Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T03:52:13.846Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Filling the preparatory interval with temporal information or visual noise: crossover effect in schizophrenics and controls

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

Ulrike Borst
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Federal Republic of Germany
Rudolf Cohen*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Federal Republic of Germany
*
1Address for correspondence: Professor Dr R. Cohen, Fachgruppe Psychologie, Universitaet Konstanz, Postfach 5560, 7750 Konstanz, Federal Republic of Germany.

Synopsis

Three variants of a simple reaction time task with regular and irregular series of different preparatory intervals (Pls) were presented to schizophrenics and a control group of alcoholics and normals: the visual warning signal (WS) either (1) remained fixed, (2) shrank at a constant rate, or (3) changed in size erratically. Time information provided by shrinking WSs reduced the average reaction time in all groups and attenuated the crossover effect (COE) of schizophrenics, although even here more schizophrenics than controls had COE-scores ≥ 25 msec. Erratically changing WSs introduced the COE in controls. Within groups the COE was not related to general response speed, age, duration of hospitalization, or medication. Results are interpreted as indicating that schizophrenics eschew controlled anticipatory processes on the basis of WS- and PI-information which are crucial for motor preparation when PIs are too long to allow an automatic perceptual grouping of warning and imperative stimulus. This may also occur in controls when time estimation is impeded by distraction.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1989

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association (1980). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed.). APA: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Bellissimo, A. & Steffy, R. A. (1972). Redundancy-associated deficit in schizophrenic reaction time performance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 80, 299299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borst, U. & Cohen, R. (1987) Impact of time estimation on the crossover effect in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 22, 331331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J. P. & Raulin, M. L. (1976). Scales for physical and social anhedonia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 85, 374374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J. P. & Raulin, M. L (1978). Body-image aberration in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 87. 399407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cromwell, R. L. (1975). Assessment of schizophrenia. Annual Review of Psychology 26, 593593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Amicis, L. A. & Cromwell, R. L. (1979). Reaction time crossover in process schizophrenic patients, their relatives, and control subjects. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 167, 593593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elbert, T.Ulrich, R., Rockstroh, B., Lutzenberger, W & Cavicchioli, A. (1989). The processing of temporal intervals as reflected by CNV-like potentials. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Epstein, S. (1967). Toward a unified theory of anxiety. In Progress in Experimental Personality Research (ed. Maher, B.), (pp. 189). Academic Press: New York.Google Scholar
Fraisse, P. (1984). Perception and estimation of time. Annual Review of Psychology 35, 11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geisser, S. & Greenhouse, S. W. (1958). An extension of Box's results on the use of F distribution in multivariate analysis. Annals of Mathematical Statistics 29, 885885.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, C L. (1943). Principles of Behaviour. Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York.Google Scholar
Kaplan, R. D. (1974). The crossover phenomenon: three studies of the effect of training and information on process schizophrenic reaction time. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Waterloo: Ontario.Google Scholar
King, H. A. (1954). Psychomotor Aspects of Mental Illness. Harvard University Press: Cambridge, Mass.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mo, S. S. & Kersey, R. (1980). Foreperiod effect on time estimation and simple reaction time in schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical Psychology 36, 9494.3.0.CO;2-0>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niemi, P. & Näätänen, R. (1981) Foreperiod and simple reaction time. Psychological Bulletin 89, 133133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nuechterlein, K. H. (1977). Reaction time and attention in schizophrenia: a critical evaluation of the data and theories. Schizophrenia Bulletin 3, 373373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Overall, J. E & Gorham, D R. (1976). 047 BPRS. Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. In ECDEV Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology (DHEW Publication No. ADM 76–338) (ed. Guy, W), pp. 157169. National Institute of Mental Health. Rockville, MdGoogle Scholar
Pöppel, E. (1988). Mindworks: Time and Conscious Experience. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich: Boston.Google Scholar
Rist, F & Cohen, R (in the press). Sequential effects in the reaction times of schizophrenics: Crossover and modality shift effects. In Handbook of Schizophrenia: Vol 2 Neuropsychology, Psychophysiology and Information Processing (ed. Nasrallah, H. A.). Elsevier: Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Rodnick, E & Shakow, D. (1940). Set in the schizophrenic as measured by a composite reaction time index. American Journal of Psychiatry 97, 214225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shakow, D. (1963). Psychological deficit in schizophrenia. Behavioural Sciences 8, 275275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simons, R. F., MacMillan, F. W III, & Ireland, F. B. (1982). Reaction-time crossover in preselected schizotypic subjects Journal of Abnormal Psychology 91, 414414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spring, B. & Zubin, J. (1977). Reaction time and attention in schizophrenia: a comment on K. H Nuechterlein's critical evaluation of the data and theories. Schizophrenia Bulletin 3, 437437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steffy, R. A. & Galbraith, K. J (1974). A comparison of segmental set and inhibitory deficit explanations of the crossover pattern in process schizophrenic reaction time. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 83, 227227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steffy, R A. & Galbraith, K. (1975) Time-linked impairment in schizophrenic reaction time performance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 84, 315324CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tizard, J. & Venables, P. H. (1957). The influence of extraneous stimulation on the reaction time of schizophrenics. British Journal of Psychology 48, 299299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed