Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T14:07:21.167Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Developmental milestones in early childhood and genetic liability to neurodevelopmental disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 September 2021

Laurie J. Hannigan*
Affiliation:
Nic Waals Institute, Lovisenberg Diaconal Hospital, Oslo, Norway MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK Department of Mental Disorders, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway
Ragna Bugge Askeland
Affiliation:
Department of Mental Disorders, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway
Helga Ask
Affiliation:
Department of Mental Disorders, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway
Martin Tesli
Affiliation:
Department of Mental Disorders, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway NORMENT Centre, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Elizabeth Corfield
Affiliation:
Department of Mental Disorders, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway
Ziada Ayorech
Affiliation:
Nic Waals Institute, Lovisenberg Diaconal Hospital, Oslo, Norway MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
Per Magnus
Affiliation:
Centre for Fertility and Health, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway Institute of Health and Society, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Pål Rasmus Njølstad
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Science, KG Jebsen Center for Diabetes Research, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway Department of Pediatrics and Adolescents, Haukeland University Hospital, Bergen, Norway
Anne-Siri Øyen
Affiliation:
Nic Waals Institute, Lovisenberg Diaconal Hospital, Oslo, Norway Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway
Camilla Stoltenberg
Affiliation:
Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway Department of Global Public Health and Primary Care, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
Ole A. Andreassen
Affiliation:
NORMENT Centre, Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
Angelica Ronald
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Sciences, Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, University of London
George Davey Smith
Affiliation:
MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
Ted Reichborn-Kjennerud
Affiliation:
Department of Mental Disorders, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Alexandra Havdahl
Affiliation:
Nic Waals Institute, Lovisenberg Diaconal Hospital, Oslo, Norway MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK Department of Mental Disorders, Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway Department of Psychology, Promenta Research Center, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
*
Author for correspondence: Alexandra Havdahl, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Timing of developmental milestones, such as age at first walking, is associated with later diagnoses of neurodevelopmental disorders. However, its relationship to genetic risk for neurodevelopmental disorders in the general population is unknown. Here, we investigate associations between attainment of early-life language and motor development milestones and genetic liability to autism, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and schizophrenia.

Methods

We use data from a genotyped sub-set (N = 25699) of children in the Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort Study (MoBa). We calculate polygenic scores (PGS) for autism, ADHD, and schizophrenia and predict maternal reports of children's age at first walking, first words, and first sentences, motor delays (18 months), and language delays and a generalised measure of concerns about development (3 years). We use linear and probit regression models in a multi-group framework to test for sex differences.

Results

We found that ADHD PGS were associated with earlier walking age (β = −0.033, padj < 0.001) in both males and females. Additionally, autism PGS were associated with later walking (β = 0.039, padj = 0.006) in females only. No robust associations were observed for schizophrenia PGS or between any neurodevelopmental PGS and measures of language developmental milestone attainment.

Conclusions

Genetic liabilities for neurodevelopmental disorders show some specific associations with the age at which children first walk unsupported. Associations are small but robust and, in the case of autism PGS, differentiated by sex. These findings suggest that early-life motor developmental milestone attainment is associated with genetic liability to ADHD and autism in the general population.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Children's timely attainment of developmental milestones is an important indicator of the extent to which early neurodevelopment is progressing typically (Bishop, Thurm, Farmer, & Lord, Reference Bishop, Thurm, Farmer and Lord2016; Johnson, Gliga, Jones, & Charman, Reference Johnson, Gliga, Jones and Charman2015; Mayes & Calhoun, 2003). It is well established that both language and motor delays in early-life predict subsequent diagnoses of autism spectrum disorder (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Gliga, Jones and Charman2015; Jones, Gliga, Bedford, Charman, & Johnson, Reference Jones, Gliga, Bedford, Charman and Johnson2014; hereafter we use preferred term ‘autism’, as per Kenny et al., Reference Kenny, Hattersley, Molins, Buckley, Povey and Pellicano2015). Evidence is less consistent regarding associations between early developmental milestones and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Havmoeller, Thomsen, & Lemcke, Reference Havmoeller, Thomsen and Lemcke2018; Mitchell et al. Reference Mitchell, Brian, Zwaigenbaum, Roberts, Szatmari, Smith and Bryson2006), whereas delays in attaining early motor milestones do appear to be associated with diagnoses of schizophrenia later in life (Filatova et al., Reference Filatova, Koivumaa-Honkanen, Hirvonen, Freeman, Ivandic, Hurtig and Miettunen2017; Isohanni et al., Reference Isohanni, Jones, Moilanen, Rantakallio, Veijola, Oja and Järvelin2001). Given that each of these disorders has been shown to be substantially genetic in origin (ADHD: Demontis et al. Reference Demontis, Walters, Martin, Mattheisen, Als, Agerbo and Neale2019; Khan & Faraone, Reference Khan and Faraone2006; autism: Grove et al. Reference Grove, Ripke, Als, Mattheisen, Walters, Won and Børglum2019; Waye & Cheng, Reference Waye and Cheng2018; schizophrenia: Birnbaum & Weinberger, Reference Birnbaum and Weinberger2017; PGC Schizophrenia Working Group, 2014), it is plausible that links to early-life developmental milestones are underpinned by shared genes.

Autism is typically diagnosed during early childhood and is often (though not always) associated with language and motor difficulties. There is substantial evidence that delays in motor development (Harris, Reference Harris2017; West, Reference West2019) and language (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Gliga, Jones and Charman2015; Jones et al., Reference Jones, Gliga, Bedford, Charman and Johnson2014) predicts subsequent autism diagnoses. Autism shares some features with ADHD, another childhood-onset neurodevelopmental condition, but ADHD is typically diagnosed in mid-childhood or later (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Amminger, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Lee and Üstün2007). Delays in language development have been shown to predict ADHD diagnoses, but these may partly be accounted for by co-occurring autism (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Gliga, Jones and Charman2015). For early motor development, the directions of documented associations with ADHD are inconsistent (Havmoeller et al., Reference Havmoeller, Thomsen and Lemcke2018). Schizophrenia, although typically not diagnosed until early adulthood (Jones, Reference Jones2013), is also increasingly conceptualised as a neurodevelopmental disorder (Lewis & Levitt, Reference Lewis and Levitt2002). This conceptualisation is supported both by evidence of some clinical overlap with disorders such as autism and ADHD (De Crescenzo et al., Reference De Crescenzo, Postorino, Siracusano, Riccioni, Armando, Curatolo and Mazzone2019), and also by associations between delays in the attainment of early developmental milestones and later schizophrenia diagnoses (Filatova et al., Reference Filatova, Koivumaa-Honkanen, Hirvonen, Freeman, Ivandic, Hurtig and Miettunen2017; Isohanni et al., Reference Isohanni, Jones, Moilanen, Rantakallio, Veijola, Oja and Järvelin2001; Sørensen et al., Reference Sørensen, Mortensen, Schiffman, Reinisch, Maeda and Mednick2010; Stochl et al., Reference Stochl, Whittier, Wagner, Veijola, Jääskeläinen, Miettunen and Jones2019).

Individual differences in liability to neurodevelopmental disorders are partly genetic in origin (Lichtenstein, Carlström, Råstam, Gillberg, & Anckarsäter, Reference Lichtenstein, Carlström, Råstam, Gillberg and Anckarsäter2010). Although some of the genetic variants involved occur at low frequencies in the population, much of the genetic risk for neurodevelopmental disorders is conferred by a vast number of common single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) distributed across the genome. These SNPs have small effects individually, but cumulatively explain a substantial proportion of variance in the risk for neurodevelopmental disorders [SNP heritability of 45% in schizophrenia, 22% in ADHD and 12% in autism; (Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Agrawal, Bulik, Andreassen, Børglum and Breen2018)]. Recently, genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have begun to identify specific SNPs conferring increased risk for neurodevelopmental disorders. In the case of autism, the most recent GWAS (Grove et al., Reference Grove, Ripke, Als, Mattheisen, Walters, Won and Børglum2019) of ~18 000 cases and ~ 28 000 controls found five independent genome-wide significant loci, while for ADHD (Demontis et al., Reference Demontis, Walters, Martin, Mattheisen, Als, Agerbo and Neale2019; ~ 20 000 cases; ~ 35 000 controls) 12 independent loci were found. Sample sizes for both of these traits remain relatively small in GWAS terms, whereas schizophrenia is one of the most comprehensively studied psychiatric disorders in a GWAS context, with a sample size of more than 69,000 cases and 236,000 controls yielding 270 genome wide significant loci (The Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, Ripke, Walters, & O’Donovan, Reference Ripke, Walters and O'Donovan2020). By aggregating the effects of these loci, along with weaker effects of many other SNPs across the genome, into an individual-level polygenic score, it is possible to explore the manifestations of genetic risk for neurodevelopmental disorders in non-clinical populations (Wray et al., Reference Wray, Lee, Mehta, Vinkhuyzen, Dudbridge and Middeldorp2014).

It is plausible that shared genetic factors underpin observed links between neurodevelopmental disorders and early motor and language development, both of which are also heritable (Dale et al., Reference Dale, Simonoff, Bishop, Eley, Oliver, Price and Plomin1998; Smith et al., Reference Smith, van Jaarsveld, Llewellyn, Fildes, López Sánchez, Wardle and Fisher2017). This principle is demonstrated by evidence that rare protein-truncating genetic variants implicated in autism risk are associated with delayed walking and global developmental delays (Satterstrom et al., Reference Satterstrom, Kosmicki, Wang, Breen, De Rubeis, An and Walters2020). Polygenic approaches have only recently begun to be applied in this area (Thapar, Reference Thapar2020). Across two studies, Serdarevic and colleagues showed that genetic liability to schizophrenia (Serdarevic et al., Reference Serdarevic, Jansen, Ghassabian, White, Jaddoe, Posthuma and Tiemeier2018) and autism (but not ADHD; Serdarevic et al., Reference Serdarevic, Tiemeier, Jansen, Alemany, Xerxa, Neumann and Ghassabian2020) are associated with less advanced overall infant motor development measured between 2 and 5 months of age. A recent study in a sample of Japanese children reported a similar association between autism polygenic scores (PGS) and gross motor delay at 18 months, as well as a link with receptive (but not expressive) language delay (Takahashi et al., Reference Takahashi, Harada, Nishimura, Okumura, Choi, Iwabuchi and Tsuchiya2020). Despite the popularity of the ‘neurodevelopmental hypothesis’ of schizophrenia, and widespread application of schizophrenia PGS in analyses of early childhood behavioural measures (e.g. Jansen et al. Reference Jansen, Polderman, Bolhuis, Ende, Jaddoe, Verhulst and Tiemeier2018; Riglin et al. Reference Riglin, Collishaw, Richards, Thapar, Maughan, O'Donovan and Thapar2017), we are aware of no published analyses of schizophrenia PGS and early motor and language developmental milestones.

In the current study, we consider both typical and delayed motor and language development in the context of common genetic risk for autism, ADHD, and schizophrenia. We have two related aims. First, we aim to establish whether there are linear relationships between the extent of individuals' genetic ‘load’ of neurodevelopmental disorder-associated alleles and the age at which they first walk and talk. Second, we aim to establish whether there are probabilistic relationships between the same indices of genetic risk and the likelihood of motor or language delays during early childhood. With neurodevelopmental disorders diagnosed in males at a substantially higher rate than in females, we also test for sex differences in these relationships.

Methods

Participants and procedure

The Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort Study (MoBa; Magnus et al. Reference Magnus, Group, Irgens, Group, Haug, Group and Group2006, Reference Magnus, Birke, Vejrup, Haugan, Alsaker, Daltveit and Stoltenberg2016) is a population-based pregnancy cohort study conducted by the Norwegian Institute of Public Health. Participants were recruited from all over Norway from 1999 to 2008. The women consented to participation in 41% of the pregnancies. The cohort now includes 114 500 children, 95 200 mothers and 75 200 fathers. The current study is based on version 12 of the quality-assured data files released for research in January 2019.

The establishment of and data collection in MoBa was previously based on a license from the Norwegian Data Protection Agency and approval from The Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics and it is now based on regulations related to the Norwegian Health Registry Act. The current study was approved by The Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics (2016/1702).

Genotype data

In MoBa, blood samples were obtained from children (umbilical cord) at birth (Rønningen et al., Reference Rønningen, Paltiel, Meltzer, Nordhagen, Lie, Hovengen and Hoppin2006). At the time of writing, genotyping of the entire MoBa cohort is ongoing, and the available sample at the time of analysis was used. Quality control procedures used to process the genotype data are outlined in online Supplementary eMethods 1. After quality control and exclusions on the basis of sample overlap,Footnote Footnote 1 a core sample of ethnically homogeneous, unrelated children (N = 25·699) with a high genotype call-rate (>95%) and 7 141 482 SNPs at a minor allele frequency >1% were identified for use in our analysis.

Measures

Polygenic scores

We calculated PGS using PRSice2 (Choi & O'Reilly, Reference Choi and O'Reilly2019), as weighted sums of risk alleles per individual, with weights based on effect sizes in European samples in the most recent GWAS of, respectively, ADHD (Demontis et al., Reference Demontis, Walters, Martin, Mattheisen, Als, Agerbo and Neale2019), autism spectrum disorders (Grove et al., Reference Grove, Ripke, Als, Mattheisen, Walters, Won and Børglum2019; referred to here as autism), and schizophrenia (The Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, Ripke, Walters, & O'Donovan, Reference Ripke, Walters and O'Donovan2020). Separate PGS were calculated using 10 nested sets of SNPs, with a SNP included in the first score only if the p value of its association in the original GWAS was < 5 × 10−8 (the commonly applied ‘genome-wide significance threshold’), then in the next score if p < 1 × 10−6, and so on through a further eight thresholds (1 ×  10−5. 1 × 10−4. 0.001, 0.005, 0.01, 0.1, 0.5) culminating in one (p < 1) with all SNPs included. Although the specific thresholds are selected somewhat arbitrarily, the approach in general aims to maximise the polygenic signal by including increasing numbers of SNPs with increasingly weak associations, with the assumption that prediction will continue to increase until additional SNPs contribute only statistical noise to the score. All PGS were regressed on genotype batch and 10 principal components to account for possible structural artefacts in the data. Next, in order to have a single polygenic predictor per neurodevelopmental trait in the analyses, we extracted the first principal component from analyses of scores at all 10 thresholds for a given phenotype – an approach recently shown by Coombes, Ploner, Bergen, and Biernacka (Reference Coombes, Ploner, Bergen and Biernacka2020) to maximise prediction while reducing the risk of over-fitting. Full details of the parameters used in generating the PGS and correlations between the PGS at each threshold and the principal component score (henceforth: the PGS) used in these analyses are presented in online Supplementary eMethods 2.

Motor development

Mothers reported on the age (in months) at which their child first walked unsupported at two waves of data collection: when children were 18 months old and 3 years old. We constructed a single age-at-first-walking variable for analysis by taking available data from either the 18-month or 3-year-old version if only one was available, and by calculating a mean (rounded to the nearest month) if both were available [these measures are highly correlated in the full MoBa sample (r = 0.84, n = 50 940)]. Values considered to be implausible (i.e. children reported as walking at <6 months) were excluded.

In addition to the continuous age-at-first-walking measure, we derived a binary measure relating to the presence or absence of motor delays. This was based on three items included in the questionnaire completed by mothers when their children were 18 months old. The first simply asked whether or not children were yet able to walk unaided, and two items from the Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ; Squires, LaWanda, and Bricker, Reference Squires, LaWanda and Bricker1999); for validation of the Norwegian version see: Richter and Janson, Reference Richter and Janson2007) asked mothers to report whether their child ‘can walk well and seldom fall’, ‘moves around by walking, rather than by crawling on his/her hands and knees’. If mothers responded No/Not yet to any of these items, we coded the child as having signs of delayed motor abilities at 18 months. Details of internal validation checks of this derived measure are presented in online Supplementary eMethods 3.

Language development

When the children were 5 years old, mothers reported on the age (in months) at which their child used their first single words and, separately, their first sentences (putting together 2–3 words). Values considered to be implausible (i.e. children reported as talking at <6 months) were excluded. By subtracting children's age at first words from their age at first sentence use, we also calculated a variable indexing the rate of language learning.

In addition to the continuous language development measures, we also constructed a binary measure relating to the presence or absence of delays in language abilities. This variable flagged children as having potential language delays if, when children were 3 years old, their mothers reported either that they were ‘not yet’ making ‘sentences that are three or four words long’ (an item from the ASQ) or if they reported children as either ‘Not yet talking’, ‘… talking, but you can't understand him/her’, ‘Talking in one-word utterances, such as milk or down’, or ‘Talking in 2- to 3-word phrases, such as me got ball or give doll’ (as opposed to either using ‘fairly complete’ or ‘long and complicated’ sentences). This item comes from (and has been validated in) a UK-based twin sample (Dale, Price, Bishop, & Plomin, Reference Dale, Price, Bishop and Plomin2003). Details of internal validation checks of this derived measure are presented in online Supplementary eMethods 3.

To augment the measures addressing motor and language development in specific terms, we included one additional binary variable from the questionnaire administered when children were 3 years old. This was a question asking mothers to report (‘Yes’/”No”) whether ‘others (family, nursery, health visitor) have expressed concerns about your child's development’.

Assessment for bias and selection effects

We investigated two selection-related biases in our analyses. First, it is possible that there are selection effects associated with inclusion in the early batches of MoBa genotyping, which are enhanced for complete trios. We quantified this by comparing the genotyped sub-sample with the remainder of the full MoBa sample on the phenotypic measures from the main analyses. Second, selective attrition (where drop-out in longitudinal studies is predicted by analysis-relevant variables) also represents a potential source of bias. We quantified this by assessing the extent to which provision of data at each wave was associated with individuals' PGS for ADHD, autism, and schizophrenia.

Statistical methods

Linear regression in multi-group framework

For the continuous variables (age at first walking, age at first words, age at first sentences, rate of language development) we fitted linear regression models in a multi-group framework, with sex as a grouping variable and the various PGS as predictors (one PGS per model). These models were then systematically constrained across sex (with first only the regression coefficients equated across sex, then both the regression coefficients and the intercepts equated across sex, then all model parameters equated). These nested models were compared using χ2 difference tests.

Probit regression in multi-group framework

For the binary variables (motor delays at 18 months; language delays at 3 years; concern expressed by others by 3 years), we fitted probit regression models in in a multi-group framework, with sex as a grouping variable and the various PGS as predictors (one PGS per model). As with the linear regressions, models were constrained to test for sex differences, and nested models compared using χ2 difference tests.

Multiple testing

The strength of the evidence for the PGS association in the context of multiple tests being performed was evaluated by using three alpha levels as benchmarks: an uncorrected 0.05 alpha level, an alpha level corrected using the Benjamini−Hochberg method to control the false-discovery rate, and a conservative Bonferroni corrected alpha level of 0.05/42 = 0.001, where 42 corresponds to the maximum possible number of effects of interest from the main analyses (3 PGS×7 outcomes×2 groups).

Software and analytic code

All modelling was carried out in R version 3.4.4 using the lavaan package (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012). For the linear regression models, a full information maximum likelihood (FIML) approach was used, meaning that all available data contributed to the model. Maximum likelihood estimation is not available for the probit regression models, meaning that a robust weighted least squares estimator (WLSMV in lavaan) with listwise deletion was used. Other R packages used in the project are listed in with citations in online Supplementary eMethods 4.

All analytic code is openly available at https://github.com/psychgen/ndd-prs-milestones-moba. The consent given by the participants does not allow for storage of data on an individual level in repositories or journals. Researchers can apply for access to data for replication purposes via MoBa, in line with their data access policies.

Descriptive statistics

Descriptive statistics for the main study variables among genotyped individuals are shown, for males and females separately, in Table 1. Males and females differed minimally in terms of their motor development. On average, males spoke their first words [t = 6.047 (3234.5), p < 0.001] and sentences [t = 7.159 (3152.1), p < 0.001] later than females, progressing from words to sentences at a slower rate [t = 3.995 (3043.6), p < 0.001] and were more likely to be reported as having language delays [χ2 = 120.13 (1), p < 0.001]. Additionally, mothers of boys were also more likely to report that others had expressed concern about their child's development [χ2 = 75.966 (1), p < 0.001, see online Supplementary eTable 1 for full details of cross-sex comparisons on main study variables). The correlations between PGS were ADHD-autism: 0.21 (95% confidence intervals (CIs) 0.20–0.22); ADHD-SCZ: 0.10 (0.09–0.11); and autism-SCZ: 0.01 (0.00–0.03).

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for measures of motor and language development between the ages of 18 months and 5 years in the analytic sub-sample of MoBa

Note – The difference in data availability for the age at first words variable compared to other measures in Table 1 is due to the version of the 5-year questionnaire containing the measure was only sent to a subset of MoBa participants. To check for relevant systematic differences between those who did v. did not receive the age at first words questionnaire at the 5-year wave, we compared the proportion of language delays at 3 years in these two groups, finding minimal differences [0.039 v. 0.030, χ2 = 4.968 (1), p = 0.030].

Selection and selective attrition

Results from our analyses of selection and selective attrition are presented in online Supplementary eTables 2–4. We found some evidence of selection bias associated with having genotype data available, but limited evidence of ongoing selective attrition associated with neurodevelopmental PGS.

Main analyses

The results of the main analyses are presented in Fig. 1. In the figure, standardised beta coefficients from the sex stratified and best-fitting constrained linear/probit models are plotted for each outcome variable regressed on each PGS. The size and opacity of the points indicate which model was preferred in each analysis (sex-stratified or male/females together). The CIs in Fig. 1 correspond to the uncorrected 0.05 alpha level.

Fig. 1. Results from regression models testing effects of PGS for ADHD, Autism, and schizophrenia on the attainment of motor and language developmental milestones.

Note: estimates from both sex-stratified and constrained models are presented for outcome; the presence of 95% CI bars and darker fill intensity indicate which model provided a better fit to the data; estimates are presented only for the PGS threshold which maximised R 2 in the best-fitting model

In the motor development analyses (Fig. 1; top panel), we found robust evidence that ADHD PGS is associated with younger age at first walking (β = −0.033, Bonferroni-adjusted p < 0.001) an association that was present in both males and females. Additionally, we observed robust evidence for an association of autism PGS with age at first walking in females only: β = 0.039, Bonferroni-adjusted p = 0.006). We observed no robust evidence of any association of these PGS on the measure of motor delays at 18 months, nor of schizophrenia PGS being associated with either measure of motor development. Similarly, we found no robust evidence of associations between any neurodevelopmental PGS and outcomes in the language or general development domains. Full model-fitting results for these analyses are presented in online Supplementary eTables 5, 6 and 7, with parameter estimates and raw/adjusted p values from best-fitting models in online Supplementary eTable 8.

Effect sizes

Figure 2 shows the relationships between the three neurodevelopmental PGS and, respectively, children's age at first walking, first words, first use of sentences, and rate of language development. The observed associations for ADHD and autism PGS on age at first walking (panel A) were small: a 1SD increase in ADHD polygenic burden was associated with a few days' reduction in the age at which children first walked, while a similar increase in autism PGS translated to later walking by a few days in girls only.

Fig. 2. Neurodevelopmental PGS and continuous measures of motor and language development from the MoBa sample.

Note: Y-axes differ but range remains of constant size to facilitate comparison of effect sizes.

Post hoc sensitivity analyses

To aid interpretation of the results from the main analyses we ran a series of post hoc sensitivity analyses. Firstly, we re-ran analyses using a PGS for a non-neurodevelopmental condition (major depressive disorder; Howard et al., Reference Howard, Adams, Shirali, Clarke, Marioni, Davies and McIntosh2018)) as a negative control, finding no associations with any of the outcome measures in the study (see online Supplementary eTable 9). Next, we sought to investigate the extent to which the observed associations were driven by individuals who went on to receive a diagnosis of either ADHD or autism. Based on information from the Norwegian Patient Registry, we identified and excluded 1689 individuals in our analytic sample who had received a diagnosis code in the F90 sub-chapter of ICD-10 (‘hyperkinetic disorders’) and 340 individuals F84 sub-chapter (‘pervasive developmental disorders’) as of 2018. This left an analytic sample of N = 23 852, and the pattern of results was unchanged in this sample (see online Supplementary eTable 10). Finally, to account for the possibility that an unknown number of individuals from a particular city of Norway within our analytic sample may have had parents who were included in the ADHD GWAS used as the basis of the PGS and that this potential sample overlap could have biased results, we re-ran the analyses with all individuals from this region (Hordaland, including the city of Bergen) excluded. This reduced the analytic sample size to N = 22 337 and, again, results from this sensitivity analyses were essentially unchanged from the main analyses (see online Supplementary eTable 11).

Discussion

We investigated the extent to which timely attainment of early-life developmental milestones was associated with genetic liability to neurodevelopmental disorders. The most robust evidence was observed for an association between genetic liability to ADHD and earlier walking in both boys and girls, and genetic liability to autism and later walking in girls only. We found no robust evidence for associations between genetic risk for neurodevelopmental disorders and language milestone attainment in early life.

ADHD PGS were associated with earlier walking in our sample, in both boys and girls. Our sensitivity analyses showed that these results were not driven exclusively by the presence of individuals who would go on to receive diagnoses of ADHD and related disorders. This suggests that liability for ADHD manifests broadly, in ways that may be neither directly related to ADHD symptomatology nor even necessarily functionally impairing at all, from very early in life. It should be noted that our finding regarding ADHD genetic liability and earlier walking is partly consistent with results from a study in the Danish National Birth Cohort (Lemcke, Parner, Bjerrum, Thomsen, & Lauritsen, Reference Lemcke, Parner, Bjerrum, Thomsen and Lauritsen2016), which found an over-representation of early-walkers among ADHD-diagnosed children. However, unlike that study, we did not find evidence of any non-linearity in this relationship (late-walkers were also over-represented in ADHD-diagnosed children in Lemcke et al., Reference Lemcke, Parner, Bjerrum, Thomsen and Lauritsen2016) – and further, we show through our sensitivity analyses that this finding is robust among children who do not receive ADHD diagnoses.

We also observed associations between autism PGS and age at first walking. In contrast to those observed for ADHD PGS, these were sex-specific – appearing only in girls – and in the opposite direction, with higher PGS associated with later walking. This reversal of the direction of the effects is noteworthy given the positive (r = 0.21) correlation between ADHD and autism PGS. The finding appears to be consistent with the notion that there is a genetic basis to commonly observed associations between delays in motor development and autism (Harris, Reference Harris2017; West, Reference West2019). However, as with the ADHD PGS results, we confirmed with post hoc sensitivity analyses that this pattern was not entirely driven by autism cases within the sample. Moreover, the reason for the sex differentiated nature of the association is unclear. Diagnostic bias in autism may mean that female manifestations are less recognised clinically (Loomes, Hull, & Mandy, Reference Loomes, Hull and Mandy2017). Additionally, motor delays may be a more common early sign in autistic girls compared to autistic boys (Gabis, Attia, Roth-Hanania, & Foss-Feig, Reference Gabis, Attia, Roth-Hanania and Foss-Feig2020). In terms of genetic evidence, rare high-impact de novo risk variants for autism are associated both with female sex and later age of walking in autism case samples (Havdahl et al., Reference Havdahl, Niarchou, Starnawska, Uddin, van der Merwe and Warrier2021; Satterstrom et al., Reference Satterstrom, Kosmicki, Wang, Breen, De Rubeis, An and Walters2020). Overall, further studies are needed to establish whether, as is apparent in our results, a similar pattern exists for common inherited genetic risk for autism in the general population – and the extent to which this may be consequential for sex differentiation in clinical manifestations of autism.

We did not find any evidence for associations between neurodevelopmental PGS and measures of the timeliness with which early language milestones were attained. This absence-of-evidence should not be taken as evidence that such effects do not exist, and it should be noted that, for practical reasons detailed further in the limitations section below, none of our analyses using measures of language milestone attainment were as well powered as those using the age-at-first walking outcome. Nonetheless, it is possible that manifestations of genetic liability for neurodevelopmental disorders either emerge later in the domain of language development than for motor development, or are more observable as specific difficulties rather than delayed attainment (as we have shown in this sample elsewhere: see Askeland et al., Reference Askeland, Hannigan, Ask, Ayorech, Tesli, Corfield and Havdahl2020).

There are some limitations to this work. First, age at first walking and talking were based on maternal recall. Age of walking has been demonstrated to have high reliability when relying on parental recall (Langendonk et al., Reference Langendonk, van Beijsterveldt, Brouwer, Stroet, Hudziak and Boomsma2007) and was assessed on two occasions here. However, age of first word/sentence use was assessed only at 5 years, only in a subset of the sample, and may be more imprecise (Majnemer & Rosenblatt, Reference Majnemer and Rosenblatt1994) or subject to bias (Hus, Taylor, & Lord, Reference Hus, Taylor and Lord2011). It is also possible that discrepancies between reported and actual ages of milestone attainment are not simply due to measurement error, but also recall bias. For example, a child who is more active at the time when the mothers fill in the questionnaire may be recalled as having walked earlier. Second, the sample was subject to some selection associated with study variables. Like all cohort studies, MoBa is more successful at retaining participants with who are healthier and more educated, and this is reflected in the polygenic liabilities of those who responded on the measures of developmental milestones used in these analyses. We cannot rule out some bias in the results as a consequence of this selection bias. Finally, limitations exist around both GWAS and polygenic scoring approaches, and these apply to this work and are no less important for being well-documented in the literature (Tam et al., Reference Tam, Patel, Turcotte, Bossé, Paré and Meyre2019; Wray et al., Reference Wray, Yang, Hayes, Price, Goddard and Visscher2013). In particular, we note that strong conclusions from direct comparisons across different PGS should be avoided given the differences in the power of the original GWAS on which the PGS are based (as well as in the polygenicity and SNP-heritability of the traits).

Conclusions

Overall, our findings suggest that common genetic variants associated with neurodevelopmental disorders are also linked to the attainment of early motor developmental milestones. We find no robust evidence for similar relationships with language developmental milestones. The associations did not appear to be driven by clinical cases within the sample, suggesting that polygenic liability for neurodevelopmental disorders manifests – at least in respect of the attainment of early motor developmental milestones – from very early in life in the general population.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003330

Acknowledgement

The authors are grateful to Elise Robinson and Somer Bishop for helpful comments on an earlier version of this work.

Financial support

LJH and AH were supported by the South-Eastern Norway Regional Health Authority (grants 2018058, 2018059, 2020022). RBA and EC was supported by the Research Council of Norway (274611). AH was supported by the Research Council of Norway (288083). GDS works in the Medical Research Council Integrative Epidemiology Unit at the University of Bristol, which is supported by the Medical Research Council (MC_UU_00011/1). OAA was supported the Research Council of Norway (229129; 213837; 248778; 223273; 249711); the South-East Norway Regional Health Authority (2017–112); KG Jebsen Stiftelsen (SKGJ) and H2020 grant # 847776 CoMorMent. The Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort Study is supported by the Norwegian Ministry of Health and Care Services and the Ministry of Education and Research. We are grateful to all the participating families in Norway who take part in this on-going cohort study.

We thank the Norwegian Institute of Public Health (NIPH) for generating high-quality genomic data. This research is part of the HARVEST collaboration, supported by the Research Council of Norway (#229624). We also thank the NORMENT Centre for providing genotype data, funded by the Research Council of Norway (#223273), South East Norway Health Authority and KG Jebsen Stiftelsen. We further thank the Center for Diabetes Research, the University of Bergen for providing genotype data and performing quality control and imputation of the data funded by the ERC AdG project SELECTionPREDISPOSED, Stiftelsen Kristian Gerhard Jebsen, Trond Mohn Foundation, the Research Council of Norway, the Novo Nordisk Foundation, the University of Bergen, and the Western Norway health Authorities (Helse Vest).

Conflict of interest

OAA is a consultant for HealthLytix. No other potential conflicts are declared by any of the authors.

Footnotes

The notes appear after the main text.

1 Individuals from MoBa who were identified as having been included in one of the GWAS on which the calculation of the polygenic scores were based (or being children of a parent who had been) were excluded. For one potential source of overlap, only the geographical region of Norway from which MoBa children' parents had potentially been included in the ADHD GWAS was known, so individuals in the analytic sample from this region were removed as a sensitivity analysis.

References

Askeland, R. B., Hannigan, L. J., Ask, H., Ayorech, Z., Tesli, M., Corfield, E., … Havdahl, A. (2020). Early manifestations of genetic risk for neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. Preprint available at https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/qbvw8.Google Scholar
Birnbaum, R., & Weinberger, D. R. (2017). Genetic insights into the neurodevelopmental origins of schizophrenia. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(12), 727740. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2017.125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, S. L., Thurm, A., Farmer, C., & Lord, C. (2016). Autism spectrum disorder, intellectual disability, and delayed walking. Pediatrics, 137(3), e20152959e20152959. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2015-2959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, S. W., & O'Reilly, P. F. (2019). PRSice-2: Polygenic risk score software for biobank-scale data. GigaScience, 8(7), giz082. https://doi.org/10.1093/gigascience/giz082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coombes, B. J., Ploner, A., Bergen, S. E., & Biernacka, J. M. (2020). A principal component approach to improve association testing with polygenic risk scores. Genetic Epidemiology, 44(7), 676686. https://doi.org/10.1002/gepi.22339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, P., Simonoff, E., Bishop, D., Eley, T., Oliver, B., Price, T., … Plomin, R. (1998). Genetic influence on language delay in two-year-old children. Nature Neuroscience, 1(4), 324328. https://doi.org/10.1038/1142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, P. S., Price, T. S., Bishop, D. V. M., & Plomin, R. (2003). Outcomes of early language delay: I. Predicting persistent and transient language difficulties at 3 and 4 years. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research: JSLHR, 46(3), 544560. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2003/044).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Crescenzo, F., Postorino, V., Siracusano, M., Riccioni, A., Armando, M., Curatolo, P., & Mazzone, L. (2019). Autistic symptoms in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10(FEB), 111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demontis, D., Walters, R. K., Martin, J., Mattheisen, M., Als, T. D., Agerbo, E., … Neale, B. M. (2019). Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nature Genetics, 51(1), 6375. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0269-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Filatova, S., Koivumaa-Honkanen, H., Hirvonen, N., Freeman, A., Ivandic, I., Hurtig, T., … Miettunen, J. (2017). Early motor developmental milestones and schizophrenia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 188, 1320. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2017.01.029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabis, L. V., Attia, O. L., Roth-Hanania, R., & Foss-Feig, J. (2020). Motor delay − An early and more common ‘red flag’ in girls rather than boys with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 104, 103702. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2020.103702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grove, J., Ripke, S., Als, T. D., Mattheisen, M., Walters, R. K., Won, H., … Børglum, A. D. (2019). Identification of common genetic risk variants for autism spectrum disorder. Nature Genetics, 51(3), 431444. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-019-0344-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, S. R. (2017). Early motor delays as diagnostic clues in autism spectrum disorder. European Journal of Pediatrics, 176(9), 12591262. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00431-017-2951-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Havdahl, A., Niarchou, M., Starnawska, A., Uddin, M., van der Merwe, C., & Warrier, V. (2021). Genetic contributions to autism spectrum disorder. Psychological Medicine, 26, 114. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721000192.Google Scholar
Havmoeller, S. R., Thomsen, P. H., & Lemcke, S. (2018). The early motor development in children diagnosed with ADHD: A systematic review. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 11(3), 233240. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12402-018-0280-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howard, D. M., Adams, M. J., Shirali, M., Clarke, T.-K., Marioni, R. E., Davies, G., … McIntosh, A. M. (2018). Genome-wide association study of depression phenotypes in UK biobank identifies variants in excitatory synaptic pathways. Nature Communications, 9(1), 1470. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-03819-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hus, V., Taylor, A., & Lord, C. (2011). Telescoping of caregiver report on the autism diagnostic interview – revised: Telescoping on the ADI-R. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(7), 753760. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02398.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isohanni, M., Jones, P. B., Moilanen, K., Rantakallio, P., Veijola, J., Oja, H., … Järvelin, M. R. (2001). Early developmental milestones in adult schizophrenia and other psychoses. A 31–year follow-up of the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort. Schizophrenia Research, 52(1–2), 119. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00179-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jansen, P. R., Polderman, T. J. C., Bolhuis, K., Ende, J., Jaddoe, V. W. V., Verhulst, F. C., … Tiemeier, H. (2018). Polygenic scores for schizophrenia and educational attainment are associated with behavioural problems in early childhood in the general population. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59(1), 3947. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12759.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M. H., Gliga, T., Jones, E. J. H., & Charman, T. (2015). Annual research review: Infant development, autism, and ADHD – early pathways to emerging disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 56(3), 228247. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, E. J. H., Gliga, T., Bedford, R., Charman, T., & Johnson, M. H. (2014). Developmental pathways to autism: A review of prospective studies of infants at risk. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 39, 133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.12.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, P. B. (2013). Adult mental health disorders and their age at onset. British Journal of Psychiatry, 202(s54), s5s10. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.112.119164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenny, L., Hattersley, C., Molins, B., Buckley, C., Povey, C., & Pellicano, E. (2015). Which terms should be used to describe autism? Perspectives from the UK autism community: Autism. London, England: Sage UK. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361315588200.Google ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Amminger, G. P., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Lee, S., & Üstün, T. B. (2007). Age of onset of mental disorders: A review of recent literature. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 20(4), 359364. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0b013e32816ebc8c.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khan, S. A., & Faraone, S. V. (2006). The genetics of ADHD: A literature review of 2005. Current Psychiatry Reports, 8(5), 393397. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-006-0042-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langendonk, J. M., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., Brouwer, S. I., Stroet, T., Hudziak, J. J., & Boomsma, D. I. (2007). Assessment of motor milestones in twins. Twin Research and Human Genetics: The Official Journal of the International Society for Twin Studies, 10(6), 835839. https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.10.6.835.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemcke, S., Parner, E. T., Bjerrum, M., Thomsen, P. H., & Lauritsen, M. B. (2016). Early development in children that are later diagnosed with disorders of attention and activity: A longitudinal study in the Danish National Birth Cohort. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 25(10), 10551066. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-016-0825-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, D. A., & Levitt, P. (2002). Schizophrenia as a disorder of neurodevelopment. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 25(1), 409432. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142754.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lichtenstein, P., Carlström, E., Råstam, M., Gillberg, C., & Anckarsäter, H. (2010). The genetics of autism spectrum disorders and related neuropsychiatric disorders in childhood. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(11), 13571363. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10020223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loomes, R., Hull, L., & Mandy, W. P. L. (2017). What is the male-to-female ratio in autism spectrum disorder? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(6), 466474. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.03.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magnus, P., Birke, C., Vejrup, K., Haugan, A., Alsaker, E., Daltveit, A. K., … Stoltenberg, C. (2016). Cohort profile update: The Norwegian mother and child cohort study (MoBa). International Journal of Epidemiology, 45(2), 382388. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyw029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magnus, P., Group, T. M. S., Irgens, L. M., Group, T. M. S., Haug, K., Group, T. M. S., … Group, T. M. S. (2006). Cohort profile: The Norwegian mother and child cohort study (MoBa). International Journal of Epidemiology, 35(5), 11461150. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyl170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Majnemer, A., & Rosenblatt, B. (1994). Reliability of parental recall of developmental milestones. Pediatric Neurology, 10(4), 304308. https://doi.org/10.1016/0887-8994(94)90126-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayes, S. D., & Calhoun, S. L. (2003). Ability profiles in children with autism. Autism, 7(1), 6580. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361303007001006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, S., Brian, J., Zwaigenbaum, L., Roberts, W., Szatmari, P., Smith, I., … Bryson, S. (2006). Early language and communication development of infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(2 SUPPL. 2), S69S78. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004703-200604002-00004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
PGC Schizophrenia Working Group (2014). Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature, 511(7510), 421427. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature13595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richter, J., & Janson, H. (2007). A validation study of the Norwegian version of the ages and stages questionnaires. Acta Paediatrica, 96(5), 748752. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.2007.00246.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riglin, L., Collishaw, S., Richards, A., Thapar, A. K., Maughan, B., O'Donovan, M. C., & Thapar, A. (2017). Schizophrenia risk alleles and neurodevelopmental outcomes in childhood: A population-based cohort study. The Lancet Psychiatry, 4(1), 5762. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30406-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
The Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, Ripke, S., Walters, J. T., & O'Donovan, M. C. (2020). Mapping genomic loci prioritises genes and implicates synaptic biology in schizophrenia. MedRxiv, Preprint. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.09.12.20192922.Google Scholar
Rønningen, K. S., Paltiel, L., Meltzer, H. M., Nordhagen, R., Lie, K. K., Hovengen, R., … Hoppin, J. A. (2006). The biobank of the Norwegian mother and child cohort study: A resource for the next 100 years. European Journal of Epidemiology, 21(8), 619625. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10654-006-9041-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 136. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i02.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Satterstrom, F. K., Kosmicki, J. A., Wang, J., Breen, M. S., De Rubeis, S., An, J.-Y., … Walters, R. K. (2020). Large-scale exome sequencing study implicates both developmental and functional changes in the neurobiology of autism. Cell, 180(3), 568584.e23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2019.12.036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Serdarevic, F., Jansen, P. R., Ghassabian, A., White, T., Jaddoe, V. W. V., Posthuma, D., & Tiemeier, H. (2018). Association of genetic risk for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder with infant neuromotor development. JAMA Psychiatry, 75(1), 9696. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.3459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Serdarevic, F., Tiemeier, H., Jansen, P. R., Alemany, S., Xerxa, Y., Neumann, A., … Ghassabian, A. (2020). Polygenic risk scores for developmental disorders, neuromotor functioning during infancy, and autistic traits in childhood. Biological Psychiatry, 87(2), 132138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.06.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, L., van Jaarsveld, C. H. M., Llewellyn, C. H., Fildes, A., López Sánchez, G. F., Wardle, J., & Fisher, A. (2017). Genetic and environmental influences on developmental milestones and movement: Results from the Gemini cohort study. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 88(4), 401407. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2017.1373268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sørensen, H. J., Mortensen, E. L., Schiffman, J., Reinisch, J. M., Maeda, J., & Mednick, S. A. (2010). Early developmental milestones and risk of schizophrenia: A 45-year follow-up of the Copenhagen perinatal cohort. Schizophrenia Research, 118(1–3), 4147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2010.01.029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squires, J., LaWanda, P., & Bricker, D. (1999). The ASQ User's Guide. In ASQ User's Guide.Google Scholar
Stochl, J., Whittier, A., Wagner, A. P., Veijola, J., Jääskeläinen, E., Miettunen, J., … Jones, P. B. (2019). Association between developmental milestones and age of schizophrenia onset: Results from the northern Finland birth cohort 1966. Schizophrenia Research, 208, 228234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2019.02.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, P. F., Agrawal, A., Bulik, C. M., Andreassen, O. A., Børglum, A. D., Breen, G., … for the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. (2018). Psychiatric genomics: An update and an agenda. American Journal of Psychiatry, 175(1), 1527. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17030283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takahashi, N., Harada, T., Nishimura, T., Okumura, A., Choi, D., Iwabuchi, T., … Tsuchiya, K. J. (2020). Association of genetic risks with autism Spectrum disorder and early neurodevelopmental delays among children without intellectual disability. JAMA Network Open, 3(2), e1921644. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.21644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tam, V., Patel, N., Turcotte, M., Bossé, Y., Paré, G., & Meyre, D. (2019). Benefits and limitations of genome-wide association studies. Nature Reviews Genetics, 20(8), 467484. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41576-019-0127-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thapar, A. (2020). Infant neuromotor development: An early indicator of genetic liability for neurodevelopmental disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 87(2), 9394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.10.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waye, M. M. Y., & Cheng, H. Y. (2018). Genetics and epigenetics of autism: A review. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 72(4), 228244. https://doi.org/10.1111/pcn.12606.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West, K. L. (2019). Infant motor development in autism spectrum disorder: A synthesis and meta-analysis. Child Development, 90(6), 20532070. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wray, N. R., Lee, S. H., Mehta, D., Vinkhuyzen, A. A. E., Dudbridge, F., & Middeldorp, C. M. (2014). Research review: Polygenic methods and their application to psychiatric traits. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(10), 10681087. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wray, N. R., Yang, J., Hayes, B. J., Price, A. L., Goddard, M. E., & Visscher, P. M. (2013). Pitfalls of predicting complex traits from SNPs. Nature Reviews Genetics, 14(7), 507515. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg3457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for measures of motor and language development between the ages of 18 months and 5 years in the analytic sub-sample of MoBa

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Results from regression models testing effects of PGS for ADHD, Autism, and schizophrenia on the attainment of motor and language developmental milestones.Note: estimates from both sex-stratified and constrained models are presented for outcome; the presence of 95% CI bars and darker fill intensity indicate which model provided a better fit to the data; estimates are presented only for the PGS threshold which maximised R2 in the best-fitting model

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Neurodevelopmental PGS and continuous measures of motor and language development from the MoBa sample.Note: Y-axes differ but range remains of constant size to facilitate comparison of effect sizes.

Supplementary material: File

Hannigan et al. supplementary material

Hannigan et al. supplementary material

Download Hannigan et al. supplementary material(File)
File 60.5 KB