Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T10:17:02.780Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Divergent subcortical activity for distinct executive functions: stopping and shifting in obsessive compulsive disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 November 2015

S. Morein-Zamir*
Affiliation:
Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Department of Psychology, Anglia Ruskin University, CambridgeUK
V. Voon
Affiliation:
Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
C. M. Dodds
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Exeter, UK
A. Sule
Affiliation:
Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK South Essex Partnership Trust, UK
J. van Niekerk
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
B. J. Sahakian
Affiliation:
Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
T. W. Robbins
Affiliation:
Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: Dr S. Morein-Zamir, Department of Psychology, Anglia Ruskin University, East Road, Cambridge, CB1 1PT, UK. (Email: [email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

There is evidence of executive function impairment in obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) that potentially contributes to symptom development and maintenance. Nevertheless, the precise nature of these executive impairments and their neural basis remains to be defined.

Method

We compared stopping and shifting, two key executive functions previously implicated in OCD, in the same task using functional magnetic resonance imaging, in patients with virtually no co-morbidities and age-, verbal IQ- and gender-matched healthy volunteers. The combined task allowed direct comparison of neural activity in stopping and shifting independent of patient sample characteristics and state variables such as arousal, learning, or current symptom expression.

Results

Both OCD patients and controls exhibited right inferior frontal cortex activation during stopping, and left inferior parietal cortex activation during shifting. However, widespread under-activation across frontal-parietal areas was found in OCD patients compared to controls for shifting but not stopping. Conservative, whole-brain analyses also indicated marked divergent abnormal activation in OCD in the caudate and thalamus for these two cognitive functions, with stopping-related over-activation contrasting with shift-related under-activation.

Conclusions

OCD is associated with selective components of executive function, which engage similar common elements of cortico-striatal regions in different abnormal ways. The results implicate altered neural activation of subcortical origin in executive function abnormalities in OCD that are dependent on the precise cognitive and contextual requirements, informing current theories of symptom expression.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015

Introduction

Executive functions enabling suppression or shifting away from no longer relevant actions or thoughts, may be impaired in several neuropsychiatric disorders. In particular, obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD), which is characterized by intrusive distressing thoughts and compulsions (APA, 2013). Whereas stopping or response inhibition involves the deliberate overriding or resisting of dominant responses, shifting refers to the ability to flexibly switch between mental sets or tasks (Miyake et al. Reference Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter and Wager2000; Snyder et al. Reference Snyder, Kaiser, Warren and Heller2015). Both may contribute to OCD symptom development and maintenance, with cognitive inflexibility and difficulties in inhibiting unwanted behaviour fostering rigid beliefs and repetitive behaviours that are resistant to change (Chamberlain et al. Reference Chamberlain, Blackwell, Fineberg, Robbins and Sahakian2005). Indeed, several recent meta-analyses have concluded that despite heterogeneity in the literature, OCD is associated with broad executive function impairments with medium to large effect sizes (Abramovitch et al. Reference Abramovitch, Abramowitz and Mittelman2013; Shin et al. Reference Shin, Lee, Kim and Kwon2014; Snyder et al. Reference Snyder, Kaiser, Warren and Heller2015). Despite the specific theoretical importance suggested for stopping and shifting in relation to OCD (Chamberlain et al. Reference Chamberlain, Blackwell, Fineberg, Robbins and Sahakian2005), impairments of broadly similar magnitude were noted for additional executive function sub-domains such as working memory and planning (Abramovitch et al. Reference Abramovitch, Abramowitz and Mittelman2013; Snyder et al. Reference Snyder, Kaiser, Warren and Heller2015). Thus, it has been proposed that executive function difficulties in OCD are due to a shared, general overlapping component (Snyder et al. Reference Snyder, Kaiser, Warren and Heller2015).

Evidence from functional imaging can test this hypothesis. Executive function abnormalities are broadly consistent with the fronto-striatal dysfunction reported in OCD (Menzies et al. Reference Menzies, Chamberlain, Laird, Thelen, Sahakian and Bullmore2008; Milad & Rauch, Reference Milad and Rauch2012). However, functional imaging studies of executive functioning in OCD have yielded heterogeneous findings, with limited convergence. Some studies have shown increased prefrontal activation in patients, having attributed this to overactive monitoring (Maltby et al. Reference Maltby, Tolin, Worhunsky, O'Keefe and Kiehl2005). However, extensive hypoactivation in switching, working memory and spatial planning tasks has also been reported (van den Heuvel et al. Reference van den Heuvel, Veltman, Groenewegen, Cath, van Balkom, van Hartskamp, Barkhof and van Dyck2005; Nakao et al. Reference Nakao, Nakagawa, Nakatani, Nabeyama, Sanematsu, Yoshiura, Togao, Tomita, Masuda, Yoshioka, Kuroki and Kanba2009). To reconcile such disparities, it was suggested that over-activation may characterize affective ventral corticostriatal systems with hypoactivation in more dorsal, putatively cognitive, circuits (van den Heuvel et al. Reference van den Heuvel, Veltman, Groenewegen, Cath, van Balkom, van Hartskamp, Barkhof and van Dyck2005; Nakao et al. Reference Nakao, Nakagawa, Nakatani, Nabeyama, Sanematsu, Yoshiura, Togao, Tomita, Masuda, Yoshioka, Kuroki and Kanba2009). However, complex and even contradictory over- and under-activation patterns have been noted in non-affective executive tasks. For example, during response inhibition both OCD-specific increased and decreased activation in the caudate, thalamus and cingulate have been reported (Maltby et al. Reference Maltby, Tolin, Worhunsky, O'Keefe and Kiehl2005; Roth et al. Reference Roth, Saykin, Flashman, Pixley, West and Mamourian2007; Page et al. Reference Page, Rubia, Deeley, Daly, Toal, Mataix-Cols, Giampietro, Schmitz and Murphy2009; Kang et al. Reference Kang, Jang, Han, Kim, Jung, Choi, Choi and Kwon2013).

These seemingly contradictory results have yielded competing interpretations. Increased activation during executive functioning has been interpreted as compensatory (Roth et al. Reference Roth, Saykin, Flashman, Pixley, West and Mamourian2007; Page et al. Reference Page, Rubia, Deeley, Daly, Toal, Mataix-Cols, Giampietro, Schmitz and Murphy2009; de Vries et al. Reference de Vries, de Wit, Cath, van der Werf, van der Borden, van Rossum, van Balkom, van der Wee, Veltman and van den Heuvel2014) or overactive self-regulation processes (Ursu et al. Reference Ursu, Stenger, Shear, Jones and Carter2003). At the same time, reduced activation was taken to indicate general dysfunction/executive impairment (Remijnse et al. Reference Remijnse, van den Heuvel, Nielen, Vriend, Hendriks, Hoogendijk, Uylings and Veltman2013) or insufficient recruitment possibly due to interference from chronic OCD symptom-related over-activation (Evans et al. Reference Evans, Lewis and Iobst2004). Abnormal activation patterns may also reflect compensatory use of alternate neural substrates in patients (Page et al. Reference Page, Rubia, Deeley, Daly, Toal, Mataix-Cols, Giampietro, Schmitz and Murphy2009).

As with behavioural studies, heterogeneous findings can be attributed in part to between-study differences in patient characteristics such as symptom severity, co-morbidities and medication status (Kuelz et al. Reference Kuelz, Hohagen and Voderholzer2004). Additionally, task demands, including difficulty, load, and learning requirements vary considerably between studies, leading to differences in state fluctuations in attention, motivation or even current symptom expression.

To address the competing interpretations regarding functional abnormalities in OCD, we combined different subconstructs from the Research Domain Criteria framework (www.nimh.nih.gov/research-priorities/rdoc/index.shtml) found under cognitive control, in a theoretically driven manner (Miyake et al. Reference Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter and Wager2000). Specifically, we contrasted the neural correlates of stopping and shifting within the same task in adult OCD patients compared to matched healthy controls, thus controlling not only for patient-related confounds but importantly also for task-related confounds. This in turn promotes the understanding of the neural networks involved in response inhibition and switching, possibly leading to implications for patients’ symptoms and experience. In healthy adults, this combined task has previously revealed stopping specific activation in the right inferior frontal cortex (IFC) and shifting specific activation in the left inferior parietal cortex (IPC), against a background of extensive co-activation for both in fronto-parietal regions (Dodds et al. Reference Dodds, Morein-Zamir and Robbins2011). In sum, examining two key distinct yet overlapping executive functions (Miyake et al. Reference Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter and Wager2000), of likely relevance to OCD symptoms, allowed us to investigate the neurobehavioural specificity in the dysexecutive functioning of patients.

Method and materials

Participants

Nineteen OCD patients with median Yale–Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (YBOCS) score of 20 (range 12–30) were matched for age and gender with 19 healthy controls (14 females in each group). OCD patients were recruited from the Cambridgeshire and Peterborough Foundation NHS Trust and from local support groups. Diagnosis according to the DSM-IV criteria followed a detailed interview with a psychiatrist or clinical psychologist supplemented with the MINI (Sheehan et al. Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs, Weiller, Hergueta, Baker and Dunbar1998). The patients did not satisfy DSM-IV criteria for other Axis-I disorders with the exception of two who satisfied criteria for generalized anxiety disorder. Thirteen patients were prescribed serotonin reuptake inhibitors and one a tricyclic antidepressant. Exclusion criteria included substance abuse in the last 3 months and prior diagnosis of schizophrenia, psychotic disorders, bipolar disorder or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Control participants were recruited via posters in the community and from the Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute participant panel. Data from three controls were included in a previous report (Dodds et al. Reference Dodds, Morein-Zamir and Robbins2011). For controls, exclusion criteria included no current or past psychiatric disorders and no psychoactive medications. For all participants further exclusion criteria were current or past neurological disorders (including tic disorders), brain damage or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) contraindications. At testing, the YBOCS (Goodman et al. Reference Goodman, Price, Rasmussen, Mazure, Fleischmann, Hill, Heninger and Charney1989) and Obsessive Compulsive Inventory – Revised (OCI-R; Foa et al. Reference Foa, Huppert, Leiberg, Langner, Kichic, Hajcak and Salkovskis2002) assessed OCD severity, the National Adult Reading Test (NART; Nelson, Reference Nelson1982) assessed verbal IQ and the Montgomery–Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS; Montgomery & Asberg, Reference Montgomery and Asberg1979) assessed depressive symptom severity. The Cambridge Local Research Ethics Committee (08/H0308/65) approved the study, and participants provided informed consent and were reimbursed for participation.

Procedure

Participants performed a combined shifting go/no-go task (Dodds et al. Reference Dodds, Morein-Zamir and Robbins2011). On each trial they were presented with a superimposed image of a face and a house. The image border colour determined relevant stimulus dimension, for example a red border denoted faces while blue denoted houses as presently relevant. In complex blocks border colour changed every few trials, with that trial constituting a shift trial, where subjects had to shift their attention accordingly between face and house stimuli dimensions. Go/no-go responses were determined by face gender or house storey. For example, participants were told when the border was red they had to attend the faces and respond when the face is female and withhold responding when the face is male (see Supplementary Fig. S1). When the border was blue, they had to attend to houses and respond to two-storey houses but not to one-storey houses. In simple blocks the colour remained constant and subjects attended a single stimulus dimension (faces or houses) throughout. Participants completed a simple and complex block in each of two runs, with block order and go/no-go rules counterbalanced across subjects within each group. On each trial, a red or blue border appeared for 1000 ms, following which the image of an overlapping face and house was presented inside this frame. On go trials, participants had to respond within 725 ms whereupon the display disappeared. On no-go trials, participants had to refrain from responding for the same duration. Following a correct response, a blank screen appeared for 1000 ms, whereupon negative verbal feedback was presented for the same duration following an incorrect response.

Prior to entering the scanner, participants practised both conditions to ensure they understood the task and instructions, which were again presented before each block in the scanner for 10 s, informing participants of the go/no-go and shift rules. In the simple version there were a total of 40 stop and 280 go trials, and in the complex version, there were 40 stop, 40 shift, and 240 go trials, yielding a ratio of stop:go trials and shift:go trials of 1–7. Blocks consisted of approximately 160 trials (158–166), with 4–12 go trials between consecutive stop trials and between consecutive shift trials. The task was presented via E-Prime (Psychological Software Tools Inc., USA) and projected onto a mirror in the scanner, where responses were registered via a customized button box.

Scanning acquisition

Scanning was carried out at the Wolfson Brain Imaging Centre, Cambridge, on a 3-T Siemens Tim Trio scanner. Functional imaging data were collected in a single session using whole-brain echo planar images (EPI) with the following parameters: repetition time (TR) = 2000 ms; echo time (TE) = 30 ms; flip angle = 78°; 32 slices with slice thickness 3 mm plus 0.75 mm gap; matrix = 64 × 64; field of view (FOV) = 192 × 192 mm yielding 3 × 3 mm in-plane resolution; echo spacing 0.47 ms and bandwidth 2442 Hz/Px. Volumes acquired per run varied from 456 to 485 depending on total trial number. Structural T1-weighted MR scans using a magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient-echo (MPRAGE) sequence were used for registration (176 slices of 1 mm thickness; TR = 2300 ms; TE = 2.98 ms, TI = 900 ms, flip angle = 9°, FOV = 240 × 256 mm).

Data analysis

For behavioural data, repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) contrasted group (OCD v. controls) on commission errors and omission errors for each block (simple v. complex). Additionally, a 2 × 2 × 3 ANOVA compared group correct go reaction times (RT) for face v. house stimuli on simple, complex and switch trials. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data were processed and analysed using Statistical Parametric Mapping 8 (SPM, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). Images from the first five volumes were discarded to allow for T1 equilibrium effects. Images were slice time-corrected and spatially realigned, and then co-registered to the structural image using the mean functional volume. Subsequent normalization to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template was followed with re-sampling of EPI volumes to 2 mm isotropic voxels and smoothing with a 6-mm full-width half-maximum Gaussian kernel. Design matrices were implemented using the general linear model (GLM). First-level regressors for complex blocks: correct stop trials, shift trials, and two subsets of correct go trials; for simple blocks: correct stop trials and a subset of correct go trials. Additional regressors of no interest included incorrect stop trials, and parametric modulators for go and shift RT. Go trials comprised separate random selections of trials matched in number to correct stop or shift trials in that block, and were included to allow subsequent conjunction analyses with separate baselines (see Dodds et al. Reference Dodds, Morein-Zamir and Robbins2011 for additional details). Regressors, modelled at target onset, were convolved with a canonical haemodynamic response function. The data were high-pass filtered (1/128-Hz cut-off) and serial correlations were accounted for by a first-degree autoregressive AR (1) model. Mean number of trials was 26, 30 and 34 for complex stop, shift and simple stop contrasts, respectively. Contrasts for each participant for shift v. go, stop v. go and simple stop v. go were used in second-level analyses.

Second-level analyses compared the groups in complex stopping v. go, shifting v. go, and simple stopping v. go in two-sample t tests. To investigate common or diverging process-specific abnormalities, we further examined trial/task type and group in second-level whole-brain repeated-measures ANOVAs. Activations associated with general executive functions were investigated with common activations for stopping and shifting using random-effects conjunction analyses against the conjunction null hypothesis. These served as a search area to inspect potential group differences in overall activation in relevant fronto-parietal regions. Divergent abnormalities were examined with the interaction between task and group. All analyses, both between and within groups, were conducted at the whole-brain with family-wise error (FWE) correction set at p < 0.05 unless otherwise stated. Secondary uncorrected whole-brain analyses were set to p < 0.001 with minimal extent of 5 voxels to provide a more complete overview of the findings and to counteract concerns regarding type II error. Where appropriate, to better characterize results from the above whole-brain analyses, post-hoc analyses were conducted on anatomical regions of interest (ROIs; Brett et al. Reference Brett, Anton, Valabregue and Poline2002), taken from the Automated Anatomical Labeling atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al. Reference Tzourio-Mazoyer, Landeau, Papathanassiou, Crivello, Etard, Delcroix, Mazoyer and Joliot2002).

Results

Demographics and clinical measures

The groups were matched for age, gender and verbal IQ with OCD patients reporting increased OCD symptom severity levels and slightly elevated depression, although not in the clinical range (see Table 1).

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of OCD and control groups

OCD, Obsessive compulsive disorder; IQ, intelligence quotient; MADRS, Montgomery–Asberg Depression Rating Scale; YBOCS, Yale–Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale; OCI-R, Obsessive Compulsive Inventory – revised; STAI, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory.

Behavioural measures

There were no significant behavioural differences between OCD patients and controls in any performance indices. There were more commission errors in the complex (28.26%) compared to the simple (15.05%) tasks, (F 1,36 = 46.60, p < 0.001), but no significant group effect (p = 0.629) nor did group interact with difficulty (p = 0.176). Mean omission errors in simple go trials was 2.76%, in complex go trials 3.47% and in switch trials 5.02% (F 2,72 = 5.75, p < 0.01). There was no significant group effect (p = 0.218), nor did it interact with trial type (p = 0.931). Finally, in an ANOVA with group, trial type and stimulus type as factors, mean RT was 599 ms for controls and 614 ms for patients, which was not significantly different (F 1,36 = 1.11, p = 0.298). Responses to faces were faster than to houses (600 v. 613 ms, respectively; F 1,36 = 21.947, p < 0.001). There was an interaction with trial (F 1,36 = 23.479, p < 0.001), with slower RTs to houses compared to faces in complex go trials (F 1,36 = 40.23, p < 0.001) and shift trials (F 1,36 = 28.69, p < 0.001) but not simple trials (p = 0.121). This pattern clearly indicated participants successfully shifted their attention on shift trials to the relevant dimension, and planned comparisons indicated this was the case for both controls (F 1,36 = 16.24, p < 0.01) and patients (F 1,36 =  12.57, p < 0.01). Additional comparisons of switch costs similarly did not reveal any group differences (p's > 0.31). In sum, no group differences were noted in any analyses. Additionally, no performance indices correlated with OCD or depression severity in the patients. The absence of behavioural group differences means that any changes in fMRI activations below cannot be attributed to performance effects, being more likely to represent underlying neural group differences.

Neuroimaging

Shifting

OCD patients showed lower activation associated with shifting than healthy controls in the left pre-supplementary motor cortex, right precuneus, occipital cortex bilaterally and right thalamus, in a whole-brain analysis corrected at FWE, p < 0.05 (see Table 2). There was no evidence for increased activations in the OCD group compared to controls even when lowering the threshold to whole-brain uncorrected p < 0.001. When the groups were assessed individually, whole-brain analyses corrected at FWE, p < 0.05 showed shifting related activation in controls in fronto-parietal regions, with peaks in the left inferior parietal, IFC bilaterally and striatum in addition to the occipital cortex bilaterally. Patients showed only a few clusters in the left inferior parietal with additional isolated activations in the right parietal lobe (see Fig. 1 and Supplementary material).

Fig. 1. Whole-brain shifting-related activation with a threshold of p < 0.05 family-wise error corrected. (a) Illustration of fronto-parietal and occipital region activations in a group of healthy control participants. (b) Illustration of inferior parietal region activations in a group of obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) participants.

Table 2. Group differences in brain activation in whole-brain analyses, family-wise error corrected p < 0.05

OCD, Obsessive compulsive disorder; MNI, Montreal Neurological Institute.

Coordinates are in MNI space. All differences are at whole-brain with family-wise error corrected at p < 0.05 unless otherwise stated.

a Interaction refers to voxels associated with greater activation in OCD patients compared to controls when stopping but reduced activation in OCD patients compared to controls when shifting.

*Significant at p < 0.06 corrected for family-wise error.

Stopping

During complex stopping, OCD patients demonstrated greater activation than controls in the left occipital lobe in a whole-brain analysis corrected at FWE, p < 0.05 and left caudate, p < 0.06 (see Table 2). There was no evidence of hypoactivation in the OCD patients compared to controls, even a threshold of whole-brain uncorrected, p < 0.001. When each group was inspected individually, whole-brain analyses corrected at FWE, p < 0.05 showed stopping related activations in healthy controls in the IFC bilaterally as well as the parietal and occipital lobes bilaterally. At this threshold, patients demonstrated stopping related activation confined to the right IFC, the parietal cortex bilaterally and the left thalamus in addition to the left occipital lobe (see Supplementary material).

During simple stopping no significant differences were noted between patients and controls. Each group demonstrated significant activation in the right IFC, with patients showing also activation in the left fusiform and controls showing activation in the inferior parietal bilaterally in addition to the right angular gyrus, right fusiform and occipital cortex (see Supplementary material for further analyses).

Common activation associated with stopping and shifting

The conjunction whole-brain analyses corrected at FWE, p < 0.05 across all individuals revealed fronto-parietal activations during stop and shift trials compared to go trials. These areas included clusters in the inferior parietal cortex bilaterally, in addition to IFC bilaterally, left supplementary motor area, left fusiform gyrus, left middle occipital gyrus, right precuneus and right middle and superior and frontal gyri (Fig. 2). Overall mean activation across this search area was significantly reduced in the OCD group compared to controls for shifting (t 35 = 3.17, p < 0.001) but not stopping (t 35 = 0.57, p > 0.701). These results survived when the conjunction-based search area was defined by a more liberal threshold (p < 0.001 uncorrected) or an independent search area (Morein-Zamir et al. Reference Morein-Zamir, Dodds, van Hartevelt, Schwarzkopt, Sahakian, Muller and Robbins2014).

Fig. 2. Areas commonly activated during stop and shift trial relative to go trials across all participants overlaid on the MNI brain. Images are displayed at x = 40, y = 8 and z = 38 in the sagittal, coronal and axial planes, respectively, with a voxel-wise threshold of p < 0.001 uncorrected. Colour bar represents t scores.

Opposing abnormal activation associated with stopping and shifting

We also investigated whether there were brain regions associated with opposing activations during stopping and shifting in the OCD patients compared to controls, using a mixed-measures ANOVA with stopping and shifting as repeated measures and group as a between-subjects measure. As noted in Table 2, whole-brain analyses corrected at FWE, p < 0.05 indicated significant activations in the thalamus and caudate bilaterally, in addition to the right precuneus, with patients showing increased activation for stopping but decreased for shifting compared to controls (Fig. 3a ). To better characterize the interaction in the caudate observed in the whole-brain analysis, individual contrast values were derived from caudate anatomical ROIs and entered into a mixed-measures ANOVA with group, trial type and side as independent variables. Though no main effect of group, there was a group×task interaction (F 1,36 = 23.73, p < 0.001). Comparisons indicated greater bilateral caudate activation in controls compared to patients for shifting (F 1,36 = 13.61, p = 0.001) and greater bilateral caudate activation in patients compared to controls for stopping (F 1,36 = 5.23, p = 0.028) (Fig. 3b ). Correlation analysis revealed greater symptom severity was associated with reduced caudate activation during shifting, (r = −0.41, and −0.43, p < 0.08, for left and right caudate, respectively).

Fig. 3. Voxels associated with greater activation in obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) patients compared to controls when stopping but reduced activation in OCD patients compared to controls when shifting. (a) Areas showing this pattern of activation are displayed overlaid on the MNI brain. Images are displayed at x = −12, y = −6 and z = 16 in the sagittal, coronal and axial planes, respectively, with a voxel-wise threshold of p < 0.05 family-wise error corrected. Colour bars represent t scores. (b) Region-of-interest post-hoc analysis of activity for stop and shift trials in control and OCD patients groups in the caudate bilaterally. Error bars represent s.e.m..

Discussion

In a combined stop-shift task, OCD patients with virtually no co-morbidities engaged broadly the same regions as healthy volunteers, with right IFC activations during stopping and left IPC activations during shifting. Importantly, however, extensive under-activation specifically during shifting was found for patients compared to controls across fronto-parietal regions associated with executive functioning. During stopping patients exhibited focal over-activation in the caudate and thalamus and the medial occipital lobe. The caudate and thalamus, regions previously implicated in OCD, showed contrasting patterns of abnormality in the patients using conservative whole-brain analyses. The under-activation during shifting, showing some association with symptom severity in the caudate, and the opposing over-activation of this same region during stopping, support fronto-striatal abnormalities in OCD, while also clearly implicating fronto-parietal regions in aspects of executive dysfunction. In contrast to the hypothesis that there would be overlapping abnormalities between stopping and shifting, indicative of general executive impairment in OCD, the findings point to multiple distinct neural correlates of executive abnormalities.

The opposing aberrant activations demonstrate how observed functional abnormalities in OCD depend on the precise cognitive requirements with no tendency for general task-related ‘hypoactivation’ or ‘hyperactivation’ of key structures. This may have implications more generally for the interpretation of imaging findings in OCD, as it underlines how not only cognitive demands but also symptom provocation and task challenges may yield opposite activations in regions such as the caudate or orbitofrontal cortex (Milad & Rauch, Reference Milad and Rauch2012). Present findings also help clarify the inconsistent imaging results reported, purporting that seemingly opposing findings for a given neural region in different settings could be a key characteristic of fronto-striatal OCD dysfunction. Participants were required to inhibit responding and shift attention in the same task, ruling out a host of situational variables (e.g. on-task symptom expression, fatigue, and practice) that could underlie between-task or between-study differences. Similarly, shifting and stopping did not differ in important task demands, with neither requiring trial-and-error learning, and both occurring equally infrequently, rendering salience or attentional capture unlikely to account for the results. For both stopping and shifting, instructions left no ambiguity regarding which was the appropriate response on each trial. Such factors have likely contributed to inconsistencies in findings (Morein-Zamir et al. Reference Morein-Zamir, Papmeyer, Gillan, Crockett, Fineberg, Sahakian and Robbins2013b ). The results caution against simple models of executive impairment in OCD and against attributing global over- or under-activation to particular circuits. Inhibitory dysfunction is thus unlikely simply to result from hypoactive brain regions associated with cognitive control and/or overactive brain areas associated with error monitoring (Page et al. Reference Page, Rubia, Deeley, Daly, Toal, Mataix-Cols, Giampietro, Schmitz and Murphy2009; de Wit et al. Reference de Wit, de Vries, van der Werf, Cath, Heslenfeld, Veltman, van Balkom, Veltman and van den Heuvel2012). Similarly, caudate and thalamus overactivation during stopping indicates that their hypoactivation during shifting is not due to these regions being generally less capable of recruitment. Rather, present findings indicate that neural abnormalities associated with executive function in OCD appear, as a rule, task-dependent.

The hypoactivation associated with rule-determined shifting demonstrates insufficient widespread recruitment for this cognitive function, lending credence to former findings (Gu et al. Reference Gu, Park, Kang, Lee, Yoo, Jo, Choi, Lee and Kwon2008; Page et al. Reference Page, Rubia, Deeley, Daly, Toal, Mataix-Cols, Giampietro, Schmitz and Murphy2009). In contrast to a previous study reporting no shift-related activation in OCD patients (Gu et al. Reference Gu, Park, Kang, Lee, Yoo, Jo, Choi, Lee and Kwon2008), we noted left IPC activation, albeit at a reduced level (see Supplementary material). This region, also found in the controls, is implicated in switching or shifting (Wager et al. Reference Wager, Jonides and Reading2004), suggesting that patients utilize the relevant neural substrates, although insufficiently so. Reduced brain activation despite adequate task performance is commonly observed in OCD (Maltby et al. Reference Maltby, Tolin, Worhunsky, O'Keefe and Kiehl2005; Nakao et al. Reference Nakao, Nakagawa, Yoshiura, Nakatani, Nabeyama, Yoshizato, Kudoh, Tada, Yoshioka and Kawamoto2005; Page et al. Reference Page, Rubia, Deeley, Daly, Toal, Mataix-Cols, Giampietro, Schmitz and Murphy2009), supporting the suggestion that it may be a sensitive index of neurocognitive dysfunction even in the absence of behavioural differences. This is in line with the notion that performance in cognitive flexibility tasks where correct responding is determined by explicit rules may be insensitive to the commonly reported inflexibility and perfectionism (Moritz et al. Reference Moritz, Hubner and Kluwe2004; Meiran et al. Reference Meiran, Diamond, Toder and Nemets2011). At the same time, the tentative association between symptom severity and reduced caudate activation links inefficient recruitment during shifting to a key brain region implicated in the disorder. The widespread hypoactivation may also relate to difficulties in late-stage disengagement reported in OCD (Morein-Zamir et al. Reference Morein-Zamir, Fineberg, Robbins and Sahakian2010, Reference Morein-Zamir, Papmeyer, Durieux, Fineberg, Sahakian and Robbins2013a ) providing a more specific delineation of cognitive inflexibility.

In contrast to the widespread shift-related under-activation, stop-related over-activation was largely specific to the caudate and thalamus in whole-brain analyses, both previously implicated in response inhibition. This conforms with abnormal response control in OCD involving fronto-striatal loops (Menzies et al. Reference Menzies, Chamberlain, Laird, Thelen, Sahakian and Bullmore2008). The caudate and thalamus are widely implicated in OCD pathophysiology including anatomical abnormalities (Rotge et al. Reference Rotge, Guehl, Dilharreguy, Tignol, Bioulac, Allard, Burbaud and Aouizerate2009; Shaw et al. Reference Shaw, Sharp, Sudre, Wharton, Greenstein, Raznahan, Evans, Chakravarty, Lerch and Rapoport2015) and aberrant functionality during rest, provocation and task performance (Whiteside et al. Reference Whiteside, Port and Abramowitz2004; Rotge et al. Reference Rotge, Guehl, Dilharreguy, Cuny, Tignol, Bioulac, Allard, Burbaud and Aouizerate2008). The patients also demonstrated increased cuneus activation during stopping. Although unexpected, cuneus hyperactivation in OCD during working memory has been reported (Nakao et al. Reference Nakao, Nakagawa, Nakatani, Nabeyama, Sanematsu, Yoshiura, Togao, Tomita, Masuda, Yoshioka, Kuroki and Kanba2009), as has hyperactivation during stopping in the occipital cortex (Roth et al. Reference Roth, Saykin, Flashman, Pixley, West and Mamourian2007; Page et al. Reference Page, Rubia, Deeley, Daly, Toal, Mataix-Cols, Giampietro, Schmitz and Murphy2009). This could reflect heightened processing due to exaggerated emotional responsiveness and arousal or be indicative of compensatory mechanisms allowing adequate performance (Page et al. Reference Page, Rubia, Deeley, Daly, Toal, Mataix-Cols, Giampietro, Schmitz and Murphy2009). The results stress the importance of whole-brain analyses and the role of posterior areas in mediating abnormal cognitive function in OCD (Menzies et al. Reference Menzies, Chamberlain, Laird, Thelen, Sahakian and Bullmore2008).

Prefrontal activation, particularly right IFC, was noted in both groups during stopping with no hypoactivation in OCD, consistent with some studies (Maltby et al. Reference Maltby, Tolin, Worhunsky, O'Keefe and Kiehl2005; Page et al. Reference Page, Rubia, Deeley, Daly, Toal, Mataix-Cols, Giampietro, Schmitz and Murphy2009) but not others adopting ROI or liberal approaches (Roth et al. Reference Roth, Saykin, Flashman, Pixley, West and Mamourian2007; de Wit et al. Reference de Wit, de Vries, van der Werf, Cath, Heslenfeld, Veltman, van Balkom, Veltman and van den Heuvel2012). Variable prefrontal cortex findings in OCD may result from its prolonged developmental trajectory along with formation of compensatory cognitive strategies and patients’ generally high level of cooperation and motivation. This interpretation is consistent with the adequate performance levels noted. This was advantageous as the brain activation results were not confounded by performance differences (Frith et al. Reference Frith, Friston, Herold, Silbersweig, Fletcher, Cahill, Dolan, Frackowiak and Liddle1995; Weinberger & Berman, Reference Weinberger and Berman1996). Response inhibition deficits are observed in OCD when inhibitory demands are high, but not when they are lower as in go/no-go tasks (Watkins et al. Reference Watkins, Sahakian, Robertson, Veale, Rogers, Pickard, Aitken and Robbins2005; Menzies et al. Reference Menzies, Achard, Chamberlain, Fineberg, Chen, del Campo, Sahakian, Robbins and Bullmore2007; Bohne et al. Reference Bohne, Savage, Deckersbach, Keuthen and Wilhelm2008; Morein-Zamir et al. Reference Morein-Zamir, Fineberg, Robbins and Sahakian2010). The present task was not designed to be challenging, employing considerable practice and clear instructions, though it is anticipated that with additional demands, behavioural impairments would have become apparent (Morein-Zamir et al. Reference Morein-Zamir, Papmeyer, Gillan, Crockett, Fineberg, Sahakian and Robbins2013b ). Further, participants responded within a limited time-window, which may have facilitated performance particularly in OCD patients. In any case, the findings point to a role for subcortical functional integrity during response inhibition in OCD, which may manifest during challenging situations encountered in everyday life. In sum, even a conservative interpretation of present results implicates aberrant striatal and thalamic functioning in OCD during executive functioning.

The results also delineate the advantage of using similar functional paradigms across psychiatric disorders. Fronto-striatal abnormalities and stopping and shifting impairments, have also been implicated in drug dependence, schizophrenia and ADHD (Willcutt et al. Reference Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone and Pennington2005; Robbins, Reference Robbins2007). Executive function difficulties appear similar between disorders, although direct between-group comparisons are often hindered by sample confounds including age, medication and co-morbidity status. How then can seemingly similar cognitive difficulties contribute to dysfunction in disorders with such disparate symptoms? For example, inhibitory difficulties in ADHD and stimulant users have been linked to an impulsive style, in contrast to OCD (Morein-Zamir & Robbins, Reference Morein-Zamir and Robbins2014). Present results illustrate how neurobiological differences can inform this issue: as opposed to the caudate hyperactivation in OCD, stop-related caudate hypoactivation was found in ADHD children and occasional stimulant users (Rubia et al. Reference Rubia, Smith, Brammer, Toone and Taylor2005, Reference Rubia, Cubillo, Woolley, Brammer and Smith2011; Harle et al. Reference Harle, Shenoy, Stewart, Tapert, Yu and Paulus2014). Moreover, a recent study of adult ADHD using the same stop-shift task in our group found strikingly different results to those reported here for OCD. Whereas performance was impaired and abnormalities noted in the right IFC, no shift-related under-activation or stop-related over-activation was observed (Morein-Zamir et al. Reference Morein-Zamir, Dodds, van Hartevelt, Schwarzkopt, Sahakian, Muller and Robbins2014). Similar right IFC under-activation in chronic stimulant users was also reported in a stop-signal task (Morein-Zamir et al. Reference Morein-Zamir, Simon Jones, Bullmore, Robbins and Ersche2013c ). We speculate that stopping abnormalities in OCD are more closely linked to response control aberrations, being less attributable to attentional or executive function difficulties. As such, though subtle behaviourally (Abramovitch et al. Reference Abramovitch, Abramowitz and Mittelman2013), stopping abnormalities could contribute to and result from executing deliberate repetitive actions over many years. Taken together, the results demonstrate that, although the neural circuitry and cognitive processes mediating various neuropsychiatric disorders overlap, the disparate clinical features are accompanied by highly distinct functional abnormalities, particularly in the striatum (Hart et al. Reference Hart, Radua, Nakao, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2013; Shaw et al. Reference Shaw, Sharp, Sudre, Wharton, Greenstein, Raznahan, Evans, Chakravarty, Lerch and Rapoport2015).

This study employed a well-characterized sample of mixed gender and medication status with almost no co-morbid Axis-I disorders, including depression. Whilst secondary analyses suggested medication was unlikely to play a role (see Supplementary material) as does the evidence from first degree siblings (Chamberlain et al. Reference Chamberlain, Menzies, Hampshire, Suckling, Fineberg, del Campo, Aitken, Craig, Owen, Bullmore, Robbins and Sahakian2008), present sample size was not sufficiently large to address this definitively and future studies should verify the role of medication directly. Similarly, the sample did not allow for analyses regarding symptom dimensions, though patients reported increased symptom severity for all OCI-R subscales except hoarding. The study design did not include null events or rest conditions and so group differences in go trials could not be verified. OCD patients, however, appear to have abnormal resting state activation (Whiteside et al. Reference Whiteside, Port and Abramowitz2004) and therefore inclusion of such conditions could have limited utility. At the same time, the study has several key strengths including examining multiple executive functions within the same task, allowing control of state variables and task demand confounds and employment of conservative whole-brain analyses. In summary, executive dysfunction in OCD appears to be mediated by separable cognitive functions, each associated with distinct patterns of abnormality not only across but also within the same cortico-striatal substrates. The latter finding suggests a new perspective for interpreting the neural substrates of OCD.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715002330.

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by a Wellcome Trust grant (089589/Z/09/Z) awarded to T. W. Robbins, B. J. Everitt, A. C. Roberts, J. W. Dalley and B. J. Sahakian. Work was completed at the Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute which is supported by a joint award from the Medical Research Council and Wellcome Trust (G00001354). We thank all participants who took part in the study.

Declaration of Interest

T.W.R. consults for Cambridge Cognition, Lilly, Lundbeck, Teva, Shire Pharmaceuticals and Merck, Sharp and Dohme. He has received research grants from Lilly, Lundbeck and GSK. B.J.S. has consulted for Boehringer-Ingelheim, Cambridge Cognition, Eli Lilly, GSK, Novartis, and Shire; and holds a grant funded by Johnson and Johnson. All remaining authors report no potential conflicts of interest.

References

Abramovitch, A, Abramowitz, JS, Mittelman, A (2013). The neuropsychology of adult obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychological Review 33, 11631171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
APA (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-5. American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Bohne, A, Savage, CR, Deckersbach, T, Keuthen, NJ, Wilhelm, S (2008). Motor inhibition in trichotillomania and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research 42, 141150.Google Scholar
Brett, M, Anton, J, Valabregue, R, Poline, JB (2002). Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox. In Abstract presented at the 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain, June 2–6, Sendai, Japan.Google Scholar
Chamberlain, SR, Blackwell, AD, Fineberg, NA, Robbins, TW, Sahakian, BJ (2005). The neuropsychology of obsessive compulsive disorder: the importance of failures in cognitive and behavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews 29, 399419.Google Scholar
Chamberlain, SR, Menzies, L, Hampshire, A, Suckling, J, Fineberg, NA, del Campo, N, Aitken, M, Craig, K, Owen, AM, Bullmore, ET, Robbins, TW, Sahakian, BJ (2008). Orbitofrontal dysfunction in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and their unaffected relatives. Science 321, 421422.Google Scholar
de Vries, FE, de Wit, SJ, Cath, DC, van der Werf, YD, van der Borden, V, van Rossum, TB, van Balkom, AJ, van der Wee, NJ, Veltman, DJ, van den Heuvel, OA (2014). Compensatory frontoparietal activity during working memory: an endophenotype of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry 76, 878887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Wit, SJ, de Vries, FE, van der Werf, YD, Cath, DC, Heslenfeld, DJ, Veltman, EM, van Balkom, AJ, Veltman, DJ, van den Heuvel, OA (2012). Presupplementary motor area hyperactivity during response inhibition: a candidate endophenotype of obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 169, 11001108.Google Scholar
Dodds, CM, Morein-Zamir, S, Robbins, TW (2011). Dissociating inhibition, attention, and response control in the frontoparietal network using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cerebral Cortex 21, 11551165.Google Scholar
Evans, DW, Lewis, MD, Iobst, E (2004). The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in normally developing compulsive-like behaviors and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain and Cognition 55, 220234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foa, EB, Huppert, JD, Leiberg, S, Langner, R, Kichic, R, Hajcak, G, Salkovskis, PM (2002). The obsessive-compulsive inventory: development and validation of a short version. Psychological Assessment 14, 485496.Google Scholar
Frith, CD, Friston, KJ, Herold, S, Silbersweig, D, Fletcher, P, Cahill, C, Dolan, RJ, Frackowiak, RS, Liddle, PF (1995). Regional brain activity in chronic schizophrenic patients during the performance of a verbal fluency task. British Journal of Psychiatry 167, 343349.Google Scholar
Goodman, WK, Price, LH, Rasmussen, SA, Mazure, C, Fleischmann, RL, Hill, CL, Heninger, GR, Charney, DS (1989). The yale-brown obsessive compulsive scale. I. Development, use, and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry 46, 10061011.Google Scholar
Gu, BM, Park, JY, Kang, DH, Lee, SJ, Yoo, SY, Jo, HJ, Choi, CH, Lee, JM, Kwon, JS (2008). Neural correlates of cognitive inflexibility during task-switching in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain 131, 155164.Google Scholar
Harle, KM, Shenoy, P, Stewart, JL, Tapert, SF, Yu, AJ, Paulus, MP (2014). Altered neural processing of the need to stop in young adults at risk for stimulant dependence. Journal of Neuroscience 34, 45674580.Google Scholar
Hart, H, Radua, J, Nakao, T, Mataix-Cols, D, Rubia, K (2013). Meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of inhibition and attention in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: exploring task-specific, stimulant medication, and age effects. JAMA Psychiatry 70, 185198.Google Scholar
Kang, DH, Jang, JH, Han, JY, Kim, JH, Jung, WH, Choi, JS, Choi, CH, Kwon, JS (2013). Neural correlates of altered response inhibition and dysfunctional connectivity at rest in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 40, 340346.Google Scholar
Kuelz, AK, Hohagen, F, Voderholzer, U (2004). Neuropsychological performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a critical review. Biological Psychology 65, 185236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maltby, N, Tolin, DF, Worhunsky, P, O'Keefe, TM, Kiehl, KA (2005). Dysfunctional action monitoring hyperactivates frontal-striatal circuits in obsessive-compulsive disorder: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 24, 495503.Google Scholar
Meiran, N, Diamond, GM, Toder, D, Nemets, B (2011). Cognitive rigidity in unipolar depression and obsessive compulsive disorder: examination of task switching, Stroop, working memory updating and post-conflict adaptation. Psychiatry Research 185, 149156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menzies, L, Achard, S, Chamberlain, SR, Fineberg, N, Chen, CH, del Campo, N, Sahakian, BJ, Robbins, TW, Bullmore, E (2007). Neurocognitive endophenotypes of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain 130, 32233236.Google Scholar
Menzies, L, Chamberlain, SR, Laird, AR, Thelen, SM, Sahakian, BJ, Bullmore, ET (2008). Integrating evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies of obsessive-compulsive disorder: the orbitofronto-striatal model revisited. Neuroscience Biobehavioural Reviews 32, 525549.Google Scholar
Milad, MR, Rauch, SL (2012). Obsessive-compulsive disorder: beyond segregated cortico-striatal pathways. Trends in Cognitive Science 16, 4351.Google Scholar
Miyake, A, Friedman, NP, Emerson, MJ, Witzki, AH, Howerter, A, Wager, TD (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘Frontal Lobe’ tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41, 49100.Google Scholar
Montgomery, SA, Asberg, M (1979). A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. British Journal of Psychiatry 134, 382389.Google Scholar
Morein-Zamir, S, Dodds, C, van Hartevelt, TJ, Schwarzkopt, W, Sahakian, BJ, Muller, U, Robbins, TW (2014). Hypoactivation in right inferior frontal cortex is specifically associated with motor response inhibition in adult ADHD. Human Brain Mapping 35, 51415152.Google Scholar
Morein-Zamir, S, Fineberg, NA, Robbins, TW, Sahakian, BJ (2010). Inhibition of thoughts and actions in obsessive-compulsive disorder: extending the endophenotype? Psychological Medicine 40, 263272.Google Scholar
Morein-Zamir, S, Papmeyer, M, Durieux, A, Fineberg, NA, Sahakian, BJ, Robbins, TW (2013 a). Investigation of attentional bias in obsessive compulsive disorder with and without depression in visual search. PLoS ONE 8, e80118.Google Scholar
Morein-Zamir, S, Papmeyer, M, Gillan, CM, Crockett, MJ, Fineberg, NA, Sahakian, BJ, Robbins, TW (2013 b). Punishment promotes response control deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder: evidence from a motivational go/no-go task. Psychological Medicine 43, 391400.Google Scholar
Morein-Zamir, S, Robbins, TW (2014). Fronto-striatal circuits in response-inhibition: relevance to addiction. Brain Research. Published online: 16 September 2014. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.09.012.Google Scholar
Morein-Zamir, S, Simon Jones, P, Bullmore, ET, Robbins, TW, Ersche, KD (2013 c). Prefrontal hypoactivity associated with impaired inhibition in stimulant-dependent individuals but evidence for hyperactivation in their unaffected siblings. Neuropsychopharmacology 38, 19451953.Google Scholar
Moritz, S, Hubner, M, Kluwe, R (2004). Task switching and backward inhibition in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 26, 677683.Google Scholar
Nakao, T, Nakagawa, A, Nakatani, E, Nabeyama, M, Sanematsu, H, Yoshiura, T, Togao, O, Tomita, M, Masuda, Y, Yoshioka, K, Kuroki, T, Kanba, S (2009). Working memory dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a neuropsychological and functional MRI study. Journal of Psychiatry Research 43, 784791.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakao, T, Nakagawa, A, Yoshiura, T, Nakatani, E, Nabeyama, M, Yoshizato, C, Kudoh, A, Tada, K, Yoshioka, K, Kawamoto, M (2005). A functional MRI comparison of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and normal controls during a Chinese character Stroop task. Psychiatry Research 139, 101114.Google Scholar
Nelson, HE (1982). The National Adult Reading Test Manual. NFER-Nelson: Windsor.Google Scholar
Page, LA, Rubia, K, Deeley, Q, Daly, E, Toal, F, Mataix-Cols, D, Giampietro, V, Schmitz, N, Murphy, DG (2009). A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of inhibitory control in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Research 174, 202209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Remijnse, PL, van den Heuvel, OA, Nielen, MM, Vriend, C, Hendriks, GJ, Hoogendijk, WJ, Uylings, HB, Veltman, DJ (2013). Cognitive inflexibility in obsessive-compulsive disorder and major depression is associated with distinct neural correlates. PLoS ONE 8, e59600.Google Scholar
Robbins, TW (2007). Shifting and stopping: fronto-striatal substrates, neurochemical modulation and clinical implications. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences 362, 917932.Google Scholar
Rotge, JY, Guehl, D, Dilharreguy, B, Cuny, E, Tignol, J, Bioulac, B, Allard, M, Burbaud, P, Aouizerate, B (2008). Provocation of obsessive-compulsive symptoms: a quantitative voxel-based meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 33, 405412.Google Scholar
Rotge, JY, Guehl, D, Dilharreguy, B, Tignol, J, Bioulac, B, Allard, M, Burbaud, P, Aouizerate, B (2009). Meta-analysis of brain volume changes in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry 65, 7583.Google Scholar
Roth, RM, Saykin, AJ, Flashman, LA, Pixley, HS, West, JD, Mamourian, AC (2007). Event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging of response inhibition in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry 62, 901909.Google Scholar
Rubia, K, Cubillo, A, Woolley, J, Brammer, MJ, Smith, A (2011). Disorder-specific dysfunctions in patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder compared to patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder during interference inhibition and attention allocation. Human Brain Mapping 32, 601611.Google Scholar
Rubia, K, Smith, AB, Brammer, MJ, Toone, B, Taylor, E (2005). Abnormal brain activation during inhibition and error detection in medication-naive adolescents with ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 10671075.Google Scholar
Shaw, P, Sharp, W, Sudre, G, Wharton, A, Greenstein, D, Raznahan, A, Evans, A, Chakravarty, MM, Lerch, JP, Rapoport, J (2015). Subcortical and cortical morphological anomalies as an endophenotype in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Molecular Psychiatry 20, 224231.Google Scholar
Sheehan, DV, Lecrubier, Y, Sheehan, KH, Amorim, P, Janavs, J, Weiller, E, Hergueta, T, Baker, R, Dunbar, GC (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 59 (Suppl. 20), 2233; quiz 34–57.Google Scholar
Shin, NY, Lee, TY, Kim, E, Kwon, JS (2014). Cognitive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 44, 11211130.Google Scholar
Snyder, HR, Kaiser, RH, Warren, SL, Heller, W (2015). Obsessive-compulsive disorder is associated with broad impairments in executive function: a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychological Science 3, 301330.Google Scholar
Tzourio-Mazoyer, N, Landeau, B, Papathanassiou, D, Crivello, F, Etard, O, Delcroix, N, Mazoyer, B, Joliot, M (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273289.Google Scholar
Ursu, S, Stenger, VA, Shear, MK, Jones, MR, Carter, CS (2003). Overactive action monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychological Science 14, 347353.Google Scholar
van den Heuvel, OA, Veltman, DJ, Groenewegen, HJ, Cath, DC, van Balkom, AJ, van Hartskamp, J, Barkhof, F, van Dyck, R (2005). Frontal-striatal dysfunction during planning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 62, 301309.Google Scholar
Wager, TD, Jonides, J, Reading, S (2004). Neuroimaging studies of shifting attention: a meta-analysis. NeuroImage 22, 16791693.Google Scholar
Watkins, LH, Sahakian, BJ, Robertson, MM, Veale, DM, Rogers, RD, Pickard, KM, Aitken, MR, Robbins, TW (2005). Executive function in Tourette's syndrome and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychological Medicine 35, 571582.Google Scholar
Weinberger, DR, Berman, KF (1996). Prefrontal function in schizophrenia: confounds and controversies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences 351, 14951503.Google Scholar
Whiteside, SP, Port, JD, Abramowitz, JS (2004). A meta–analysis of functional neuroimaging in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 132, 6979.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willcutt, EG, Doyle, AE, Nigg, JT, Faraone, SV, Pennington, BF (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry 57, 13361346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of OCD and control groups

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Whole-brain shifting-related activation with a threshold of p < 0.05 family-wise error corrected. (a) Illustration of fronto-parietal and occipital region activations in a group of healthy control participants. (b) Illustration of inferior parietal region activations in a group of obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) participants.

Figure 2

Table 2. Group differences in brain activation in whole-brain analyses, family-wise error corrected p < 0.05

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Areas commonly activated during stop and shift trial relative to go trials across all participants overlaid on the MNI brain. Images are displayed at x = 40, y = 8 and z = 38 in the sagittal, coronal and axial planes, respectively, with a voxel-wise threshold of p < 0.001 uncorrected. Colour bar represents t scores.

Figure 4

Fig. 3. Voxels associated with greater activation in obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) patients compared to controls when stopping but reduced activation in OCD patients compared to controls when shifting. (a) Areas showing this pattern of activation are displayed overlaid on the MNI brain. Images are displayed at x = −12, y = −6 and z = 16 in the sagittal, coronal and axial planes, respectively, with a voxel-wise threshold of p < 0.05 family-wise error corrected. Colour bars represent t scores. (b) Region-of-interest post-hoc analysis of activity for stop and shift trials in control and OCD patients groups in the caudate bilaterally. Error bars represent s.e.m..

Supplementary material: File

Morein-Zamir supplementary material

Morein-Zamir supplementary material 1

Download Morein-Zamir supplementary material(File)
File 202.9 KB