Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T14:11:27.929Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cumulative stressor exposure predicts menstrual cycle affective changes in a transdiagnostic outpatient sample with past-month suicidal ideation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 October 2024

Anisha Nagpal*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Illinois Chicago, USA
Jordan C. Barone
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Illinois Chicago, USA
Hafsah Tauseef
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Illinois Chicago, USA
Jaclyn Ross
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Illinois Chicago, USA
Zach J. Gray
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Science, University of Arkansas, USA
Katja M. Schmalenberger
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Illinois Chicago, USA
Grant Shields
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Science, University of Arkansas, USA
George M. Slavich
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California, Los Angeles, USA
Tory Eisenlohr-Moul
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Illinois Chicago, USA
*
Corresponding author: Anisha Nagpal; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Affective responses to the menstrual cycle vary widely. Some individuals experience severe symptoms like those with premenstrual dysphoric disorder, while others have minimal changes. The reasons for these differences are unclear, but prior studies suggest stressor exposure may play a role. However, research in at-risk psychiatric samples is lacking.

Methods

In a large clinical sample, we conducted a prospective study of how lifetime stressors relate to degree of affective change across the cycle. 114 outpatients with past-month suicidal ideation (SI) provided daily ratings (n = 6187) of negative affect and SI across 1–3 menstrual cycles. Participants completed the Stress and Adversity Inventory (STRAIN), which measures different stressor exposures (i.e. interpersonal loss, physical danger) throughout the life course, including before and after menarche. Multilevel polynomial growth models tested the relationship between menstrual cycle time and symptoms, moderated by stressor exposure.

Results

Greater lifetime stressor exposure predicted a more pronounced perimenstrual increase in active SI, along with marginally significant similar patterns for negative affect and passive SI. Additionally, pre-menarche stressors significantly increased the cyclicity of active SI compared to post-menarche stressors. Exposure to more interpersonal loss stressors predicted greater perimenstrual symptom change of negative affect, passive SI and active SI. Exploratory item-level analyses showed that lifetime stressors moderated a more severe perimenstrual symptom trajectory for mood swings, anger/irritability, rejection sensitivity, and interpersonal conflict.

Conclusion

These findings suggest that greater lifetime stressor exposure may lead to heightened emotional reactivity to ovarian hormone fluctuations, elevating the risk of psychopathology.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

While most individuals assigned female at birth (AFAB) do not experience notable menstrual cycle affective change (MCAC), a prominent minority experience moderate to severe MCAC that causes distress or impairment (Gehlert, Song, Chang, & Hartlage, Reference Gehlert, Song, Chang and Hartlage2009). MCAC is caused by an abnormal sensitivity to normal hormone changes (i.e. hormone sensitivity) rather than by abnormal levels or trajectories of ovarian hormones (Gehlert et al., Reference Gehlert, Song, Chang and Hartlage2009; Schmidt, Nieman, Danaceau, Adams, & Rubinow, Reference Schmidt, Nieman, Danaceau, Adams and Rubinow1998). The most extensively studied form is codified in the DSM-5 as Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder (PMDD), a chronic affective disorder with symptoms confined to the luteal phase (Epperson et al., Reference Epperson, Steiner, Hartlage, Eriksson, Schmidt, Jones and Yonkers2012). However, MCAC transcends diagnostic categories, with patients across psychiatric disorders demonstrating premenstrual exacerbation (PME) of symptoms (e.g. 60% of patients with depressive disorders; Nolan & Hughes, Reference Nolan and Hughes2022). Further, MCAC is bi-directionally associated with suicidal ideation (SI) and behavior; psychiatric patients recruited for SI are particularly susceptible to hormone-sensitive changes in affect and suicidality around menses onset (perimenstrually; Owens et al., Reference Owens, Eisenlohr-Moul, Schmalenberger, Bowers, Rubinow, Prinstein and Eisenlohr-Moul2023; Ross et al., Reference Ross, Barone, Tauseef, Schmalenberger, Nagpal, Crane and Eisenlohr-Moul2024), and patients with PMDD report high levels of SI (72% lifetime, 40% current) and attempts (34% lifetime; Eisenlohr-Moul et al., Reference Eisenlohr-Moul, Divine, Schmalenberger, Murphy, Buchert, Wagner-Schuman and Ross2022; Wikman et al., Reference Wikman, Sacher, Bixo, Hirschberg, Kopp Kallner, Epperson and Sundström Poromaa2022). Despite this evidence that MCAC is both common and dangerous, little is known about who is at greatest risk for these cyclical affective changes.

Greater exposure to psychosocial stressors is a plausible risk factor for MCAC. In animals, stressor exposure leads to GABAergic and serotonergic dysregulation, such as altered GABAA receptor subunit conformation and mRNA SERT expression in brain regions involved in stress-related psychiatric disorders (Bravo, Dinan, & Cryan, Reference Bravo, Dinan and Cryan2014; Everington, Gibbard, Swinny, & Seifi, Reference Everington, Gibbard, Swinny and Seifi2018; Gardner, Hale, Lightman, Plotsky, & Lowry, Reference Gardner, Hale, Lightman, Plotsky and Lowry2009; Lages, Rossi, Krahe, & Landeira-Fernandez, Reference Lages, Rossi, Krahe and Landeira-Fernandez2021; Skilbeck, Johnston, & Hinton, Reference Skilbeck, Johnston and Hinton2018); this dysregulation has been linked to MCAC (Frokjaer et al., Reference Frokjaer, Pinborg, Holst, Overgaard, Henningsson, Heede and Knudsen2015; Hantsoo & Epperson, Reference Hantsoo and Epperson2020; Marjoribanks, Brown, O'Brien, & Wyatt, Reference Marjoribanks, Brown, O'Brien and Wyatt2013a; Roca et al., Reference Roca, Schmidt, Smith, Danaceau, Murphy and Rubinow2002). Human research also supports psychosocial stressor exposure as a risk factor for MCAC. In individuals recruited for at least one affective symptom of PMDD, those with a history of physical abuse (v. without) experienced a tighter link between fluctuations in progesterone (P4) and mood or interpersonal symptoms, whereas those with a history of sexual abuse (v. without) reported more pronounced estradiol (E2)-linked anxiety symptoms (Eisenlohr-Moul et al., Reference Eisenlohr-Moul, Rubinow, Schiller, Johnson, Leserman and Girdler2016). Similarly, PMDD patients with higher childhood adversity (i.e. higher total sum of physical, sexual, and emotional abuse as well as emotional and physical neglect) exhibit stronger increases in negative affect and stronger decreases in positive affect from the follicular to late luteal phase (Nayman, Schricker, Reinhard, & Kuehner, Reference Nayman, Schricker, Reinhard and Kuehner2023). In nonclinical samples, one study found that higher perceived stress in the previous month predicted greater premenstrual affective changes (Gollenberg et al., Reference Gollenberg, Hediger, Mumford, Whitcomb, Hovey, Wactawski-Wende and Schisterman2010); another found no impact of perceived stress on midluteal affect (v. all other phases) (Guevarra et al., Reference Guevarra, Louis, Gloe, Block, Kashy, Klump and Moser2023). Despite this evidence suggesting a link between stressor exposure and MCAC, particularly in clinical samples, small sample sizes and inconsistent, imprecise stress measurement limit these conclusions (Slavich, Reference Slavich2019).

Moreover, associations of stressor exposure and MCAC may differ based on type of stressors experienced and when stressors occurred in the life course (McLaughlin, Sheridan, & Lambert, Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan and Lambert2014; Stenson et al., Reference Stenson, Nugent, van Rooij, Minton, Compton, Hinrichs and Jovanovic2021). Although prior studies measure various stressors (e.g. exposure to lifetime physical abuse v. childhood adversity), none have comprehensively cataloged all major stressors across the life course, which is necessary to understand how diverse stressor exposures across developmental stages relate to MCAC.

To address this gap, we examined continuous daily cyclical affective symptoms in a large sample of transdiagnostic psychiatric outpatients, known to be more vulnerable to MCAC than a community sample (Hartlage, Brandenburg, & Kravitz, Reference Hartlage, Brandenburg and Kravitz2004; Kuehner & Nayman, Reference Kuehner and Nayman2021; Nolan & Hughes, Reference Nolan and Hughes2022). We assessed daily symptoms and lifetime stressor exposure using the Stress and Adversity Inventory (STRAIN; Slavich & Shields, Reference Slavich and Shields2018). The STRAIN improves on prior studies by assessing exposure to various distinct stressors across the lifetime (Slavich, Reference Slavich2019). It includes stressors characterized by physical danger, ranging from personal injury to physical and sexual abuse, and interpersonal loss, ranging from parental divorce to neglect to the death of a loved one, thus capturing exposure to ‘threat’ and ‘loss’ as conceptualized in the NIMH's Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) Framework (Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn and Wang2010). These dimensions also fit the Dimensional Model of Adversity and Psychopathology framework, where ‘threat’ (experiences posing danger to physical well-being) and ‘deprivation’ (absence of expected environmental stimuli and social support) are hypothesized to have distinct effects on neural development and behavioral outcomes (McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan and Lambert2014).

To test hypotheses, we used cross-level interactions in multilevel models to examine whether person-level stress variables shaped day-level associations between the cycle and symptoms. First, we tested if total lifetime stressor exposure predicts steeper premenstrual affective symptom increase and slower postmenstrual recovery. Of note, participants in the study were previously included in a larger analysis demonstrating average perimenstrual symptom worsening in the sample (Ross et al., Reference Ross, Barone, Tauseef, Schmalenberger, Nagpal, Crane and Eisenlohr-Moul2024). Next, we tested associations of specific stressor characteristics with cyclical symptom trajectories (pre-menarche v. post-menarche stressors, loss v. danger stressors). Based on our prior finding that physical abuse predicts stronger P4-affect coupling in PMDD (Eisenlohr-Moul et al., Reference Eisenlohr-Moul, Rubinow, Schiller, Johnson, Leserman and Girdler2016), we hypothesized that physical danger stressors would be more strongly related to cyclical affective changes than interpersonal loss stressors. Further, since early life adversity impacts neurobiological risk for psychopathology (McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan and Lambert2014), we expected earlier timing of stressor exposure – e.g. pre-menarche stressors – to be more strongly associated with cyclical affective changes than post-menarche stressors.

Materials and methods

Preregistration

All hypotheses and planned analyses were preregistered on Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/qn4e9) prior to analysis.

Recruitment and enrollment

Data were collected from two crossover clinical trials (NCT03498313, NCT04112368) investigating perimenstrual exogenous E2 and/or P4 effects on cyclical changes in SI and related psychiatric symptoms. Eligibility criteria included: AFAB; age 18–45; BMI 18–35; 21–35-day self-reported menstrual cycles; past-month SI; basic health insurance; and current outpatient mental healthcare. Exclusionary criteria included: exogenous hormonal medications/devices; pregnancy, breastfeeding, or childbirth within the prior 12 months, metabolic, autoimmune, neurological, gynecological, or other chronic nonpsychiatric diseases; heightened genetic risk for thromboembolism or hormone-related cancer; current nicotine use; diagnosis of PMDD using prospective ratings or seeking treatment for PMDD (given that evidence-based treatments for PMDD are available, and the hormone manipulations in the parent trials were not evidence-based treatments for PMDD); history of hospitalization for mania or psychosis; substance use preventing study participation; or elevated, imminent suicide risk. The study recruited a transdiagnostic sample of mental health outpatients reporting past-month SI, expecting a significant MCAC, as 60% of patients with depressive disorders demonstrate clinically-significant MCAC (Hartlage et al., Reference Hartlage, Brandenburg and Kravitz2004). No participants attempted suicide or required inpatient care after entering the study.

Enrollment included eligibility review, informed consent, Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 (SCID-5; First, Williams, Karg, & Spitzer, Reference First, Williams, Karg and Spitzer2015), demographic surveys, and the STRAIN (Slavich & Shields, Reference Slavich and Shields2018). Trained research assistants administered the SCID-5 under licensed clinical psychologist supervision. Participants were enrolled continuously regardless of cycle phase. Daily surveys began the day after enrollment. Present analyses include only participants who completed the STRAIN (Slavich & Shields, Reference Slavich and Shields2018) and documented at least one ovulation (via urine luteinizing-hormone/LH surge ovulation test).

Daily surveys

For one to three cycles before the parent trial's first experimental phase, and for an additional ‘washout’ cycle between experimental phases, participants completed daily surveys reporting menses, sleep, medication use, physical pain (0–10 scale), acute illness, and affective/ suicide-related symptoms. Some participants completed multiple baseline cycles due to scheduling conflicts, treatment changes, failure to document LH surge (e.g. due to missed tests), COVID-19 safety concerns, or low daily survey completion rates during the first cycle.

Measures

Time coding: percent of luteal and follicular phase elapsed

Menstrual cycle time was coded based on self-reported menses and estimated ovulation. Out of 421 cycles, ovulation was confirmed in 219 cycles through LH-surge ovulation tests; each participant contributed at least one menses-to-menses cycle with a positive LH test. The remaining 202 cycles predominantly occurred when ovulation testing was not required by study protocols.

For cycles without ovulation testing, we estimated ovulation. First, we measured the duration of each cycle from the onset of one menses up to and including the day before onset of subsequent menses, for example, 32 days. Using normative data from Bull et al. (Reference Bull, Rowland, Scherwitzl, Scherwitzl, Danielsson and Harper2019), which utilizes a large dataset of ovulatory cycles (over 600 000 cycles), we determined average lengths of the luteal and follicular phases for the observed cycle duration. For example, for a 31-to-35-day cycle, the average luteal length was 12.9 days (40% of the total cycle, rounded) and the average follicular length was 19.5 days (60% of the total cycle, rounded). Based on these percentages, we estimated ovulation day (mapped onto LH + 1) within each menses-to-menses cycle. In this example, ovulation would be estimated on day 19 (rounded from 19.2), after 60% of the 32-day cycle had elapsed (corresponding to the proportion of the cycle estimated to be follicular). To preserve data quality, we restricted our analysis to cycles lasting 21 to 35 days due to uncertainty of ovulation dates in shorter or longer cycles (Schmalenberger et al., Reference Schmalenberger, Tauseef, Barone, Owens, Lieberman, Jarczok and Eisenlohr-Moul2021).

To ensure shared hormonal meaning, cycle time was scaled to percentage of luteal phase elapsed and percentage of follicular phase elapsed. Previous methods count days from menses onset/ovulation and assume a consistent 14-day luteal phase (Schmalenberger et al., Reference Schmalenberger, Tauseef, Barone, Owens, Lieberman, Jarczok and Eisenlohr-Moul2021), overlooking between- and within-person variations in phase lengths (Bull et al., Reference Bull, Rowland, Scherwitzl, Scherwitzl, Danielsson and Harper2019; Fehring, Schneider, & Raviele, Reference Fehring, Schneider and Raviele2006). The luteal phase was defined from day after estimated ovulation (LH + 2) up to and including the day before the next menses onset. The follicular phase was defined from menses onset up to and including the estimated day of ovulation (LH + 1). Our analyses included 421 cycles, with cycle time scaled from −1 to + 1, where zero marked menses onset. This scaling allowed for precise modeling of outcomes centered on menses. In graphs, the −1 to 0 scale of the luteal phase was rescaled to 0 to 100 to correspond to percentage of luteal phase elapsed and is depicted by ‘%L’; the same was done for the follicular phase (i.e. ‘%F’).

Stress and adversity inventory

Participants completed the STRAIN during enrollment (Slavich & Shields, Reference Slavich and Shields2018), a 415-question, NIMH-recommended measure assessing exposure to 26 acute life events (e.g. death of relative, job loss, negative health event) and 29 chronic difficulties (e.g. persistent health, work, relationship, or financial problems) across lifetime (see https://www.strainsetup.com). An example question is ‘Was there ever a period of time when you were separated from a parent (or main caregiver) for at least one month before you were 18?’ The total possible lifetime stressor exposure count is 166 (range: 0–166).

We calculated counts of total lifetime, pre-menarche, post-menarche, lifetime interpersonal loss (e.g. parental divorce, neglect, loss of a loved one), and lifetime physical danger stressor exposures (i.e. personal injury, such as breaking a leg, instances of physical and sexual abuse) based on reported stressor timings. The STRAIN evaluates everyday stressors and events categorized as traumas by the SCID-5 post-traumatic stress disorder trauma screen (First et al., Reference First, Williams, Karg and Spitzer2015; Slavich & Shields, Reference Slavich and Shields2018). Pre-menarche and post-menarche stressor counts were based on each participant's age of menarche. The STRAIN has excellent test-retest reliability (r icc = 0.936 for total count of stressor exposure), concurrent and discriminant validity, and has been shown to predict psychological, biological, and clinical outcomes (Banica, Sandre, Shields, Slavich, & Weinberg, Reference Banica, Sandre, Shields, Slavich and Weinberg2022; Cazassa, Oliveira, Spahr, Shields, & Slavich, Reference Cazassa, Oliveira, Spahr, Shields and Slavich2019; Mayer et al., Reference Mayer, Guan, Lin, Hamlat, Parker, Brownell and Epel2023; Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Sichko, Bui, Libowitz, Shields and Slavich2023; Sturmbauer, Shields, Hetzel, Rohleder, & Slavich, Reference Sturmbauer, Shields, Hetzel, Rohleder and Slavich2019).

Negative affective symptoms and suicidal ideation

Participants completed the Daily Record of Severity of Problems (DRSP; Endicott, Nee, & Harrison, Reference Endicott, Nee and Harrison2006), a 21-item, 6-point scale (not at all to extreme) capturing symptoms of PMDD. Current analyses included items assessing (1) depressed mood, (2) hopelessness, (3) worthlessness/guilt, (4) anxiety, (5) mood swings, (6) rejection sensitivity, (7) anger/irritability, and (8) interpersonal conflict. We calculated a daily average of these core affective symptoms of DSM-5 PMDD (American Psychiatric Association, Reference American Psychiatric Association2013), termed negative affect. The study focused on cyclical emotional symptoms, so we did not use the mean total DRSP score, which would include cognitive, behavioral, and physical symptoms. The reliability of change (Rc; Revelle, Reference Revelle2023) coefficient for the eight DRSP items was 0.80 indicating reliable within-person covariation. Due to missing data at the survey-level rather than the item-level, means are utilized instead of sums.

Repeated measures correlation coefficients between the eight DRSP items (forming negative affect; Bakdash & Marusich, Reference Bakdash and Marusich2022; Bland & Altman, Reference Bland and Altman1995a, Reference Bland and Altman1995b) varied widely, ranging from 0.27 to 0.70. This prompted additional exploratory analyses to investigate cyclicity of individual DRSP symptoms and identify those driving changes in negative affect. Analyzing effects at the individual symptom-level also helps to inform future research on potential mechanisms specific to each symptom.

Daily surveys also assessed SI severity. Participants rated their past-24-hr agreement to items on a 5-point scale, (not at all to extremely). Mean daily values of items 1, 9, and 19 of the Adult Suicidal Ideation Questionnaire, as well as ‘I wished I could go to sleep and not wake up’, constituted composite passive SI (Reynolds, Reference Reynolds1991) with an R c of 0.79. Active SI items included items 2, 17, and 25 of the Adult Suicidal Ideation Questionnaire, as well as ‘I wanted to kill myself’(Reynolds, Reference Reynolds1991). Active SI was dichotomized due to the low frequency of active SI endorsement and the lack of convergence of hurdle and zero-inflated models. On days when the participant rated any of the above active SI items above a 1 (not at all), that day was assigned a value of 1; otherwise, it was assigned a 0. When considered as a continuous variable, these items did exhibit within-person reliability with an R c of 0.76.

Analytic plan

In R version 4.3.2 we conducted preregistered analyses using packages ‘lme4’ (Bates, Mächler, Bolker, & Walker, Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015), ‘lmerTest’ (Kuznetsova, Brockhoff, & Christensen, Reference Kuznetsova, Brockhoff and Christensen2017), and ‘interactions’ (Long, Reference Long2021). We tested the moderating effect of stressors (total, pre-menarche, post-menarche, loss, and danger) on the relationship between the cycle and daily symptoms (e.g. negative affect, passive SI, active SI, and individual affective symptoms). Daily symptom scores (level 1) were nested within participants (level 2). All models included random intercepts and slopes to capture individual differences in symptom trajectories.

To flexibly model nonlinear symptom patterns across the cycle, we used polynomial growth models (Hastie, Reference Hastie2017; Kennedy & Gentle, Reference Kennedy and Gentle2021). These models utilized orthogonal polynomials of cycle time (−1 to +1), reducing multicollinearity between the polynomials, and enabling the inclusion of higher degree random slopes. We assessed the fit of different degree polynomial time (quartic, cubic, quadratic, and linear) in fixed and random effects for predicting outcomes without moderators, employing likelihood ratio tests to determine the best fit. Fixed and random effects of time retained in final models are in results tables (Tables 2, online Supplementary S1-S5).

We tested moderation hypotheses by including stressor exposure (level 2; z-scored) and its interactions with time. The models included age at enrollment (level 2; z-scored) to account for the positive relationship between age and stressor count, as well as daily SSRI use (level 1; binary) given its efficacy in treating PMDD (Marjoribanks et al., Reference Marjoribanks, Brown, O'Brien and Wyatt2013; Roca et al., Reference Roca, Schmidt, Smith, Danaceau, Murphy and Rubinow2002; Steinberg, Cardoso, Martinez, Rubinow, & Schmidt, Reference Steinberg, Cardoso, Martinez, Rubinow and Schmidt2012). Sensitivity analysis covaried the interactions between time trends and daily SSRI use. Additionally, we conducted an exploratory analysis to predict daily physical pain from the interactions between time trends and lifetime stressors to consider potential confounding effects of stress on physical symptoms across the cycle.

We used the Johnson–Neyman technique (‘interactions’ package; Long, Reference Long2021) on each significant interaction to determine the range of moderator values where stressors significantly influenced cyclicity. Model-implied value graphs display the 75th, 50th, and 25th percentiles of stressor exposures. Graphs of person-mean-centered outcomes across tertiles of stressor exposure are in the supplement.

Results

Participants

The final analysis included 6187 daily ratings from 114 participants. Each contributed an average of 54.3 observations (s.d. = 39.9; min = 9; max = 283). The range is due to fewer daily ratings from those still in the trial at the time present analyses were conducted, and more from participants completing the trial during COVID-19-related pauses. Descriptives for demographics, psychiatric diagnoses, and stressor exposure are in Table 1.

Table 1. Demographics, stressor exposure, and baseline clinical categories in participants (N = 114)

a N does not sum to the sample of N = 114 since disorders are often co-morbid.

b For each participant, pre-menarche and post-menarche stressor counts do not sum to their lifetime stressor counts because the STRAIN only assesses age of stressor exposure for the most severe stressor when a stressor has occurred multiple times. The analysis of pre-menarche and post-menarche stressors only includes the counts for stressors for which there is explicit age information.

Model specification and random effects

To address non-normally distributed residuals and fulfill model assumptions, we applied natural logarithm transformations to negative affect, passive SI, pain, and all DRSP items except depressed mood, with estimates presented on the log scale in tables. To address heteroscedastic residuals of models predicting passive SI, mood swings, and anger/irritability, we utilized a robust sandwich variance-covariance estimator from the ‘clubSandwich’ R package (Pustejovsky, Reference Pustejovsky2023). Employing the original ‘CR0’ sandwich estimator due to our large sample size (Liang & Zeger, Reference Liang and Zeger1986), we adjusted for heteroscedasticity to yield standard errors less sensitive to violations of homoscedasticity (Pustejovsky, Reference Pustejovsky2023). For these outcomes, confidence intervals and p-values were calculated using robust sandwich estimates for the variance-covariance matrices of the models (Tables 2, online Supplementary S1-S5).

Table 2. Lifetime stressors predict affective symptom cyclicity

All random slopes for time trends were included in initial models and were retained based on likelihood ratio tests. Intraclass correlations for symptom outcomes ranged from 0.57–0.65, indicating approximately 60% of variance was due to between-person differences (Table 2).

Effects of cumulative life stressors on MCAC

Consistent with hypotheses that greater lifetime stressors would predict stronger cyclical symptom changes, we observed significant (for active SI) or marginally significant (for negative affect and passive SI) interactions between stressors and quadratic cycle time (Table 2). Greater stress exposure was associated with a stronger increase from the early luteal phase to menses onset, followed by a steeper decline from menses onset to the late follicular phase (Fig. 1, online Supplementary S3).

Figure 1. Lifetime stressors predict symptom trajectories across the menstrual cycle. Model-implied values of symptom trajectories across the menstrual cycle by number of lifetime stressors where squares represent more 75th percentile of number of stressors in the sample (34 stressors; P75), triangles represent 50th percentile (23 stressors; P50), and circles represent 25th percentile (15 stressors; P25). L, luteal phase; F, follicular phase. (A) Daily negative affect is a daily mean score of the core emotional symptoms of the daily record of severity of problems (rated from 1 = ‘Not at All’ to 6 = ‘ Extremely’). Marginal significance (p = 0.072) in the interaction between lifetime stressors and menstrual cycle time at > 15 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [− 18 143.50, 15.65]) (B) Daily passive SI is a daily mean score of items 1, 9, and 19 of the Adult Suicidal Ideation Questionnaire, as well as ‘I wished I could go to sleep and not wake up’ (rated from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely)). Marginal significance (p = 0.076) in the interaction between lifetime stressors and menstrual cycle time at >22 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [− 12 632.59, 22.52]). (C) Active SI items included items 2, 17, and 25 of the Adult Suicidal Ideation Questionnaire, as well as ‘I wanted to kill myself’. On days when the participant rated any of the above active SI items greater than a 1 (not at all), that day was assigned a value of 0 for active SI; otherwise, they were assigned a value of 1. Odds of active SI were predicted by the number of lifetime stressors. Significance (p < 0.05) in the interaction between lifetime stressors and menstrual cycle time at >26 stressors (of outside Johnson–Neyman interval [−82.21, 26.01]).

Johnson–Neyman intervals identified the range of values where stressors significantly influenced the association between cycle time and symptoms. When predicting active SI, the interaction between quadratic time and lifetime stressors reached significance at >26 stressors (significance outside Johnson–Neyman interval [−82.21, 26.01]). Approximately half of the sample (n = 55, 48%) experienced more than 26 stressors. When predicting negative affect, the interaction between quadratic time and lifetime stressors reached marginal significance (p = 0.072) at > 15 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [−18 143.50, 15.65]). Seventy-eight percent of the sample experienced more than 15 stressors. For passive SI, the interaction between quadratic time and lifetime stressors reached marginal significance (p = 0.076) at >22 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [−12 632.59, 22.52]). Approximately half of the sample (n = 62; 54%) experienced more than 22 stressors. For marginally significant outcomes (e.g. negative affect and passive SI), the Johnson–Neyman interval was calculated at the value of the marginal p-values, rather than α = 0.05, reflecting the interaction's marginal significance.

Effects of pre-menarche and post-menarche stressor exposure on MCAC

We hypothesized that greater pre-menarche stressors, compared to post-menarche stressors, would be associated with greater cyclical increases in negative affect, passive SI, and active SI (online Supplementary Table S1, online Supplementary Figure S1). We observed a significant interaction between pre-menarche stressors and cubic time in predicting active SI, but no other significant moderations by pre-menarche or post-menarche stressors. Increased pre-menarche stressors was associated with an earlier and steeper luteal symptom increase (online Supplementary Figure S1, S4). This interaction reached significance at ≥11 pre-menarche stressors (significance outside Johnson–Neyman interval [0.28, 10.41]); however, just 6.1% of the sample experienced more than 11 pre-menarche stressors (n = 7).

Effects of interpersonal loss and physical danger stressors on MCAC

We hypothesized that danger stressors would predict more pronounced affective cyclicity than loss stressors. Instead, loss was the more useful moderator (online Supplementary Table S2). Loss exposure moderated quadratic and cubic cycle change in negative affect. Specifically, greater loss stressors predicted a steeper symptom increase from the mid-to-late luteal phase, followed by a steeper decline from menses to the late follicular phase. This transition from increasing to decreasing symptoms also occurred later in the cycle for those with higher loss exposure (Fig. 2, online Supplementary S5). Interestingly, greater loss was not associated with greater mean symptoms (Fig. 2, online Supplementary S5). When predicting negative affect, the interaction between cubic time and loss stressors became significant at >3 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [−34.35, 3.64]); 71.1% of the sample experienced more than 3 loss stressors (n = 81).

Figure 2. Interpersonal loss stressors predict symptom trajectories across the menstrual cycle. Model-implied values of symptom trajectories across the menstrual cycle by number of interpersonal loss stressors where squares represent more 75th percentile of number of loss stressors in the sample (7.75 stressors; P75), triangles represent 50th percentile (5 stressors; P50), and circles represent 25th percentile (3 stressors; P25). L, luteal phase; F, follicular phase. (A) Daily negative affect is a daily mean score of the core emotional symptoms of the daily record of severity of problems (rated from 1 = ‘Not at All’ to 6 = ‘ Extremely’). Significance (p < 0.05) in the interaction between menstrual cycle time and loss stressors at > 3 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [−34.35, 3.64]). (B) Daily passive SI is a daily mean score of items 1, 9, and 19 of the Adult Suicidal Ideation Questionnaire, as well as ‘I wished I could go to sleep and not wake up’ (rated from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely)). Significance (p < 0.05) in the interaction between menstrual cycle time and loss stressors at >7 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [0.43, 6.83]). (C) Active SI items included items 2, 17, and 25 of the Adult Suicidal Ideation Questionnaire, as well as ‘I wanted to kill myself’. On days when the participant rated any of the above active SI items greater than a 1 (not at all), that day was assigned a value of 0 for active SI; otherwise, they were assigned a value of 1. Significance (p < 0.05) in the interaction between menstrual cycle time and loss stressors at > 5 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [−2.18, 5.55]). Odds of active SI were predicted by the number of loss stressors.

We observed a similar pattern for passive SI, with an interaction between cubic time and loss stressors (Fig. 2, online Supplementary S5). This interaction reached significance at >7 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [0.43, 6.83]); 25.4% of the sample experienced more than 7 loss stressors (n = 29).

When predicting active SI, we found an interaction between quadratic time and loss stressors (online Supplementary Table S2). The trajectory of active SI symptom cyclicity moderated by loss stressors is depicted in Fig. 2, with person-mean centered plots stratified by loss level in online Supplementary Figure S5. This interaction reached significance at >5 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [−2.18, 5.55]); 44.7% of the sample experienced more than 5 loss stressors (n = 40).

SSRI sensitivity analysis

We augmented hypothesis 1 models with interactions between SSRI use and time trends (online Supplementary Table S3). These were not statistically significant and did not reduce the significance of stressor by time trend interactions (online Supplementary Table S3, Figure S2).

Exploratory analysis: predicting physical pain cyclicity from stressor exposure

To examine whether the moderating effects of stressor exposure on cyclical affective symptoms were confounded by similar effects on daily physical pain (e.g. dysmenorrhea), we predicted physical pain from the interaction of cycle time trends and lifetime stressors (online Supplementary Table S4). These interactions did not significantly predict physical pain, suggesting that the influence of stressor exposure on symptom cyclicity is not due to effects on physical pain.

Exploratory analysis: predicting individual DRSP symptom cyclicity from stressor exposure

Lifetime stressor moderation of symptom cyclicity was significant for mood swings, anger/irritability, and interpersonal conflict, and marginally significant for rejection sensitivity (online Supplementary Table S5). It was not a significant moderator of cyclical effects on depressed mood, hopelessness, worthlessness/guilt, or anxiety. For mood swings, anger/irritability, and rejection sensitivity, greater stressors were associated with an increased slope in symptoms luteally, reaching a peak around menses onset, followed by a steep decline in symptoms into the late follicular phase (Fig. 3, online Supplementary S6, S7). For interpersonal conflict, greater stressors were associated with an increased slope in symptoms across the luteal phase and a slower decline follicularly (Fig. 3, online Supplementary S7).

Figure 3. Lifetime stressors predict mood swings, rejection sensitivity, anger and irritability, and interpersonal conflict trajectories across the menstrual cycle. Model-implied values of symptom trajectories across the menstrual cycle by number of lifetime stressors where squares represent more 75th percentile of number of stressors in the sample (34 stressors; P75), triangles represent 50th percentile (23 stressors; P50), and circles represent 25th percentile (15 stressors; P25). L, luteal phase; F, follicular phase. (A) Daily mood swings (rated from 1 = ‘Not at All’ to 6 = ‘ Extremely’) across the menstrual cycle are predicted by number of lifetime stressors. Significance (p < 0.05) in the interaction between menstrual cycle time and stressors at > 13 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [−98.05, 12.61]). (B) Daily rejection sensitivity (rated from 1 = ‘Not at All’ to 6 = ‘ Extremely’) across the menstrual cycle are predicted by number of lifetime stressors. Marginal significance (p = 0.079) in the interaction between lifetime stressors and menstrual cycle time at >22 stressors (inside Johnson–Neyman interval [22.40, 731.81]). (C) Daily anger and irritability (rated from 1 = ‘Not at All’ to 6 = ‘ Extremely’) across the menstrual cycle are predicted by number of lifetime stressors. Significance (p < 0.05) in the interaction between menstrual cycle time and stressors at > 18 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [−455.46, 18.05]). (D) Daily interpersonal conflict (rated from 1 = ‘Not at All’ to 6 = ‘ Extremely’) across the menstrual cycle are predicted by number of lifetime stressors. Significance (p < 0.05) in the interaction between menstrual cycle time and stressors at > 31 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [1.84, 31.00]).

Discussion

Although prior research identifies psychosocial stressors as a risk factor for MCAC, it has treated stressor exposure as a unitary construct, overlooking the diversity of stressors across different life stages. To address this, we conducted the largest prospective study to date on the impact of stressors across the life course on symptom reactivity to ovarian hormone fluctuations in a psychiatric sample. Results indicate that greater lifetime stressor exposure predicts more severe perimenstrual affective changes.

Association between lifetime stressor exposure and affective reactivity to the cycle

Greater lifetime stressor exposure predicted more severe perimenstrual worsening of negative affect, passive SI, and active SI. This effect was significant for active SI, and marginally significant for passive SI and negative affect. While fixed effects reveal that more lifetime stressors predicted more severe symptom trajectories, random effects of cycle time indicate significant variability in MCAC between individuals, consistent with prior longitudinal studies (Eisenlohr-Moul et al., Reference Eisenlohr-Moul, Kaiser, Weise, Schmalenberger, Kiesner, Ditzen and Kleinstäuber2020, Reference Eisenlohr-Moul, Rubinow, Schiller, Johnson, Leserman and Girdler2016; Ross et al., Reference Ross, Barone, Tauseef, Schmalenberger, Nagpal, Crane and Eisenlohr-Moul2024). These results align with cumulative stress models suggesting recent and past stressors have an additive effect (Epel et al., Reference Epel, Crosswell, Mayer, Prather, Slavich, Puterman and Mendes2018; Evans, Li, & Whipple, Reference Evans, Li and Whipple2013), leading to worse health outcomes over time (Slavich & Shields, Reference Slavich and Shields2018).

The impact of lifetime stressors on hormone-related affective symptoms across the reproductive lifespan

These results contribute to literature demonstrating that increased stressful life events influence affective symptoms across hormonal fluctuation periods – an effect that has been observed during the menstrual cycle and other reproductive transitions like puberty, pregnancy, and menopause (Allen et al., Reference Allen, Goslawski, Taple, Sakowicz, Alvarado-Goldberg and Miller2023; Andersen, Klusmann, Eisenlohr-Moul, Baresich, & Girdler, Reference Andersen, Klusmann, Eisenlohr-Moul, Baresich and Girdler2023; Gordon, Rubinow, Eisenlohr-Moul, Leserman, & Girdler, Reference Gordon, Rubinow, Eisenlohr-Moul, Leserman and Girdler2016). During the peripubertal transition, hormone increases predict greater affective symptoms in high-stress situations (Andersen et al., Reference Andersen, Klusmann, Eisenlohr-Moul, Baresich and Girdler2023). Pregnant patients with greater exposure to adverse childhood events experience a more severe depression trajectory throughout the hormonally dynamic antenatal period (Allen et al., Reference Allen, Goslawski, Taple, Sakowicz, Alvarado-Goldberg and Miller2023). Finally, across the perimenopause transition, estradiol fluctuation predicts greater rejection sensitivity during psychosocial stress (Gordon et al., Reference Gordon, Rubinow, Eisenlohr-Moul, Leserman and Girdler2016).

Pre-menarche stressors, compared to post-menarche stressors influence symptom reactivity to the cycle

Compared to post-menarche stressors, pre-menarche stressors predicted a more severe symptom trajectory of active SI, but not negative affect or passive SI. Greater pre-menarche stressors were associated with an earlier, steeper luteal increase (online Supplementary Figure S1, S4). However, the trend was only significant at high numbers of pre-menarche stressors, experienced by a small subset of the sample (6.1%, n = 7). Early life stress may exacerbate cyclical increases in suicidality in individuals with high stress exposure. Further studies comparing early and later life stressors are necessary to validate these findings, as this study is the first to directly compare stressors from different life stages in the context of affective hormone sensitivity.

Interpersonal loss stressors, compared to physical danger stressors, influence symptom reactivity to the cycle

Compared to danger stressors, loss stressors predicted more severe perimenstrual worsening of negative affect, passive SI, and active SI. Loss and total lifetime stressors followed a similar pattern, with greater stressors linked to steeper symptom increases from the mid-to-late luteal phase, followed by steeper declines from menses to the late follicular phase (Fig. 2, online Supplementary Table S2). While greater loss exposure was not associated with higher mean symptoms, individuals with more stressors experienced more pronounced cyclic symptom patterns, highlighting the possibility that loss stressors more strongly impact symptomatic responses to cycling hormones compared to general risk of symptoms.

Exploratory analysis: lifetime stressors and individual DRSP item reactivity to the cycle

We examined whether lifetime stressor exposure predicted individual DRSP symptom trajectories to clarify if stressors predicting negative affect was driven by specific symptoms. Lifetime stressor exposure significantly predicted cyclicity of mood swings, anger/irritability, interpersonal conflict, and was a marginally significant moderator of cyclical worsening of rejection sensitivity (online Supplementary Table S5). In contrast, lifetime stressor exposure did not significantly moderate cyclical effects on depressed mood, hopelessness, worthlessness/guilt, or anxiety. These effects are reminiscent of previous research showing that histories of physical abuse predicted a stronger progesterone effect on mood swings, rejection sensitivity, anger/irritability, interpersonal conflict, and depressed mood, with the smallest effect observed on depressed mood (Eisenlohr-Moul et al., Reference Eisenlohr-Moul, Rubinow, Schiller, Johnson, Leserman and Girdler2016). Further work should investigate how specific stressors, particularly physical abuse, predict cyclical changes in mood swings, anger/irritability, and interpersonal conflict.

These findings suggest that cyclical worsening of negative affect is not driven by a single symptom and reflects a broader phenomenon, with changes in mood instability and interpersonal symptoms more strongly linked to stress than depression and anxiety. Notably, conflict displayed a slower follicular return to baseline, possibly related to the stress generation hypothesis (Hammen, Reference Hammen1991): highly stressed individuals who experience increases in negative affect luteally may generate more new stressors (e.g. conflicts), prolonging the return to affective baseline.

Strengths, limitations, and future directions

The present study employs an innovative, intensive, within-person design to explore relationships between lifetime stressor exposures and affective symptom change across the menstrual cycle. Strengths include pre-registered hypotheses, a large transdiagnostic sample, prospective data, and direct comparisons between pre- and post-menarche stressors, as well as between interpersonal loss and physical danger stressors. Furthermore, we introduced novel methods by standardizing cycle time between participants and flexibly modeling nonlinear trajectories with multilevel polynomial growth models.

However, several limitations should be acknowledged. Although cycle time was determined using LH-confirmed ovulation dates, not every cycle included in the study had an LH-surge confirmed ovulation date due to participants not being required to ovulation test during portions of the parent trial. Instead, we imputed ovulation timing based on a large normative dataset (600 000 cycles) and known menses-to-menses cycle lengths in our data. Ideally, future studies should confirm ovulation in all cycles using biological methods described in Schmalenberger et al. (Reference Schmalenberger, Tauseef, Barone, Owens, Lieberman, Jarczok and Eisenlohr-Moul2021). Additionally, given the rarefied nature of this high-risk sample and descriptive nature of this paper, we prioritized reducing type 2 errors and did not correct for multiple comparisons. We acknowledge that many of the effects described herein would not survive correction for multiple tests, and some may therefore represent Type I errors.

Our results highlight variability in symptom cyclicity, with all models fitting significantly better with random slopes. To understand individual-level symptom patterns, idiographic modeling should be explored. Furthermore, centering cycle time on ovulation rather than (or in addition to) menses onset may reveal whether the periovulatory window is a period of heightened risk for some. Our findings suggest potential periovulatory peaks in negative affect, passive SI, and active SI, demonstrating the importance of investigating this timeframe. Finally, to deepen understanding of how stressors impact cyclical symptoms, future research should examine perceived severity of stressors.

Conclusion

The present study represents the largest prospective test of the link between cumulative lifetime stressor exposure and MCAC in a psychiatric sample. Results suggest that cumulative lifetime stressor exposure predicts greater cycle-related affective changes. Additional research is needed to elucidate the neurobiology of these stress-related effects.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724001661.

Author contributions

AN generated the research idea, developed hypotheses, ran the statistical analyses, and drafted the manuscript. JB contributed to data management and analysis as well as manuscript preparation. HT assisted with data management and helped with manuscript preparation. JR helped with manuscript writing and preparation. KMS helped with manuscript writing and preparation. ZG assisted with data management and analysis. GS contributed to data analysis, manuscript review and preparation. GMS contributed to manuscript review and preparation. TEM provided funding, procurement, assisted with study conceptualization and data analysis, and assisted with manuscript preparation.

Funding statement

This study was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health (K99R00MH109667, RF1MH120843, T32MH067631). KMS was supported by grant #SCHM 3732/1-1, 470147139, from the German Research Foundation (DFG). GMS was supported by grant #OPR21101 from the California Governor's Office of Planning and Research/California Initiative to Advance Precision Medicine. The findings and conclusions in this article are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views or opinions of these organizations, which had no role in designing or planning this study; in collecting, analyzing, or interpreting the data; in writing the article; or in deciding to submit this article for publication.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

References

Allen, E. C., Goslawski, M. M., Taple, B. J., Sakowicz, A., Alvarado-Goldberg, M., & Miller, E. S. (2023). The association between adverse childhood experiences and perinatal depression symptom trajectories. American Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology MFM, 5(8), 101039. doi:10.1016/j.ajogmf.2023.101039CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association, (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM–5) (5th ed.). doi:10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersen, E., Klusmann, H., Eisenlohr-Moul, T., Baresich, K., & Girdler, S. (2023). Life stress influences the relationship between sex hormone fluctuation and affective symptoms in peripubertal female adolescents. Development and Psychopathology, 36(2), 821833. doi:10.1017/S095457942300010XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakdash, J. Z., & Marusich, L. R. (2022). rmcorr: Repeated Measures Correlation. Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/rmcorr/index.htmlGoogle Scholar
Banica, I., Sandre, A., Shields, G. S., Slavich, G. M., & Weinberg, A. (2022). Associations between lifetime stress exposure and the error-related negativity (ERN) differ based on stressor characteristics and exposure timing in young adults. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 22(4), 672689. doi:10.3758/s13415-021-00883-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 148. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bland, J. M., & Altman, D. G. (1995a). Calculating correlation coefficients with repeated observations: Part 2—correlation between subjects. BMJ, 310(6980), 633. doi:10.1136/bmj.310.6980.633CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bland, J. M., & Altman, D. G. (1995b). Statistics notes: Calculating correlation coefficients with repeated observations: Part 1—correlation within subjects. BMJ, 310(6977), 446. doi:10.1136/bmj.310.6977.446CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bravo, J. A., Dinan, T. G., & Cryan, J. F. (2014). Early-life stress induces persistent alterations in 5-HT1A receptor and serotonin transporter mRNA expression in the adult rat brain. Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience, 7, 24. doi:10.3389/fnmol.2014.00024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bull, J. R., Rowland, S. P., Scherwitzl, E. B., Scherwitzl, R., Danielsson, K. G., & Harper, J. (2019). Real-world menstrual cycle characteristics of more than 600000 menstrual cycles | npj digital medicine. Npj Digital Medicine, 2(1), 83. doi:10.1038/s41746-019-0152-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cazassa, M. J., Oliveira, M. d. S., Spahr, C. M., Shields, G. S., & Slavich, G. M. (2019). The stress and adversity inventory for adults (adult STRAIN) in Brazilian Portuguese: Initial validation and links with executive function, sleep, and mental and physical health. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 3083. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.03083CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A., Divine, M., Schmalenberger, K., Murphy, L., Buchert, B., Wagner-Schuman, M., … Ross, J. (2022). Prevalence of lifetime self-injurious thoughts and behaviors in a global sample of 599 patients reporting prospectively confirmed diagnosis with premenstrual dysphoric disorder. BMC Psychiatry, 22(1), 199. doi:10.1186/s12888-022-03851-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A., Kaiser, G., Weise, C., Schmalenberger, K. M., Kiesner, J., Ditzen, B., & Kleinstäuber, M. (2020). Are there temporal subtypes of premenstrual dysphoric disorder?: Using group-based trajectory modeling to identify individual differences in symptom change. Psychological Medicine, 50(6), 964972. doi:10.1017/S0033291719000849CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A., Rubinow, D. R., Schiller, C. E., Johnson, J. L., Leserman, J., & Girdler, S. S. (2016). Histories of abuse predict stronger within-person covariation of ovarian steroids and mood symptoms in women with menstrually related mood disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 67, 142152. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.01.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Endicott, J., Nee, J., & Harrison, W. (2006). Daily record of severity of problems (DRSP): Reliability and validity. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 9(1), 4149. doi:10.1007/s00737-005-0103-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epel, E. S., Crosswell, A. D., Mayer, S. E., Prather, A. A., Slavich, G. M., Puterman, E., & Mendes, W. B. (2018). More than a feeling: A unified view of stress measurement for population science. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 49, 146169. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2018.03.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epperson, C. N., Steiner, M., Hartlage, S. A., Eriksson, E., Schmidt, P. J., Jones, I., & Yonkers, K. A. (2012). Premenstrual dysphoric disorder: Evidence for a new category for DSM-5. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169(5), 465475. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11081302CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, G. W., Li, D., & Whipple, S. S. (2013). Cumulative risk and child development. Psychological Bulletin, 139(6), 13421396. doi:10.1037/a0031808CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Everington, E. A., Gibbard, A. G., Swinny, J. D., & Seifi, M. (2018). Molecular characterization of GABA-A receptor subunit diversity within major peripheral organs and their plasticity in response to early life psychosocial stress. Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience, 11, 18. Retrieved from https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnmol.2018.00018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fehring, R. J., Schneider, M., & Raviele, K. (2006). Variability in the phases of the menstrual cycle. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic & Neonatal Nursing, 35(3), 376384. doi:10.1111/j.1552-6909.2006.00051.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, M. B., Williams, J. B. W., Karg, R. S., & Spitzer, R. L. (2015). Structured clinical interview for DSM-5—research version (SCID-5 for DSM-5, research version; SCID-5-RV). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association, 2015.Google Scholar
Frokjaer, V. G., Pinborg, A., Holst, K. K., Overgaard, A., Henningsson, S., Heede, M., … Knudsen, G. M. (2015). Role of serotonin transporter changes in depressive responses to sex-steroid hormone manipulation: A positron emission tomography study. Biological Psychiatry, 78(8), 534543. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.04.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, K. L., Hale, M. W., Lightman, S. L., Plotsky, P. M., & Lowry, C. A. (2009). Adverse early life experience and social stress during adulthood interact to increase serotonin transporter mRNA expression. Brain Research, 1305, 4763. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.065CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gehlert, S., Song, I. H., Chang, C.-H., & Hartlage, S. A. (2009). The prevalence of premenstrual dysphoric disorder in a randomly selected group of urban and rural women. Psychological Medicine, 39(1), 129136. doi:10.1017/S003329170800322XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollenberg, A. L., Hediger, M. L., Mumford, S. L., Whitcomb, B. W., Hovey, K. M., Wactawski-Wende, J., & Schisterman, E. F. (2010). Perceived stress and severity of perimenstrual symptoms: The BioCycle study. Journal of Women's Health, 19(5), 959967. doi:10.1089/jwh.2009.1717CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, J. L., Rubinow, D. R., Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A., Leserman, J., & Girdler, S. S. (2016). Estradiol variability, stressful life events and the emergence of depressive symptomatology during the menopause transition. Menopause (New York, N.Y.), 23(3), 257266. doi:10.1097/GME.0000000000000528CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guevarra, D. A., Louis, C. C., Gloe, L. M., Block, S. R., Kashy, D. A., Klump, K. L., & Moser, J. S. (2023). Examining a window of vulnerability for affective symptoms in the mid-luteal phase of the menstrual cycle. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 147, 105958. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2022.105958CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammen, C. (1991). Generation of stress in the course of unipolar depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100(4), 555561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hantsoo, L., & Epperson, C. N. (2020). Allopregnanolone in premenstrual dysphoric disorder (PMDD): Evidence for dysregulated sensitivity to GABA-A receptor modulating neuroactive steroids across the menstrual cycle. Neurobiology of Stress, 12, 100213. doi:10.1016/j.ynstr.2020.100213CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartlage, S. A., Brandenburg, D. L., & Kravitz, H. M. (2004). Premenstrual exacerbation of depressive disorders in a community-based sample in the United States. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66(5), 698706. doi:10.1097/01.psy.0000138131.92408.b9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hastie, T. J. (Ed.). (2017). Statistical models in S. New York: Routledge. doi:10.1201/9780203738535Google Scholar
Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., … Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(7), 748751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, W.J., & Gentle, J. E. (2021). Statistical computing. New York: Routledge. doi:10.1201/9780203738672CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuehner, C., & Nayman, S. (2021). Premenstrual exacerbations of mood disorders: Findings and knowledge gaps. Current Psychiatry Reports, 23(11), 78. doi:10.1007/s11920-021-01286-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). Lmertest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82, 126. doi:10.18637/jss.v082.i13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lages, Y. V. M., Rossi, A. D., Krahe, T. E., & Landeira-Fernandez, J. (2021). Effect of chronic unpredictable mild stress on the expression profile of serotonin receptors in rats and mice: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 124, 7888. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.01.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liang, K. Y., & Zeger, S. L. (1986). Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika, 73(1), 1322. doi:10.1093/biomet/73.1.13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, J. A. (2021). interactions: Comprehensive, User-Friendly Toolkit for Probing Interactions. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=interactionsGoogle Scholar
Marjoribanks, J., Brown, J., O'Brien, P. M. S., & Wyatt, K. (2013a). Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors for premenstrual syndrome. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 2013 (6), CD001396. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD001396.pub3Google ScholarPubMed
Mayer, S. E., Guan, J., Lin, J., Hamlat, E., Parker, J. E., Brownell, K., … Epel, E. S. (2023). Intergenerational effects of maternal lifetime stressor exposure on offspring telomere length in Black and White women. Psychological Medicine, 53(13), 61716182. doi:10.1017/S0033291722003397CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., & Lambert, H. K. (2014). Childhood adversity and neural development: Deprivation and threat as distinct dimensions of early experience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 47, 578591. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, M. L. M., Sichko, S., Bui, T. Q., Libowitz, M. R., Shields, G. S., & Slavich, G. M. (2023). Intergenerational transmission of lifetime stressor exposure in adolescent girls at differential maternal risk for depression. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 79(2), 431448. doi:10.1002/jclp.23417CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nayman, S., Schricker, I. F., Reinhard, I., & Kuehner, C. (2023). Childhood adversity predicts stronger premenstrual mood worsening, stress appraisal and cortisol decrease in women with Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder. Frontiers in Endocrinology, 14, 1278531. doi:10.3389/fendo.2023.1278531CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolan, L. N., & Hughes, L. (2022). Premenstrual exacerbation of mental health disorders: A systematic review of prospective studies. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 25(5), 831852. doi:10.1007/s00737-022-01246-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owens, S. A., Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A., Schmalenberger, K. M., Bowers, S., Rubinow, D. R., Prinstein, M. J., … Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A. (2023). Cyclical exacerbation of suicidal ideation in female outpatients: Prospective evidence from daily ratings in a transdiagnostic sample. Journal of Psychopathology and Clinical Science, 132(6), 704715. doi:10.1037/abn0000838CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pustejovsky, J. (2023). clubSandwich: Cluster-Robust (Sandwich) Variance Estimators with Small-Sample Corrections. Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/clubSandwich/index.htmlGoogle Scholar
Revelle, W. (2023). psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research. Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/psych/index.htmlGoogle Scholar
Reynolds, W. M. (1991). Psychometric characteristics of the adult suicidal ideation questionnaire in college students. Journal of Personality Assessment, 56(2), 289307. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa5602_9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roca, C. A., Schmidt, P. J., Smith, M. J., Danaceau, M. A., Murphy, D. L., & Rubinow, D. R. (2002). Effects of metergoline on symptoms in women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(11), 18761881. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.159.11.1876CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, J. M., Barone, J. C., Tauseef, H., Schmalenberger, K. M., Nagpal, A., Crane, N. A., & Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A. (2024). Predicting acute changes in suicidal ideation and planning: A longitudinal study of symptom mediators and the role of the menstrual cycle in female psychiatric outpatients with suicidality. American Journal of Psychiatry, 181(1), 5767. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.20230303CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmalenberger, K. M., Tauseef, H. A., Barone, J. C., Owens, S. A., Lieberman, L., Jarczok, M. N., … Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A. (2021). How to study the menstrual cycle: Practical tools and recommendations. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 123, 104895. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.104895CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, P. J., Nieman, L. K., Danaceau, M. A., Adams, L. F., & Rubinow, D. R. (1998). Differential behavioral effects of gonadal steroids in women with and in those without premenstrual syndrome. New England Journal of Medicine, 338(4), 209216. doi:10.1056/NEJM199801223380401CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skilbeck, K. J., Johnston, G. A. R., & Hinton, T. (2018). Long-lasting effects of early-life intervention in mice on adulthood behaviour, GABAA receptor subunit expression and synaptic clustering. Pharmacological Research, 128, 179189. doi:10.1016/j.phrs.2017.09.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M. (2019). Stressnology: The primitive (and problematic) study of life stress exposure and pressing need for better measurement. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 75, 35. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2018.08.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M., & Shields, G. S. (2018). Assessing lifetime stress exposure using the stress and adversity inventory for adults (adult STRAIN): An overview and initial validation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 80(1), 1727. doi:10.1097/PSY.0000000000000534CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, E. M., Cardoso, G. M. P., Martinez, P. E., Rubinow, D. R., & Schmidt, P. J. (2012). Rapid response to fluoxetine in women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 29(6), 531540.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stenson, A. F., Nugent, N. R., van Rooij, S. J. H., Minton, S. T., Compton, A. B., Hinrichs, R., & Jovanovic, T. (2021). Puberty drives fear learning during adolescence. Developmental Science, 24(1), e13000. doi:10.1111/desc.13000CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sturmbauer, S. C., Shields, G. S., Hetzel, E.-L., Rohleder, N., & Slavich, G. M. (2019). The stress and adversity inventory for adults (adult STRAIN) in German: An overview and initial validation. PLOS ONE, 14(5), e0216419. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0216419CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wikman, A., Sacher, J., Bixo, M., Hirschberg, A. L., Kopp Kallner, H., Epperson, C. N., … Sundström Poromaa, I. (2022). Prevalence and correlates of current suicidal ideation in women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder. BMC Women's Health, 22(1), 35. doi:10.1186/s12905-022-01612-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographics, stressor exposure, and baseline clinical categories in participants (N = 114)

Figure 1

Table 2. Lifetime stressors predict affective symptom cyclicity

Figure 2

Figure 1. Lifetime stressors predict symptom trajectories across the menstrual cycle. Model-implied values of symptom trajectories across the menstrual cycle by number of lifetime stressors where squares represent more 75th percentile of number of stressors in the sample (34 stressors; P75), triangles represent 50th percentile (23 stressors; P50), and circles represent 25th percentile (15 stressors; P25). L, luteal phase; F, follicular phase. (A) Daily negative affect is a daily mean score of the core emotional symptoms of the daily record of severity of problems (rated from 1 = ‘Not at All’ to 6 = ‘ Extremely’). Marginal significance (p = 0.072) in the interaction between lifetime stressors and menstrual cycle time at > 15 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [− 18 143.50, 15.65]) (B) Daily passive SI is a daily mean score of items 1, 9, and 19 of the Adult Suicidal Ideation Questionnaire, as well as ‘I wished I could go to sleep and not wake up’ (rated from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely)). Marginal significance (p = 0.076) in the interaction between lifetime stressors and menstrual cycle time at >22 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [− 12 632.59, 22.52]). (C) Active SI items included items 2, 17, and 25 of the Adult Suicidal Ideation Questionnaire, as well as ‘I wanted to kill myself’. On days when the participant rated any of the above active SI items greater than a 1 (not at all), that day was assigned a value of 0 for active SI; otherwise, they were assigned a value of 1. Odds of active SI were predicted by the number of lifetime stressors. Significance (p < 0.05) in the interaction between lifetime stressors and menstrual cycle time at >26 stressors (of outside Johnson–Neyman interval [−82.21, 26.01]).

Figure 3

Figure 2. Interpersonal loss stressors predict symptom trajectories across the menstrual cycle. Model-implied values of symptom trajectories across the menstrual cycle by number of interpersonal loss stressors where squares represent more 75th percentile of number of loss stressors in the sample (7.75 stressors; P75), triangles represent 50th percentile (5 stressors; P50), and circles represent 25th percentile (3 stressors; P25). L, luteal phase; F, follicular phase. (A) Daily negative affect is a daily mean score of the core emotional symptoms of the daily record of severity of problems (rated from 1 = ‘Not at All’ to 6 = ‘ Extremely’). Significance (p < 0.05) in the interaction between menstrual cycle time and loss stressors at > 3 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [−34.35, 3.64]). (B) Daily passive SI is a daily mean score of items 1, 9, and 19 of the Adult Suicidal Ideation Questionnaire, as well as ‘I wished I could go to sleep and not wake up’ (rated from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely)). Significance (p < 0.05) in the interaction between menstrual cycle time and loss stressors at >7 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [0.43, 6.83]). (C) Active SI items included items 2, 17, and 25 of the Adult Suicidal Ideation Questionnaire, as well as ‘I wanted to kill myself’. On days when the participant rated any of the above active SI items greater than a 1 (not at all), that day was assigned a value of 0 for active SI; otherwise, they were assigned a value of 1. Significance (p < 0.05) in the interaction between menstrual cycle time and loss stressors at > 5 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [−2.18, 5.55]). Odds of active SI were predicted by the number of loss stressors.

Figure 4

Figure 3. Lifetime stressors predict mood swings, rejection sensitivity, anger and irritability, and interpersonal conflict trajectories across the menstrual cycle. Model-implied values of symptom trajectories across the menstrual cycle by number of lifetime stressors where squares represent more 75th percentile of number of stressors in the sample (34 stressors; P75), triangles represent 50th percentile (23 stressors; P50), and circles represent 25th percentile (15 stressors; P25). L, luteal phase; F, follicular phase. (A) Daily mood swings (rated from 1 = ‘Not at All’ to 6 = ‘ Extremely’) across the menstrual cycle are predicted by number of lifetime stressors. Significance (p < 0.05) in the interaction between menstrual cycle time and stressors at > 13 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [−98.05, 12.61]). (B) Daily rejection sensitivity (rated from 1 = ‘Not at All’ to 6 = ‘ Extremely’) across the menstrual cycle are predicted by number of lifetime stressors. Marginal significance (p = 0.079) in the interaction between lifetime stressors and menstrual cycle time at >22 stressors (inside Johnson–Neyman interval [22.40, 731.81]). (C) Daily anger and irritability (rated from 1 = ‘Not at All’ to 6 = ‘ Extremely’) across the menstrual cycle are predicted by number of lifetime stressors. Significance (p < 0.05) in the interaction between menstrual cycle time and stressors at > 18 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [−455.46, 18.05]). (D) Daily interpersonal conflict (rated from 1 = ‘Not at All’ to 6 = ‘ Extremely’) across the menstrual cycle are predicted by number of lifetime stressors. Significance (p < 0.05) in the interaction between menstrual cycle time and stressors at > 31 stressors (outside Johnson–Neyman interval [1.84, 31.00]).

Supplementary material: File

Nagpal et al. supplementary material 1

Nagpal et al. supplementary material
Download Nagpal et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 27.6 KB
Supplementary material: File

Nagpal et al. supplementary material 2

Nagpal et al. supplementary material
Download Nagpal et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 486.7 KB
Supplementary material: File

Nagpal et al. supplementary material 3

Nagpal et al. supplementary material
Download Nagpal et al. supplementary material 3(File)
File 449 KB
Supplementary material: File

Nagpal et al. supplementary material 4

Nagpal et al. supplementary material
Download Nagpal et al. supplementary material 4(File)
File 286.9 KB