery. Therefore, higher flexibility rather than greater efforts in ER strategy use are related to faster recovery of NA in everyday life in people with early psychosis.

Between-person associations

Greater variability in NA was associated with higher flexibility in ER strategy use in people with early psychosis, which indicated that individuals with early psychosis who reported greater use of different ER strategies, also demonstrated higher levels of variability in NA. No significant between-person association between NA variability and ER efforts was found. The greater NA variability in early psychosis were associated with higher levels of NA in general. The association between age or sex and NA variability was not significant.

Contrary to the results of within-person associations, greater instability in NA was associated with greater flexibility in ER strategy use. No significant association between ER efforts and instability in NA was found. In addition, as predicted, the greater instability in NA in early psychosis was associated higher levels of NA. The association between age or sex and NA instability was not significant.

No significant between-person associations were found for NA inertia, with ER flexibility, or ER efforts, or NA. The association between age or sex and NA inertia was not significant.

The linear regression analysis showed no significant between-person associations between NA recovery with ER flexibility or ER effort. Thus, recovery speed of NA in early psychosis did not differ by levels of variability or average efforts in ER strategy use.

Discussion

The current study examined the within and between-person associations between ER flexibility and dynamics in NA in early psychosis. The results showed different and even opposite patterns of associations. At the within-person level, higher flexibility in ER, but not more effort, was associated with more stable NA and a quicker recovery from stressors during the day. In contrast, at the between-person level, higher flexibility in ER strategy use related to higher variability and instability in NA in early psychosis. The results provide evidence for the effectiveness of ER strategy use by showing within-person links between ER flexibility and NA instability and recovery in the early stages of psychosis. The findings also highlight the necessity of considering ER flexibility in everyday life and incorporating various emotional dynamic dimensions in evaluating regulatory effectiveness in psychosis.

ER flexibility and emotion dynamics

The results showed that greater flexibility in ER strategies was associated with lower instability of NA during the day, while no such associations were found for variability or inertia of NA. Although measures of emotional variability, instability, and inertia are mathematically and conceptually related, they capture distinct facets of emotional dynamics, with instability capturing the frequent and extreme emotional changes from one moment to the next (Jahng, Wood, & Trull, Reference Jahng, Wood and Trull2008). These findings also corroborate the concern regarding the relevance of emotional inertia or variability for psychosis compared to emotional instability (Nowak, Krkovic, Kammerer, & Lincoln, Reference Nowak, Krkovic, Kammerer and Lincoln2022a), as people with psychosis and healthy volunteers were differentiated on levels of emotional instability only. In addition, previous ESM studies demonstrated that more pronounced instability in NA was associated with more frequent and severe paranoia in healthy participants (Nittel et al., Reference Nittel, Lamster, Rief, Kircher, Soll and Mehl2019; Nowak & Lincoln, Reference Nowak and Lincoln2021), while a longitudinal study found higher mood instability concurrently and prospectively predicted more severe psychotic experiences (Marwaha, Broome, Bebbington, Kuipers, & Freeman, Reference Marwaha, Broome, Bebbington, Kuipers and Freeman2014), further underscoring the relevance of NA instability in psychosis.

The negative within-person associations between ER flexibility and instability in NA are consistent with our hypothesis. When people in the early stages of psychosis flexibly adapt the use of different strategies to fit with the situational demands, they experience less extreme and less frequent changes in NA, suggesting regulatory effectiveness. Similarly, there is evidence supporting regulatory effectiveness in chronic psychosis, showing that the use of certain strategies was associated with subsequent reductions in NA and delusional intensity among patients with schizophrenia (Ludwig, Mehl, Krkovic, & Lincoln, Reference Ludwig, Mehl, Krkovic and Lincoln2020; Raugh, Bartolomeo, Zhang, James, & Strauss, Reference Raugh, Bartolomeo, Zhang, James and Strauss2023). This finding is also in line with studies in healthy populations, which show that high ER flexibility is related with a variety of desirable outcomes such as lower NA and less severe depressive symptoms (Blanke et al., Reference Blanke, Brose, Kalokerinos, Erbas, Riediger and Kuppens2019; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Blain, Meng, Liu and Qiu2021).

In support of our hypothesis, results showed that when people with early psychosis were relatively more flexible in managing their emotions elicited by the most negative event of that day, they recovered faster from stressors during the day, indicating that the more flexible ER strategy use fosters a swifter return of NA to baseline. Although previous research linked impaired recovery to early psychosis (Vaessen et al., Reference Vaessen, Viechtbauer, van der Steen, Gayer-Anderson, Kempton, Valmaggia and Myin-Germeys2019; Weintraub et al., Reference Weintraub, Weisman de Mamani, Villano, Evans, Millman, Hooley and Timpano2019), the results suggest that this association is moderated by level of regulatory flexibility. The finding that higher regulatory flexibility relates to faster affective recovery is consistent with assumptions of the flexibility model of ER, which emphasized the value of such flexibility and links it to positive adaption (Aldao et al., Reference Aldao, Sheppes and Gross2015). Likewise, a body of work has related other forms of flexibility to adaptive outcomes, for example, experimental studies showed that people with greater expressive flexibility showed a more rapid recovery from acute pain (Feldner et al., Reference Feldner, Hekmat, Zvolensky, Vowles, Secrist and Leen-Feldner2006) as well as a better adjustment at a 3-year follow-up among individuals experiencing high level of cumulative life stress (Westphal, Seivert, & Bonanno, Reference Westphal, Seivert and Bonanno2010). These findings are further supplemented by longitudinal studies showing that lower levels of cognitive and coping flexibility predicted more severe mental health problems during the COVID-19 pandemic (Chen & Bonanno, Reference Chen and Bonanno2021; Hemi et al., Reference Hemi, Sopp, Bonanno, Michael, McGiffin and Levy-Gigi2023).

When people with early psychosis engaged in greater regulatory efforts, it is associated with a slower recovery during the day. That is, greater endorsement of ER strategies, did not hasten but prolong the recovery process in early psychosis. This is not surprising because a study in a healthy population has suggested that greater ER effort is an indicator of regulatory failure, revealing positive associations between ER efforts and higher levels of NA (Blanke et al., Reference Blanke, Brose, Kalokerinos, Erbas, Riediger and Kuppens2019). Previous work in schizophrenia suggests that the ER effort made by people with psychosis, while adequate, but might not be contextually appropriate. For example, it has been shown that relative to controls, individuals with schizophrenia reported more frequent regulatory attempts (Raugh & Strauss, Reference Raugh and Strauss2022; Visser, Esfahlani, Sayama, & Strauss, Reference Visser, Esfahlani, Sayama and Strauss2018), such that they fail to select the most appropriate strategies that meet the situational demands (Raugh et al., Reference Raugh, Bartolomeo, Zhang, James and Strauss2023; Strauss et al., Reference Strauss, Zamani Esfahlani, Visser, Dickinson, Gruber and Sayama2019). Moreover, individuals with schizophrenia have been found to switch too much and terminate too late in the regulatory process (Bartolomeo, Raugh, & Strauss, Reference Bartolomeo, Raugh and Strauss2022). However, how people at the early stages of psychosis vary their ER efforts across the four stages of the emotion generation process and whether they differ from the healthy population warrants further investigation. Furthermore, the role of potential moderators, such as the severity of psychotic experience, in shaping ER efforts in early psychosis also requires clarification, because patients with schizophrenia have been found to report higher levels but less effective ER efforts during psychotic experiences (Kimhy et al., Reference Kimhy, Lister, Liu, Vakhrusheva, Delespaul, Malaspina and Wang2020; Strauss et al., Reference Strauss, Zamani Esfahlani, Visser, Dickinson, Gruber and Sayama2019). In addition, no between-person associations were found for ER flexibility and affective recovery, thus again, ER flexibility seems to have a unique role in influencing affective recovery in everyday life, at the within-person level. These results add evidence to the emerging literature examining the different facets of ER and support that effort and flexibility capture two related but distinct dimensions.

Comparing the within- and between-person effects

When examining between-person effects, we found that people with early psychosis who reported greater variability and instability in NA showed higher flexibility in ER strategy use generally. That is, people with early psychosis who experienced highly variable and unstable NA have a tendency to search for and apply different strategies in an attempt to regulate emotions, compared to those with lower NA fluctuations. The results are corroborated by a recent study, showing that the higher levels of NA in daily life were associated with a greater desire and more attempts to regulate emotions in the healthy population (Daros et al., Reference Daros, Daniel, Boukhechba, Chow, Barnes and Teachman2020). On the other hand, results of the within-person associations have shown that when an individual is able to flexibly apply different ERs, they seem to experience reduced daily fluctuation in NA around his/her mean. These findings support that the more diverse ER use in managing negative emotions is effective, in changing certain dynamics patterns of NA (i.e. instability and recovery) within the daily context in early psychosis.

It is noteworthy that when comparing within-person and between person effects, opposite relationships were found. For example, while higher levels of ER flexibility were positively related to instability in NA at the between-person level, negative associations were found at the within-person level. The reversed relationship, although contrary to our hypothesis, is not surprising and has been observed in other research areas (Snippe et al., Reference Snippe, Simons, Hartmann, Menne-Lothmann, Kramer, Booij and Wichers2016; Thorvaldsson et al., Reference Thorvaldsson, Skoog, Hofer, Borjesson-Hanson, Ostling, Sacuiu and Johansson2012). For example, although the between-person associations between exercise and heart rate are generally negative, within-person there is a positive association, heart rates increase during exercise (Arai et al., Reference Arai, Saul, Albrecht, Hartley, Lilly, Cohen and Colucci1989; Fletcher et al., Reference Fletcher, Balady, Blair, Blumenthal, Caspersen, Chaitman and Pollock1996). Therefore, these findings corroborate the view that drawing inferences about within-person effects based on between-person observations can be dangerous (Kievit, Frankenhuis, Waldorp, & Borsboom, Reference Kievit, Frankenhuis, Waldorp and Borsboom2013), highlighting the necessity of separating and disentangling effects that operate at different levels.

Strengths and limitations

This study has several notable methodological strengths, including different measures of emotional dynamics, adjusting for the confounding effect of mean levels of ER and emotional intensity, examining the associations at between-person level, as well as within-person level at a more micro timescale. Yet, there are also several limitations to be mentioned. First, we assessed ER strategy use at the end of the day, but NA over the entire day. The mismatch in timescales of measures of ER and affect means that this association is not temporally linked. Thus, it is possible that other factors than the way people with early psychosis respond to the most intense negative events may have influenced daily fluctuations and recovery in NA during the day. Future work might benefit by measuring ER strategy use within the same time window of NA to better account for their temporal and reciprocal relations (Blanke, Neubauer, Houben, Erbas, & Brose, Reference Blanke, Neubauer, Houben, Erbas and Brose2022). Furthermore, we operationalized ER flexibility as variety in ER strategy use in a given situation. As a result, we may not have captured the full complexity of ER flexibility. The effects of other components of ER flexibility, such as context sensitivity (Battaglini et al., Reference Battaglini, Rnic, Jameson, Jopling, Albert and LeMoult2022; Li et al., Reference Li, Lafit, van Aubel, Vaessen, Hiekkaranta, Houben and Myin-Germeys2023) in relation to psychosis requires further validation. In addition, our operationalization of recovery of NA relied on emotional responses to the first stressful event to avoid spill-over effects from previous stressors. Thus, the way how people response to other stressors later in the day was not examined. A major challenge for future studies is to account for this temporal complexity by further considering contextual demands of stressors. Also, we focused on dynamic patterns of NA only, temporal dynamic and the recovery process of positive affect might also be informative in understanding risk factors related to psychosis (Ader et al., Reference Ader, Schick, Simons, Delespaul, Myin-Germeys, Vaessen and Reininghaus2022). Finally, one noteworthy limitation of this study is the absence of a healthy control group, which precludes the examination of whether individuals with early psychosis are as flexible and effective as a healthy population comparison population in their regulatory process.

Conclusion

In order to understand whether and how ER flexibility is associated with emotional dynamics in early psychosis, we investigated the association of ER flexibility with three commonly used dynamics measures of NA and recovery speed in NA in people with early psychosis, from a within-person and between-person perspective. Results of the within-person effects demonstrated that when people with early psychosis are more flexible in regulating emotions, they experience less abrupt changes in NA and their NA recover faster from negative events. Results of the between-person effects showed that people with early psychosis who had more variable and unstable in NA are more diverse in ER strategy use. These results add empirical evidence to the theoretical reasoning regarding adaptiveness of ER flexibility and support effectiveness of such regulatory flexibility in reducing instability in NA and foster NA recovery in early psychosis. Such findings further confirm the importance of disaggregating within-person from between-person effects, which may provide a more nuanced understanding regarding the core affective correlates of ER flexibility in early psychosis.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724000151.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank all participants, all therapists involved in administering treatment, as well as research coordinators, research assistants, and data manager who were involved in the INTERACT trial.

Funding statement

This work has been supported by EU funding (ERC INTERACT grant, 309767) to IMG and, a grant from the Natural Science Foundation of China (31700957) to XL. XL was funded by a scholarship granted by the China Scholarship Council (CSC grant, 202006775035).

Competing interests

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Ader, L., Schick, A., Simons, C., Delespaul, P., Myin-Germeys, I., Vaessen, T., & Reininghaus, U. (2022). Positive affective recovery in daily life as a momentary mechanism across subclinical and clinical stages of mental disorder: Experience sampling study. JMIR Mental Health, 9(11), e37394. doi:10.2196/37394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 217237. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aldao, A., Sheppes, G., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation flexibility. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 39(3), 263278. doi:10.1007/s10608-014-9662-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arai, Y., Saul, J. P., Albrecht, P., Hartley, L. H., Lilly, L. S., Cohen, R. J., & Colucci, W. S. (1989). Modulation of cardiac autonomic activity during and immediately after exercise. American Journal of Physiology-Heart and Circulatory Physiology, 256(1), 132141. doi:10.1152/ajpheart.1989.256.1.H132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartolomeo, L. A., Raugh, I. M., & Strauss, G. P. (2022). Deconstructing emotion regulation in schizophrenia: The nature and consequences of abnormalities in monitoring dynamics. Schizophrenia Research, 240, 135142. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2021.12.043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 148. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Battaglini, A. M., Rnic, K., Jameson, T., Jopling, E., Albert, A. Y., & LeMoult, J. (2022). The association of emotion regulation flexibility and negative and positive affect in daily life. Affective Science, 3, 673685. doi:10.1007/s42761-022-00132-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanke, E., Brose, A., Kalokerinos, E., Erbas, Y., Riediger, M., & Kuppens, P. (2019). Mix it to fix it: Emotion regulation variability in daily life. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 20(20), 473485. doi:10.1037/emo0000566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanke, E. S., Neubauer, A. B., Houben, M., Erbas, Y., & Brose, A. (2022). Why do my thoughts feel so bad? Getting at the reciprocal effects of rumination and negative affect using dynamic structural equation modeling. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 22(8), 17731786. doi:10.1037/emo0000946.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonanno, G. A., & Burton, C. L. (2013). Regulatory flexibility: An individual differences perspective on coping and emotion regulation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(6), 591612. doi:10.1177/1745691613504116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, H. C., Visser, K. F., Mittal, V. A., Gibb, B. E., Coles, M. E., & Strauss, G. P. (2020). Emotion regulation across the psychosis continuum. Development and Psychopathology, 32(1), 219227. doi:10.1017/S0954579418001682.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, S., & Bonanno, G. A. (2021). Components of emotion regulation flexibility: Linking latent profiles to depressive and anxious symptoms. Clinical Psychological Science, 9(2), 236251. doi:10.1177/2167702620956972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daros, A. R., Daniel, K. E., Boukhechba, M., Chow, P. I., Barnes, L. E., & Teachman, B. A. (2020). Relationships between trait emotion dysregulation and emotional experiences in daily life: An experience sampling study. Cognition and Emotion, 34(4), 743755. doi:10.1080/02699931.2019.1681364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Calheiros Velozo, J., Lafit, G., Viechtbauer, W., van Amelsvoort, T., Schruers, K., Marcelis, M., … Vaessen, T. (2023). Delayed affective recovery to daily-life stressors signals a risk for depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 320, 499506. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2022.09.136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dejonckheere, E., Mestdagh, M., Houben, M., Rutten, I., Sels, L., Kuppens, P., & Tuerlinckx, F. (2019). Complex affect dynamics add limited information to the prediction of psychological well-being. Nature Human Behavior, 3(5), 478491. doi:10.1038/s41562-019-0555-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldner, M. T., Hekmat, H., Zvolensky, M. J., Vowles, K. E., Secrist, Z., & Leen-Feldner, E. W. (2006). The role of experiential avoidance in acute pain tolerance: A laboratory test. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 37(2), 146158. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2005.03.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, G. F., Balady, G., Blair, S. N., Blumenthal, J., Caspersen, C., Chaitman, B., … Pollock, M. L. (1996). Statement on exercise: Benefits and recommendations for physical activity programs for all Americans. A statement for health professionals by the Committee on Exercise and Cardiac Rehabilitation of the Council on Clinical Cardiology, American Heart Association. Circulation, 94(4), 857862. doi:10.1161/01.cir.94.4.857.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grégoire, S., Chénier, C., Doucerain, M., Lachance, L., & Shankland, R. (2020). Ecological momentary assessment of stress, well-being, and psychological flexibility among college and university students during acceptance and commitment therapy. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 52(3), 231243. doi:10.1037/cbs0000175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hemi, A., Sopp, R., Bonanno, G., Michael, T., McGiffin, J., & Levy-Gigi, E. (2023). Flexibility predicts chronic anxiety and depression during the first year of the COVID-19 pandemic – A longitudinal investigation of mental health trajectories. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice and Policy, Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/tra0001517.Google ScholarPubMed
Hermans, K. S. F. M., Myin-Germeys, I., Gayer-Anderson, C., Kempton, M. J., Valmaggia, L., McGuire, P., … Reininghaus, U. (2020). Elucidating negative symptoms in the daily life of individuals in the early stages of psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 51(15), 25992609. doi:10.1017/S0033291720001154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Houben, M., & Kuppens, P. (2020). Emotion dynamics and the association with depressive features and borderline personality disorder traits: Unique, specific, and prospective relationships. Clinical Psychological Science, 8(2), 226239. doi:10.1177/2167702619871962.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houben, M., Van Den Noortgate, W., & Kuppens, P. (2015). The relation between short-term emotion dynamics and psychological well-being: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 141(4), 901930. doi:10.1037/a0038822.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jahng, S., Wood, P. K., & Trull, T. J. (2008). Analysis of affective instability in ecological momentary assessment: Indices using successive difference and group comparison via multilevel modeling. Psychological Methods, 13(4), 354375. doi:10.1037/a0014173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kashdan, T. B., & Rottenberg, J. (2010). Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of health. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(7), 865878. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kievit, R. A., Frankenhuis, W. E., Waldorp, L. J., & Borsboom, D. (2013). Simpson's paradox in psychological science: A practical guide. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 513. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, M., Hwang, W. J., Park, J., Kim, T., Oh, S., & Kwon, J. S. (2021). Neurophysiological correlate of emotion regulation by cognitive reappraisal and its association with psychotic symptoms in early psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 47(1), 8796. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbaa080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimhy, D., Lister, A., Liu, Y., Vakhrusheva, J., Delespaul, P., Malaspina, D., … Wang, Y. (2020). The impact of emotion awareness and regulation on psychotic symptoms during daily functioning. NPJ Schizophrenia, 6(1), 7. doi:10.1038/s41537-020-0096-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krkovic, K., Clamor, A., Schlier, B., & Lincoln, T. M. (2020). Emotions and persecutory ideation in daily life: On the trail of the “chicken and egg” problem. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 129(2), 215223. doi:10.1037/abn0000495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuppens, P., & Verduyn, P. (2015). Looking at emotion regulation through the window of emotion dynamics. Psychological Inquiry, 26, 7279. doi:10.1080/1047840X.2015.960505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). LmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 126. doi:10.18637/jss.v082.i13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, X., Lafit, G., van Aubel, E., Vaessen, T., Hiekkaranta, A. P., Houben, M., … Myin-Germeys, I. (2023). Emotion regulation in daily life in early psychosis: The role of contextual appraisals. Schizophrenia Research, 261, 130138. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2023.09.023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lincoln, T., Schulze, L., & Renneberg, B. (2022). The role of emotion regulation in the characterization, development and treatment of psychopathology. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1, 272286. doi:10.1038/s44159-022-00040-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lincoln, T. M., Sundag, J., Schlier, B., & Karow, A. (2018). The relevance of emotion regulation in explaining why social exclusion triggers paranoia in individuals at clinical high risk of psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 44(4), 757767. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbx135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, J., Subramaniam, M., Chong, S., & Mahendran, R. (2020). Maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies and positive symptoms in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: The mediating role of global emotion dysregulation. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 27(6), 826836. doi:10.1002/cpp.2466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ludwig, L., Mehl, S., Krkovic, K., & Lincoln, T. M. (2020). Effectiveness of emotion regulation in daily life in individuals with psychosis and nonclinical controls—An experience-sampling study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 129(4), 408421. doi:10.1037/abn0000505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maciejewski, D. F., van Roekel, E., Ha, T., De France, K., Lin, L., Lennarz, H. K., … Verhagen, M. (2023). Beyond main effects? Affect level as a moderator in the relation between affect dynamics and depressive symptoms. Journal of Emotion and Psychopathology, 1(1), 356372. doi:10.31234/osf.io/42uzq.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marwaha, S., Broome, M. R., Bebbington, P. E., Kuipers, E., & Freeman, D. (2014). Mood instability and psychosis: Analyses of British national survey data. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40(2), 269277. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbt149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, J. W., & Young, M. A. (2018). Dynamic processes in emotion regulation choice. Cognition and Emotion, 32(8), 16541662. doi:10.1080/02699931.2017.1419935.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myin-Germeys, I., Birchwood, M., & Kwapil, T. (2011). From environment to therapy in psychosis: A real-world momentary assessment approach. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 37, 244247. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbq16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myin-Germeys, I., Delespaul, P. A., & deVries, M. W. (2000). Schizophrenia patients are more emotionally active than is assumed based on their behavior. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26(4), 847854. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myin-Germeys, I., Kasanova, Z., Vaessen, T., Vachon, H., Kirtley, O., Viechtbauer, W., & Reininghaus, U. (2018). Experience sampling methodology in mental health research: New insights and technical developments. World Psychiatry, 17(2), 123132. doi:10.1002/wps.20513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myin-Germeys, I., & Kuppens, P. (2021). The open handbook of experience sampling methodology: A step-by-step guide to designing, conducting, and analyzing ESM studies (2nd ed.). Leuven: Center for Research on Experience Sampling and Ambulatory Methods, Leuven.Google Scholar
Myin-Germeys, I., van Aubel, E., Vaessen, T., Steinhart, H., Klippel, A., Lafit, G., … Reininghaus, U. (2022). Efficacy of acceptance and commitment therapy in daily life in early psychosis: Results from the multi-center INTERACT randomized controlled trial. Psychotherapy Psychosomatics, 91(6), 411423. doi:10.1159/00052227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nittel, C., Lamster, F., Rief, W., Kircher, T., Soll, D., & Mehl, S. (2019). Emotional instability and expressive suppression are related to paranoia in daily life: An electronic mobile assessment study in nonclinical individuals. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 10(3), 204380871986811. doi:10.1177/2043808719868119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowak, U., Krkovic, K., Kammerer, M. K., & Lincoln, T. M. (2022a). Pinpointing affective disturbances in psychosis: A comparison of temporal affect dynamics in individuals with psychotic disorders, individuals with attenuated psychotic symptoms, and clinical and healthy controls. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 153, 260268. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2022.06.047.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowak, U., & Lincoln, T. (2021). An experience-sampling study on the relevance of affect dynamics to paranoid ideation. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 23(1), 111123. doi:10.1037/emo0000807.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowak, U., Wood, J., Dinu, A., Wittkamp, M., Clamor, A., Oravecz, Z., & Lincoln, T. (2022b). Are paranoid ideation and hallucination spectrum experiences differently associated with affect dynamics? A continuous-time modeling approach. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 23(5), 12941305. doi:10.1037/emo0001150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oorschot, M., Lataster, T., Thewissen, V., Lardinois, M., Wichers, M., van Os, J, &… Myin-Germeys, I. (2013). Emotional experience in negative symptoms of schizophrenia—no evidence for a generalized hedonic deficit. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39(1), 217225. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbr137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Overall, J. E., & Gorham, D. R. (1962). The brief psychiatric rating scale. Psychological reports, 10(3), 799812.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raugh, I. M., Bartolomeo, L. A., Zhang, L., James, S. H., & Strauss, G. P. (2023). Deconstructing emotion regulation in schizophrenia: The nature of abnormalities at the selection and implementation stages. Journal of Psychopathology and Clinical Science, 132(7), 908920. doi:10.1037/abn0000852.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raugh, I. M., & Strauss, G. P. (2022). Deconstructing emotion regulation in schizophrenia: The nature and consequences of abnormalities at the identification stage. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 272(6), 10611071. doi:10.1007/s00406-021-01350-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Reininghaus, U., Klippel, A., Steinhart, H., Vaessen, T., van Nierop, M., Viechtbauer, W., … Myin-Germeys, I. (2019). Efficacy of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy in Daily Life (ACT-DL) in early psychosis: Study protocol for a multi-centre randomized controlled trial. Trials, 20(1), 769. doi:10.1186/s13063-019-3912-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, S. P., Cooper, J. E., Fisher, H. L., Tarrant, C. J., Lloyd, T., Banjo, J., … Jones, P. (2005). Determining the chronology and components of psychosis onset: The Nottingham Onset Schedule (NOS). Schizophrenia Research, 80(1), 117130. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2005.04.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snippe, E., Simons, C. J., Hartmann, J. A., Menne-Lothmann, C., Kramer, I., Booij, S., … Wichers, M. (2016). Change in daily life behaviors and depression: Within-person and between-person associations. Health Psychology, 35(5), 433441. doi:10.1037/hea0000312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, G. P., Kappenman, E. S., Culbreth, A. J., Catalano, L. T., Lee, B. G., & Gold, J. M. (2013). Emotion regulation abnormalities in schizophrenia: Cognitive change strategies fail to decrease the neural response to unpleasant stimuli. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39(4), 872883. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbs186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, G. P., Zamani Esfahlani, F., Visser, K. F., Dickinson, E. K., Gruber, J., & Sayama, H. (2019). Mathematically modeling emotion regulation abnormalities during psychotic experiences in schizophrenia. Clinical Psychological Science, 7(2), 216233. doi:doi:10.1177/2167702618810233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Therneau, T., Crowson, C., & Atkinson, E. (2020). Multi-state models and competing risks. CRAN-R. Retrieved from .Google Scholar
Thorvaldsson, V., Skoog, I., Hofer, S. M., Borjesson-Hanson, A., Ostling, S., Sacuiu, S., & Johansson, B. (2012). Nonlinear blood pressure effects on cognition in old age: Separating between-person and within-person associations. Psychology and Aging, 27(2), 375383. doi:10.1037/a0025631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaessen, T., Viechtbauer, W., van der Steen, Y., Gayer-Anderson, C., Kempton, M. J., Valmaggia, L., … Myin-Germeys, I. (2019). Recovery from daily-life stressors in early and chronic psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 213, 3239. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2019.03.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Visser, K. F., Esfahlani, F. Z., Sayama, H., & Strauss, G. P. (2018). An ecological momentary assessment evaluation of emotion regulation abnormalities in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 48(14), 23372345. doi:10.1017/S0033291717003865.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, X., Blain, S. D., Meng, J., Liu, Y., & Qiu, J. (2021). Variability in emotion regulation strategy use is negatively associated with depressive symptoms. Cognition and Emotion, 35(2), 324340. doi:10.1080/02699931.2020.1840337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weermeijer, J., Lafit, G., Kiekens, G., Wampers, M., Eisele, G., Kasanova, Z., … Myin-Germeys, I. (2022). Applying multiverse analysis to experience sampling data: Investigating whether preprocessing choices affect robustness of conclusions. Behavior Research Methods, 54(6), 29812992. doi:10.3758/s13428-021-01777-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weintraub, M. J., Weisman de Mamani, A., Villano, W. J., Evans, T. C., Millman, Z. B., Hooley, J. M., & Timpano, K. R. (2019). Affective and physiological reactivity to emotional comments in individuals at elevated risk for psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 206, 428435. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2018.10.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wenzel, M., Blanke, E. S., Rowland, Z., & Kubiak, T. (2021). Emotion regulation dynamics in daily life: Adaptive strategy use may be variable without being unstable and predictable without being autoregressive. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 22(7), 14871504. doi:10.1037/emo0000967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westphal, M., Seivert, N. H., & Bonanno, G. A. (2010). Expressive flexibility. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 10(1), 92100. doi:10.1037/a0018420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yung, A. R., Yuen, H. P., McGorry, P. D., Phillips, L. J., Kelly, D., Dell'Olio, M., … Buckby, J. (2005). Mapping the onset of psychosis: The comprehensive assessment of at-risk mental states. The Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 39(11-12), 964971. doi:10.1080/j.1440-1614.2005.01714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of participants

Figure 1

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and correlations among study variables

Figure 2

Table 3. Results of multilevel models and linear regression models estimating associations between ER flexibility, emotional dynamic measures and affective recovery

Figure 3

Figure 1. (a) Within-person associations between ER flexibility and instability in NA; (b) between-person associations between ER flexibility and instability in NA; (c) within-person associations between ER flexibility and NA recovery; (d) within-person associations between mean ER efforts and NA recovery.

Supplementary material: File

Li et al. supplementary material

Li et al. supplementary material
Download Li et al. supplementary material(File)
File 17.4 KB