Suicide rates in the military have steadily increased in the past decade, with most recent estimates suggesting a rate of approximately 25 per 100 000 active-duty military personnel per year (Department of Defense, 2022). Though sparse, existing research suggests aggression may be a strong predictor of suicide attempts among military personnel (Schafer et al., Reference Schafer, Duffy, Kennedy, Stentz, Leon, Herrerias and Joiner2022). Unfortunately, aggression is also common among military personnel: approximately 30% of veterans report engaging in physical aggression (e.g. behaviors intended to cause physical harm) in a given month and 10% report engaging in severe violence (e.g. use of a weapon against another) in a given year (MacManus et al., Reference MacManus, Rona, Dickson, Somaini, Fear and Wessely2015). To date, driving factors underlying the association between service member aggression and suicide remain largely unknown. Using rich, longitudinal data from the Army Study to Assess Risk and Resilience in Service Members (STARRS) Pre/Post Deployment Study (PPDS), the current study examines whether aggression predicts suicide attempts among Army soldiers and explores potential mediators of this association.
Aggression as a suicide risk factor
Substantial research highlights aggression – defined as behaviors intended to harm another person who is motivated to avoid harm (Allen & Anderson, Reference Allen, Anderson and Sturmey2017) – as a salient risk factor for suicide among civilians (Franklin et al., Reference Franklin, Ribeiro, Fox, Bentley, Kleiman, Huang and Nock2017; Schafer et al., Reference Schafer, Duffy, Kennedy, Stentz, Leon, Herrerias and Joiner2022). Longitudinal studies, for example, find that aggression tends to temporally precede suicide attempts and is associated with greater medical severity of attempts (Gvion, Reference Gvion2018; Oquendo et al., Reference Oquendo, Galfalvy, Choo, Kandlur, Burke, Sublette and Stanley2021). Comparable research among service members and veterans is sparse. Cross-sectional studies suggest aggression is correlated with suicidal ideation (McGlade et al., Reference McGlade, Bueler, DiMuzio, Sheth, Legarreta and Yurgelun-Todd2021) and seems to co-occur with suicide attempts (Elbogen et al., Reference Elbogen, Wagner, Kimbrel, Brancu, Naylor, Graziano and Crawford2018). Among veterans in a posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) treatment program, for example, endorsement of both aggression and past suicide attempts was more common than endorsement of only past suicide attempts (Watkins, Sippel, Pietrzak, Hoff, & Harpaz-Rotem, Reference Watkins, Sippel, Pietrzak, Hoff and Harpaz-Rotem2017). Similarly, in active-duty samples, service members reporting any aggression were more likely to endorse suicidal ideation than service members reporting no aggression (Start, Allard, Adler, & Toblin, Reference Start, Allard, Adler and Toblin2019).
In a recent meta-analytic review of suicide risk factors in military personnel, Schafer et al. (Reference Schafer, Duffy, Kennedy, Stentz, Leon, Herrerias and Joiner2022) identified just five longitudinal studies of aggression and suicide among service members and no longitudinal studies of this association among veterans. Nonetheless, pooled estimates from these five studies identified aggression as the strongest predictor of suicide death among service members (Schafer et al., Reference Schafer, Duffy, Kennedy, Stentz, Leon, Herrerias and Joiner2022). Relatedly, results from the STARRS Longitudinal Study suggest a history of physical assault against others and a childhood history of bullying or threatening each independently predict post-discharge suicide attempts in service members even after controlling for dozens of other potential predictors (Kearns et al., Reference Kearns, Edwards, Finley, Geraci, Gildea, Goodman and Kessler2023). The relative dearth of research on the link between aggression and suicide stands in stark contrast to evidence suggesting generally elevated rates of aggression among military personnel.
Mechanisms of the aggression – suicide association
In addition to the scarcity of research linking aggression to suicide among military personnel, few, if any, studies have identified mechanisms of this association. Many theories of suicide provide potential explanations for how aggression increases suicide risk. For instance, the Interpersonal Theory of Suicide (IPT; Van Orden et al., Reference Van Orden, Witte, Cukrowicz, Braithwaite, Selby and Joiner2010) and Three-Step Theory of Suicide (3ST; Klonsky & May, Reference Klonsky and May2015) suggest that greater numbers of interpersonal relationships may buffer against suicide risk, whereas social isolation contributes to the development of suicide attempts (Calati et al., Reference Calati, Ferrari, Brittner, Oasi, Olié, Carvalho and Courtet2019; Van Orden et al., Reference Van Orden, Witte, Cukrowicz, Braithwaite, Selby and Joiner2010). Aggressive behaviors tend to strain important relationships; correspondingly, aggression is consistently associated with greater social isolation and interpersonal conflict (Hammett, Lavner, Karney, & Bradbury, Reference Hammett, Lavner, Karney and Bradbury2021; Rynar & Coccaro, Reference Rynar and Coccaro2018). Aggression researchers speculate that these relational consequences of aggression may help explain the association between aggression and suicide (Conner, Duberstein, Conwell, & Caine, Reference Conner, Duberstein, Conwell and Caine2003; Hartley, Pettit, & Castellanos, Reference Hartley, Pettit and Castellanos2018). Nevertheless, the potential for social isolation to serve this mediating role has not been empirically tested.
IPT and 3ST also note the importance of capability to attempt suicide – factors that decrease barriers to engaging in suicide-related behaviors (Klonsky & May, Reference Klonsky and May2015; Van Orden et al., Reference Van Orden, Witte, Cukrowicz, Braithwaite, Selby and Joiner2010). Within this framework, aggression may be conceptualized as contributing to capability for suicide. Indeed, aggression is positively associated with firearm ownership (a form of practical capability) in both veteran (Heinz, Cohen, Holleran, Alvarez, & Bonn-Miller, Reference Heinz, Cohen, Holleran, Alvarez and Bonn-Miller2016) and civilian samples (Clare et al., Reference Clare, Velasquez, Mujica Martorell, Fernandez, Dinh and Montague2021; Sanchez, Jaguan, Shaikh, McKenney, & Elkbuli, Reference Sanchez, Jaguan, Shaikh, McKenney and Elkbuli2020). Firearms represent a particularly lethal method of suicide: the estimated fatality rate of attempts made with firearms is around 90% compared to around 50% for attempts by hanging, 28% for attempts by jumping, and 2% for attempts by drug overdose (Conner, Azrael, & Miller, Reference Conner, Azrael and Miller2019). In addition, military personnel are more likely to use firearms in a suicide attempt compared to civilians (Gromatsky et al., Reference Gromatsky, Edwards, Sullivan, van Lissa, Lane, Spears and Goodman2022); almost 70% of service members who make a suicide attempt do so with a firearm (Department of Defense, 2022). It may be that aggression is associated with higher rates of firearm ownership, which in turn increases risk for suicide attempts.
Lastly, problematic alcohol use may mediate the association between aggression and suicide attempts. Alcohol use represents a consistent risk factor for suicide attempts among both civilians (Poorolajal, Haghtalab, Farhadi, & Darvishi, Reference Poorolajal, Haghtalab, Farhadi and Darvishi2016) and military personnel (Bohnert, Ilgen, Louzon, McCarthy, & Katz, Reference Bohnert, Ilgen, Louzon, McCarthy and Katz2017). Alcohol use is also associated with aggression, with some evidence suggesting problematic aggression often precedes alcohol use (Coccaro et al., Reference Coccaro, Fridberg, Fanning, Grant, King and Lee2016). Theoretical models of suicide suggest that alcohol use may mediate the association between aggression and suicide such that problematic aggression gives rise to chronic or severe alcohol use which, in turn, increases risk for suicide attempts (Conner et al., Reference Conner, Duberstein, Conwell and Caine2003; Conner & Ilgen, Reference Conner, Ilgen, O'Connor, Platt and Gordon2011). Again, however, this claim has not been empirically tested.
Current study
The current study extends our understanding of aggression as a longitudinal risk factor for suicide attempts among military personnel. First, we aimed to replicate the previously observed longitudinal association between aggression and suicide attempts, thereby adding to the small but growing body of research suggesting aggression is associated with later suicide attempts. Second, we tested whether social isolation (defined by the size of one's social network), firearm ownership, and/or problematic alcohol use mediate the relation between aggression and suicide attempts. We examined these associations using the Army STARRS PPDS, a large dataset of deployed Army soldiers spanning four time points: pre-deployment (T0), 1-month post-deployment (T1), 2–3 months post-deployment (T2), and 9–12 months post-deployment (T3). The Army STARRS PPDS data are ideally suited to address the current research question as Army STARRS studies were specifically designed to examine risk and resilience factors in suicide behaviors of military personnel over time (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Colpe, Fullerton, Gebler, Naifeh, Nock and Heeringa2013a). We hypothesized that (1) aggression at T0 would predict suicide attempts at T3, while controlling for lifetime suicide attempts at T0, and that (2) social network size, firearm ownership, and problematic alcohol use at T2 would partially mediate the association between T0 aggression and T3 suicide attempts. We tested these hypotheses while controlling for history of major depressive disorder (MDD), history of PTSD, history of traumatic brain injury (TBI), highest level of education, and trauma exposure on deployment.
Methods
Participants
Soldiers from three Brigade Combat Teams were recruited for the PPDS approximately 1 month prior to a combat deployment to Afghanistan, with data collection occurring from 2012 to 2014. To be included in the current study, participants must have provided complete data on aggression and lifetime suicide attempts at the T0 survey, resulting in a sample of 8483 soldiers. The majority of soldiers identified as men (93.6%) with an average age of 26.06 years (s.d. = 6.09) at T0. Most soldiers identified as White (66.1%), followed by Hispanic/Latino (15.8%), Black/African American (12.8%), Asian (4.4%), Native American/Alaskan Native (3.1%), Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander (2.2%), and ‘Other’ (9.4%); participants could have indicated more than one racial or ethnic identity. Approximately 7% of soldiers reported earning a GED or equivalent, 41.2% a high school diploma, 25.6% some post-high school education, 3.1% a technical school certificate or degree, 7.6% an associate degree, 10.2% a bachelor's degree, and 2.4% some graduate or professional education.
Procedures
Soldiers were recruited, provided informed consent, and completed the T0 survey in a group setting approximately 1 month prior to deployment. T1 surveys were completed within 1 month of returning from deployment, T2 surveys completed 2–3 months post-deployment, and T3 surveys completed 9–12 months post-deployment. Further details about survey administration can be found elsewhere (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Colpe, Fullerton, Gebler, Naifeh, Nock and Heeringa2013a). Secondary data analysis for the current study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the first author's institution.
Measures
Aggression
Soldiers reported their aggression at T0 using four items derived from the Joint Mental Health Advisory Team 7 (Office of the Surgeon General, United States Army Medical Command et al., 2011). Participants indicated how frequently they had engaged in each item on a five-point scale ranging from 0 (never) to 4 (very often); no reference period was indicated. Items included: (1) yell, insult, swear, or threaten someone; (2) have a heated argument with someone; (3) get into a loud argument in public; (4) have a physical confrontation during an argument. Per prior research with this measure (Krauss et al., Reference Krauss, Edwards, Ruiz, Epshteyn, Coolidge and Goodman2023), responses were summed with higher scores indicating greater engagement in aggressive behaviors; scores ranged from 0 to 16. Internal consistency in the current sample was adequate (α = 0.80).
Social networks
At T0 and T2, soldiers indicated the extent of their social network across four items developed by the Army STARRS research team. Participants were asked ‘How many people do you have in your personal life of the following sorts?’ with items including: (1) people you do things with, like watch TV together, go out for a drink or movie together, or play cards; (2) people who you feel really close to; (3) people who really care for you and would be there if you needed them; (4) family or friends who need you and rely on you for help when they need it. Participants responded on a 10-point scale ranging from 0 (0 people) to 9 (31 or more people). Items were averaged with higher scores representing a larger social network; scores ranged from 0 to 9. Internal consistency was α = 0.85 at T0 and α = 0.89 at T2.
Alcohol use
Participants reported on their frequency of alcohol use at T0 and T2 using the self-administered Composite International Diagnostic Interview Screening Scales (CIDI-SC; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Santiago, Colpe, Dempsey, First, Heeringa and Ursano2013b). Soldiers were asked how often during the past 30 days they had five or more drinks of alcohol on the same day. Since frequency of alcohol use was measured with a single item, internal consistency could not be calculated.
Firearm ownership
Soldiers reported their firearm ownership at T0 and T2 using a single question: ‘How many guns in working condition do you have in your home (house, apartment, barracks) including handguns, rifles, and shotguns?’ Response options ranged from 0 (0 guns) to 6 (11 or more guns). This item is designed to assess respondents’ ownership of personal firearms rather than access to firearms available through their military service.
Suicide attempts
At T0, T2, and T3, soldiers reported on suicide attempts using a single item adapted from the Columbia Suicide Severity Rating Scale (Posner et al., Reference Posner, Brown, Stanley, Brent, Yershova, Oquendo and Mann2011). At T0, soldiers reported on whether they had ever made a suicide attempt in their lifetime. T0 suicide attempts were coded with 1 representing one or more suicide attempts in their lifetime and 0 representing no lifetime history of suicide attempts. At T2, soldiers indicated whether they had made a suicide attempt since returning from deployment. At T3, soldiers who had completed T2 reported on suicide attempts since their T2 survey, and soldiers who did not complete T2 reported on suicide attempts since returning from deployment. This ensured that soldiers who completed T3, but not T2, had a report of suicide attempts across the same time period (since returning from deployment) as soldiers who completed both T2 and T3. Consistent with previous research using this measure (Chu et al., Reference Chu, Zuromski, Bernecker, Gutierrez, Joiner, Liu and Nock2020), we combined T2 and T3 reports; the composite score was coded such that 1 represents endorsement of a suicide attempt since returning from deployment, and 0 represents no endorsement (we refer to this score as suicide attempts at T3).
Control variables
At T0, soldiers reported on their history of MDD using the CIDI-SC (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Santiago, Colpe, Dempsey, First, Heeringa and Ursano2013b) and reported on their history of PTSD using a screening version of the PTSD Checklist (Wilkins, Lang, & Norman, Reference Wilkins, Lang and Norman2011). Diagnoses were coded such that 1 represents a history of the diagnosis and 0 represents no history. Soldiers also completed seven items assessing whether they had ever experienced a head injury involving loss of consciousness or a lapse in memory. History of TBI was scored such that 1 represents the presence of TBI of at least mild severity (defined as a head injury resulting in loss of consciousness for less than 30 minutes or lapse in memory of less than 24 hours, per the American Congress for Rehabilitation Medicine; Silverberg et al., Reference Silverberg, Iverson, Cogan, Dams-O-Connor, Delmonico, Graf and Zemek2023), and 0 represents no history of TBI.
Since deployment represents a methodological control in the current study, we included trauma exposure while on deployment as a control variable. At T1, soldiers reported on their exposure to potentially traumatic events during deployment using the Army STARRS Combat Experiences Scale (CES; Sherman, Frye-Cox, & Lucier-Greer, Reference Sherman, Frye-Cox and Lucier-Greer2023), adapted from the Deployment Risk and Resilience Inventory (Vogt et al., Reference Vogt, Smith, King, King, Knight and Vasterling2013). The CES includes 12 items rated on a 5-point scale from 0 (never) to 4 (10 + times). Items assessed a variety of potentially traumatic experiences often seen in combat deployments, such as getting wounded, having a ‘close call,’ and having responsibility for the death of another. Items were averaged such that higher scores represented greater exposure on deployment; observed scores ranged from 0 to 3.9. Internal consistency in the current sample was α = 0.74. Previous research suggests good convergent validity of the CES, with CES total scores correlating positively with depressive and anxiety symptoms and negatively with coping ability (Sherman et al., Reference Sherman, Frye-Cox and Lucier-Greer2023).
Missing data analyses and data analytic plan
Missing data ranged from 0.72% (firearm ownership at T0) to 26% (social networks at T2). Table 1 presents demographic characteristics of participants with and without complete data. We imputed missing data values across all variables at all time points using missForest (Stekhoven & Bühlmann, Reference Stekhoven and Bühlmann2012), an imputation method appropriate for non-parametric, mixed type (categorical or continuous) data. MissForest outperforms other imputation methods, such as multiple imputation by chained equations, when data are non-parametric and mixed-type (Stekhoven & Bühlmann, Reference Stekhoven and Bühlmann2012). In addition to all main study variables, we also included age, gender, race, and ethnicity in the imputation model as these characteristics were related to missingness of study variables. We imputed reports of suicide attempts at T2 and T3 separately and then calculated the composite suicide attempts score after imputation. The random forest algorithm also allows for estimation of out-of-bag (OOB) error rates, providing an estimate of the convergence of imputation (OOB values close to 0 suggest excellent convergence while OOB values close to 1 suggest poor convergence). The OOB estimate was 0.25 for continuous variables and 0.17 for categorical variables, reflecting good convergence.
a Complete data were defined as full data on aggression at T0, lifetime suicide attempts at T0, all mediators at T2, age, gender, ethnicity, race, and level of education.
b Rows within the Incomplete data column may not add to 3134 as some participants had missing data on demographic characteristics.
After imputing missing data, we used path analyses through the R package lavaan (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012) to examine our mediation hypotheses; Fig. 1 presents the hypothesized mediation model. To test hypothesis 1 (aggression at T0 would predict suicide attempts at T3), we entered aggression at T0, lifetime suicide attempts at T0, and the control variables as predictors and suicide attempts at T3 as the outcome. To test hypothesis 2 (social network size, firearm ownership, and frequency of alcohol use would mediate the association between aggression and suicide attempts), we entered each ‘a’ path (aggression at T0 predicting each mediator at T2) while controlling for each mediator at T0; this allowed us to examine the association between aggression at T0 and changes in mediators from T0 to T2. We then entered each ‘b’ path (mediators at T2 predicting suicide attempts at T3) and the ‘c’ path (aggression at T0 predicting suicide attempts at T3). We again controlled for the effect of lifetime suicide attempts at T0 and our control variables on suicide attempts at T3. We calculated indirect effects using the product of coefficient approach where we computed the product of each ‘a’ and ‘b’ path (Hayes, Reference Hayes2022). Across all models, we used diagonally weighted least squares estimators with robust standard errors; this estimator is appropriate for use with categorical or skewed outcome variables as it makes no assumptions about the underlying distribution of variables (Li, Reference Li2016).
Results
Descriptive statistics
Table 2 presents descriptive statistics and correlations between main study variables. Overall prevalence of lifetime suicide attempts at T0 (2.0%, n = 165) and suicide attempts at T3 (0.9%, n = 76) was low. On average, soldiers reported moderate levels of aggression and having about four individuals in their social network. Most soldiers reported owning 0 firearms at T0 and T2; approximately 30% reported owning at least one firearm at T0 and 36% at T2.
Note. Correlations between continuous variables are Pearson's r, correlations between binary variables are phi coefficients, and correlations between a continuous and a binary variable are point-biserial correlations. Percentages and sample sizes are the proportion of participants who endorsed that variable. Aggression scores ranged from 0 to 16; social network scores ranged from 0 to 9; alcohol use scores ranged from 1 to 5; firearm ownership scores ranged from 0 to 6.
* p < 0.01.
Mediation analysis
To test our first hypothesis, we examined the association between aggression at T0 and suicide attempts at T3 while controlling for lifetime suicide attempts at T0 and our control variables. History of PTSD (β = 0.04, s.e. = 0.04, 95% CI −0.03 to 0.12, p = 0.25), history of TBI (β = 0.02, s.e. = 0.05, 95% CI −0.07 to 0.12, p = 0.62), education (β = −0.11, s.e. = 0.06, 95% CI −0.23 to 0.01, p = 0.07), and trauma exposure at T1–T3 (β = 0.01, s.e. = 0.05, 95% CI −0.09 to 0.10, p = 0.93) were not associated with suicide attempts at T3; we therefore removed these variables from the model. Aggression (β = 0.10, s.e. = 0.04, 95% CI 0.02–0.18, p = 0.01), lifetime suicide attempts (β = 0.10, s.e. = 0.02, 95% CI 0.05–0.14, p < 0.001), and history of MDD (β = 0.11, s.e. = 0.03, 95% CI 0.05–0.17, p = 0.001) were associated with suicide attempts at T3 such that greater levels of aggression, reporting a lifetime suicide attempt, and a history of MDD were positively associated with likelihood of reporting a suicide attempt at T3. This model accounted for 4.4% of the variance in suicide attempts at T3.
To test our second hypothesis, we added paths from aggression at T0 to each mediator at T2 while controlling for each mediator at T0 and paths from each mediator at T2 to suicide attempts at T3. Firearm ownership at T2 was not associated with suicide attempts at T3 (β = −0.08, s.e. = 0.05, 95% CI −0.18 to 0.02, p = 0.11), and firearm ownership did not mediate the association between aggression at T0 and suicide attempts at T3 (β = −0.01, s.e. = 0.01, 95% CI −0.01 to 0.01, p = 0.14). Thus, we removed firearm ownership from our model. The final model included aggression at T0, lifetime suicide attempts, history of MDD, and social networks and alcohol use frequency as potential mediators.
Table 3 presents results of the final mediation model. All direct effects (‘a’ paths and ‘b’ paths) were significant. Aggression at T0 was associated with both a smaller social network and greater alcohol use frequency at T2 after controlling for social network and alcohol use at T0. Aggression at T0, suicide attempts at T0, history of MDD, social networks at T2, and alcohol use frequency at T2 all evidenced direct effects on suicide attempts at T3 in predicted directions. Specifically, greater levels of aggression, reporting a lifetime suicide attempt at T0, a history of MDD, a smaller social network, and greater frequency of alcohol use were associated with reporting a suicide attempt at T3. Further, aggression at T0 demonstrated a significant indirect effect on suicide attempts at T3 via social networks at T2 and alcohol use frequency at T2. The full mediation model accounted for 6.2% of the variance in suicide attempts at T3, suggesting addition of social network and alcohol use frequency accounted for an additional 2% of the variance in T3 suicide attempts beyond T0 aggression.
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.
Discussion
This study investigated the link between aggression and suicide attempts in active duty Army personnel and explored potential mediators of this association, including social networks, alcohol use frequency, and firearm ownership. Consistent with our first hypothesis, aggression predicted later suicide attempts while controlling for lifetime suicide attempts. Indeed, aggression continued to demonstrate a direct effect on suicide attempts in our full model while accounting for the effect of social networks and alcohol use frequency. These findings add to the sparse body of research implicating aggression as a longitudinal predictor of suicide attempts in service members (Schafer et al., Reference Schafer, Duffy, Kennedy, Stentz, Leon, Herrerias and Joiner2022) and reinforce the notion that aggression is a valuable predictor of suicide attempts.
In addition to examining the direct association between aggression and suicide attempts among Army service members, the present study represents one of the first attempts to identify mechanisms of this association. In support of hypothesis 2, social networks and alcohol use frequency partially mediated the association between aggression and suicide attempts. Our findings support theories of aggression and suicide that speculate that lower social connection and frequent alcohol use may partially explain how aggression increases risk for suicide (Conner et al., Reference Conner, Duberstein, Conwell and Caine2003; Hartley et al., Reference Hartley, Pettit and Castellanos2018). Specifically, it may be that problematic aggression leads to interpersonal conflict and subsequent loss of important social connections. Individuals who struggle with aggression may then experience significant social isolation and loneliness, which are strongly associated with suicidal ideation and attempts (Calati et al., Reference Calati, Ferrari, Brittner, Oasi, Olié, Carvalho and Courtet2019). Similarly, problematic aggression may give rise to frequent alcohol use, perhaps as a way to cope with adverse consequences of aggression or to manage aggressive impulses, which in turn may increase risk for suicide. These results underscore the potential utility of enhancing social support and addressing alcohol use to mitigate suicide risk in military settings.
Unexpectedly, firearm ownership did not mediate the association between aggression and suicide attempts. On the surface, this finding seems to diverge from prior work suggesting that access to firearms escalates suicide risk (Houtsma, Butterworth, & Anestis, Reference Houtsma, Butterworth and Anestis2018). On the other hand, there are many potential explanations for this unexpected finding. For instance, our measure of firearm ownership specifically asked about firearms in the home (house, apartment, barracks), as opposed to access to firearms both in and outside of the home. It may be that access to firearms outside of the home is important in understanding the association between aggression and suicide. This suggestion is in line with research indicating that individuals with access to firearms through their occupation are at increased risk for suicide compared to those without access (Milner, Witt, Maheen, & LaMontagne, Reference Milner, Witt, Maheen and LaMontagne2017). Alternatively, it may be that firearm ownership is an important predictor of lethal suicide attempts but not of non-lethal suicide attempts. Given that all self-reported suicide attempts in the current study were non-lethal, our results may have underestimated the true association between firearm ownership and suicide attempts. Further research on the role of firearms in the link between aggression and suicide among military personnel is clearly warranted.
Implications for future research and suicide prevention
The present study holds important implications for future research and suicide prevention efforts. For one, researchers should attempt to replicate our results among samples of service members and veterans who are undergoing the process of separating from service as this period is associated with loss of military social connections and a rise in suicide-related behavior (Sokol et al., Reference Sokol, Gromatsky, Edwards, Greene, Geraci, Harris and Goodman2021; U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs, Office of Mental Health and Suicide Prevention, 2023). Second, the fact that social networks and alcohol use partially mediated the association between aggression and suicide attempts suggests the presence of other mediating factors. Future research should examine whether other potential consequences of aggression (e.g. incarceration, loss of employment, mental health service utilization) impart increased risk for suicide among military personnel. Finally, further work should explore how shared mechanisms of aggression and suicide, such as impulsivity, effect the association between aggression and suicide (Gvion & Apter, Reference Gvion and Apter2011).
Future research should examine which facets of social support are most important in understanding the connection between aggression and suicide attempts. Arguably, our measure of social networks largely assessed emotional support from others – three of the four questions on this measure are related to the number of people one feels close to or can rely on. It remains unknown how other forms of social support (e.g. practical/instrumental support, informational support) impact the association between aggression and suicide.
The current study's results also imply that interventions aimed at mitigating suicide risk within this population may benefit from directly targeting aggressive behavior. Such a suggestion is especially relevant to suicide prevention within military populations given that aggression may be viewed as a highly valued, adaptive behavior in the military (Morland, Love, Mackintosh, Greene, & Rosen, Reference Morland, Love, Mackintosh, Greene and Rosen2012). Aggression may also serve as a marker for future suicide risk among active-duty service members. Further, results of our study suggest that early identification and intervention on problematic aggression may mitigate loss of social connection and frequent alcohol use, two factors already widely addressed in current suicide prevention efforts (Hou et al., Reference Hou, Wang, Guo, Zhang, Liu, Qi and Zhou2022; Jin, Khazem, & Anestis, Reference Jin, Khazem and Anestis2016).
Limitations and future directions
Evaluation of the present results necessitates discussion of the study's limitations. First, our measures were limited to assessments of alcohol use frequency, emotional social support, access to firearms in the home, and suicide attempts. Future research would benefit from a more comprehensive assessment of such variables, including consequences from alcohol use, other facets of social support, access to firearms outside the home, and other forms of suicide behaviors. It is possible that the behaviors examined herein (e.g. suicide attempts, aggressive behavior) could be conceived as unsuitable for military service and thus likely underreported by service members due to denial, stigma, and/or over-punishment for mental health problems (Guina, Welton, Broderick, & Peirson, Reference Guina, Welton, Broderick, Peirson, Roberts and Warner2018; Palamar, Martins, Su, & Ompad, Reference Palamar, Martins, Su and Ompad2015). Relatedly, our measure of social support did not assess perceived loneliness or identify who participants felt close to. As noted earlier, these data do not identify those who died from a suicide attempt, which may have attenuated the association between firearm ownership and suicide attempts. Third, our sample was comprised of Army soldiers transitioning from pre- and post-deployment, limiting generalizability of our results to other military branches. Fourth, as with any longitudinal study, we observed missing data during follow-up assessments. Although we used an imputation method appropriate for our data, imputation can only use information from observed data. Thus, our results may not accurately reflect associations only observed in the missing data.
Conclusion
Aggression has been identified as a potent risk factor for suicide in civilian populations, and our study adds to the small but growing body of research underscoring the significance of aggression in understanding military suicide. The present study also represents one of the first efforts to identify mechanisms of this association and highlights the roles of social networks and alcohol use frequency as mediators of the relation between aggression and suicide among Army soldiers. Understanding the role of aggression can inform targeted suicide prevention interventions aimed at reducing suicide risk among service members, an essential task in the face of rising suicide rates among military personnel.
Acknowledgements
This material was supported by resources and the use of facilities at the VISN 17 Center of Excellence for Research on Returning War Veterans and the Central Texas Veterans Health Care System. This publication is based on public use data from the Army Study to Assess Risk and Resilience in Servicemembers (STARRS). The data are available from the Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research (ICPSR) at the University of Michigan (http://doi.org/10.3886/ICPSR35197.v7). The contents of this publication are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of the STARRS investigators, funders, Department of the Army, or Department of Defense.
Funding statement
This material was funded by VA VISN 17 Center of Excellence Pilot Funding and Early Career Grant awarded to Dr. Krauss. Dr. Krauss was supported by the Department of Veterans Affairs Office of Academic Affiliations Advanced Fellowship Program in Mental Illness Research and Treatment, the Central Texas VA Health Care System, the VISN 17 Center of Excellence for Returning war Veterans, and Career Development Award (CDA) IK1-CX002650 from the VA Clinical Science Research and Development Service. Dr. Greene was supported by the VISN 2 Mental Illness, Research, Education and Clinic Center (MIRECC) and CDA IK1-RX004551 from the VA Rehabilitation Research and Development Service. Dr. Edwards and Dr. Goodman were supported by the VISN 2 MIRECC.
Competing interests
Marianne Goodman is a consultant for Boehringer Ingleheim Pharmacueticals. The other authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.
Ethical standards
The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.