Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T17:13:26.442Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cross-cultural validation of the revised Green et al., paranoid thoughts scale

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 February 2024

Björn Schlier
Affiliation:
Universität Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany University of Wuppertal, Wuppertal, Germany
Tania M. Lincoln
Affiliation:
Universität Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
Jessica L. Kingston
Affiliation:
Royal Holloway, University of London, London, UK
Suzanne H. So
Affiliation:
The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR
Brandon A. Gaudiano
Affiliation:
Brown University, Providence, RI, USA
Eric M. J. Morris
Affiliation:
La Trobe University, Melbourne, Australia
Lyn Ellett*
Affiliation:
University of Southampton, Southampton, UK
*
Corresponding author: Lyn Ellett; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

With efforts increasing worldwide to understand and treat paranoia, there is a pressing need for cross-culturally valid assessments of paranoid beliefs. The recently developed Revised Green et al., Paranoid Thoughts Scale (R-GPTS) constitutes an easy to administer self-report assessment of mild ideas of reference and more severe persecutory thoughts. Moreover, it comes with clinical cut-offs for increased usability in research and clinical practice. With multiple translations of the R-GPTS already available and in use, a formal test of its measurement invariance is now needed.

Methods

Using data from a multinational cross-sectional online survey in the UK, USA, Australia, Germany, and Hong Kong (N = 2510), we performed confirmatory factory analyses on the R-GPTS and tested for measurement invariance across sites.

Results

We found sufficient fit for the two-factor structure (ideas of reference, persecutory thoughts) of the R-GPTS across cultures. Measurement invariance was found for the persecutory thoughts subscale, indicating that it does measure the same construct across the tested samples in the same way. For ideas of reference, we found no scalar invariance, which was traced back to (mostly higher) item intercepts in the Hong Kong sample.

Conclusion

We found sufficient invariance for the persecutory thoughts scale, which is of substantial practical importance, as it is used for the screening of clinical paranoia. A direct comparison of the ideas of reference sum-scores between cultures, however, may lead to an over-estimation of these milder forms of paranoia in some (non-western) cultures.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Research on the aetiology and treatment of paranoia has grown exponentially over the last few decades, resulting in the necessity to develop reliable and valid self-report instruments to quantify current levels of, and longitudinal changes in, paranoia. Consequently, several self-report questionnaires of paranoid beliefs have been developed for use in clinical and nonclinical populations, including for example the Paranoia Scale (Fenigstein & Vanable, Reference Fenigstein and Vanable1992), Peters' Delusions Inventory (Peters, Joseph, & Garety, Reference Peters, Joseph and Garety1999), the Personal Experiences of Paranoia Scale (Ellett, Lopes, & Chadwick, Reference Ellett, Lopes and Chadwick2003), the Paranoia Checklist (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Garety, Bebbington, Smith, Rollinson, Kuipers and Katarzyna2005), and the Green Paranoid Thoughts Scale (Green et al., Reference Green, Freeman, Kuipers, Bebbington, Fowler, Dunn and Garety2008). These questionnaires have refined the assessment of paranoid beliefs and have provided a sound base for aetiological, epidemiological, and intervention research.

Among these questionnaires, the Revised Green et al., Paranoid Thoughts Scale (R-GPTS; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Loe, Kingdon, Startup, Molodynski, Rosebrock and Bird2021) stands out for multiple reasons. First, building on prior theoretical (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Garety, Bebbington, Smith, Rollinson, Kuipers and Katarzyna2005) and empirical evidence (Moritz, Van Quaquebeke, & Lincoln, Reference Moritz, Van Quaquebeke and Lincoln2012) showing that paranoid thoughts are multifaceted, it includes putatively more common social evaluative concerns and ideas of reference and more severe persecutory thoughts as separate subscales. Second, its validation is based on a particularly large sample (i.e. 8386 non-clinical individuals and 2165 patients with psychosis; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Loe, Kingdon, Startup, Molodynski, Rosebrock and Bird2021). Third, a recent systematic review (Statham, Emerson, & Rowse, Reference Statham, Emerson and Rowse2019) identified the Green et al., Paranoid Thoughts Scale as the most valid and accurate questionnaire of paranoid thoughts in general population and clinical samples due to the fact that it covers the full range of mild to severe paranoid beliefs, has the most clearly defined construct underlying its items, and shows the comparatively best psychometric properties. Fourth, its validation included determining latent construct ranges that can be meaningfully interpreted as classes ranging from ‘average’ to ‘very severe’ levels of ideas of reference/persecutory thoughts. Finally, some of these class cut-offs correspond to validated cutoffs for clinical levels of paranoia (persecutory thoughts sum-score of 11) and likely persecutory delusions (persecutory thoughts sum-score of 18; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Loe, Kingdon, Startup, Molodynski, Rosebrock and Bird2021). Specifically, the cut-off between mildly elevated and moderate levels of paranoia (i.e. ‘clinical levels of paranoia’, sum-score⩾11) correspond to the optimal point identified from receiver-operator curves to differentiate between patients with clinical levels of persecutory delusions and non-clinical participants (sensitivity = 0.93, specificity = 0.85). The cut-off between moderate and severe levels of paranoia (i.e. ‘likely persecutory delusions’, sum-score⩾18) in turn corresponds to the cut-off optimized for minimal false positives (specificity = 0.93) while also maintaining sufficient levels of sensitivity (0.81) to detect patients with clinical persecutory delusions (for more details, see Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Loe, Kingdon, Startup, Molodynski, Rosebrock and Bird2021).

Despite being comparatively novel, the R-GPTS has already been used in numerous studies including interventions to reduce paranoia (Brown, Waite, Rovira, Nickless, & Freeman, Reference Brown, Waite, Rovira, Nickless and Freeman2020; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Lambe, Kabir, Petit, Rosebrock, Yu and West2022), epidemiological studies (Rek et al., Reference Rek, Freeman, Reinhard, Bühner, Grosen, Falkai and Padberg2022), and experimental studies (see Ellett et al., Reference Ellett, Varese, Owens, Rafiq, Penn and Berry2023b for a review) to explore the causal mechanisms of paranoia (Barnby, Mehta, & Moutoussis, Reference Barnby, Mehta and Moutoussis2022) in various countries. Furthermore, the R-GPTS has also already been translated into various languages, including French (Latteur, Larøi, & Bortolon, Reference Latteur, Larøi and Bortolon2022), Polish (Kowalski, Marchlewska, Molenda, Górska, & Gawęda, Reference Kowalski, Marchlewska, Molenda, Górska and Gawęda2020), as well as German and Chinese (Kingston et al., Reference Kingston, Schlier, Ellett, So, Gaudiano, Morris and Lincoln2023a). With this multi-cultural, multi-lingual implementation of the R-GPTS comes the implicit assumption that its psychometric properties (including its UK-based cutoffs; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Loe, Kingdon, Startup, Molodynski, Rosebrock and Bird2021) can be readily used in different contexts. To date, however, there have been no formal tests of the measurement invariance (i.e. equivalence of the assessed construct) of the R-GPTS across cultures and translated versions. Thus, we have yet to determine whether paranoia as assessed and quantified by the R-GPTS has the same meaning to people from different countries or whether language differences or differences in people's reaction to the item content preclude a direct comparison of scores across cultural groups. Commonly, three core components of measurement invariance are needed for a comparison of means of the latent constructs measured by a questionnaire (Putnick & Bornstein, Reference Putnick and Bornstein2016): First, configural invariance needs to be tested. In case of the R-GPTS, configural invariance is the equivalence of the underlying model of paranoia consisting of the two factors ideas of reference and persecutory thoughts. If a similar pattern of items loading on the respective factors can be established, it is then tested whether the item loadings are equal across group, i.e., whether the difference between two response options for any item is indicative of the same difference in the latent construct of ideas of reference/persecutory thoughts across cultural groups (metric invariance). Finally, scalar invariance is tested. Scalar invariance means that not only the loadings of items, but also their intercept is equal across the groups, meaning that the mean differences in the latent constructs of paranoia capture all mean differences in the variance the items share with their factor. In other words, no item introduces a systematic over- or underestimation of the latent construct in any of the cultural groups. Only if such a level of measurement invariance can be established is it possible to directly compare means between cultural groups or to establish the validity of cut-offs across cultures.

To address this gap in the literature, in this study, we examine the measurement invariance of the R-GPTS. Using data from an existing multinational cross-sectional survey with data from the UK, USA, Australia, Germany, and Hong Kong, we tested the R-GPTS for (1) configural invariance regarding its two-factor structure, followed by (2) metric invariance and scalar invariance, which would be prerequisites for meaningfully comparing sum-scores across countries. We hypothesized that the R-GPTS would show configural, metric, and scalar invariance.

However, if no invariance for the full R-GPTS was found, we aimed to explore (3) whether invariance can be established for the ideas of reference or persecutory thoughts subscale alone, and (4) whether invariance can be found within different countries and/or language versions of the R-GPTS.

Methods

Design and procedure

This study uses data from a cross-sectional online survey. Data were collected between February and March 2021 across the UK, USA, Australia, Germany, and Hong Kong on the topic of vaccine hesitancy and pandemic-specific paranoia as well as general suspiciousness; Kingston et al., Reference Kingston, Schlier, Ellett, So, Gaudiano, Morris and Lincoln2023a, Reference Kingston, Schlier, Lincoln, So, Gaudiano, Morris and Ellett2023b; Lincoln et al., Reference Lincoln, Schlier, Strakeljahn, Gaudiano, So, Kingston and Ellett2022; So et al., Reference So, Zhu, Lincoln, Gaudiano, Kingston, Ellett and Morris2022). Participants completed a questionnaire battery that included the R-GPTS, pandemic specific paranoid beliefs (pandemic paranoia scale; Kingston et al., Reference Kingston, Schlier, Ellett, So, Gaudiano, Morris and Lincoln2023a; Ellett et al., Reference Ellett, Schlier, Kingston, Zhu, So, Lincoln and Gaudiano2023a), indicators of mental health in general (e.g. depression, anxiety, stress, worrying) as well as various resilience and risk factors (e.g. trauma experiences, core beliefs about one-self and others). In the current study, only the R-GPTS was analyzed.

Ethical approval was obtained separately from local ethics committees at each of the host sites. Potential participants were contacted by Qualtrics to take part. Consenting participants completed the questionnaires online via Qualtrics and were reimbursed via Qualtrics sampling services. Participants were required to respond to all questions on each page before progressing through the survey. Data accuracy was optimized by (1) including five attention check questions across the survey. Participants had to correctly respond to all five attention checks to be included. Moreover, (2) participants completing the survey at less than half of the median completion time and (3) participants with a geographical location outside the corresponding site location were excluded. Participants who did not fulfill the data accuracy requirements, did not give their informed consent to their data being used or dropped out without completing the full questionnaire battery were excluded at source by Qualtrics (excluded participants: n = 3555).

Participants

Participants were recruited via Qualtrics using stratified quota sampling. Each sample was stratified to be representative of the respective general population in terms of sex, age, and level of education. Sample size was determined based on the minimum sample size to validate the newly constructed pandemic paranoia scale (Kingston et al., Reference Kingston, Schlier, Ellett, So, Gaudiano, Morris and Lincoln2023a). A total of 2510 participants (UK n = 512, USA n = 535, Australia n = 502, Germany n = 516, and Hong Kong n = 445) met quota and quality assurance conditions and were included in the final sample. Sample characteristics are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1. Demographic and clinical sample characteristics by site

Measure

R-GPTS

The Revised Green et al. Paranoid Thoughts Scale (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Loe, Kingdon, Startup, Molodynski, Rosebrock and Bird2021) is an 18-item measure that comprises two subscales: ideas of reference (8 items, e.g. ‘I have been upset by friends and colleagues judging me critically’) and paranoia/persecutory thoughts (10 items, .e.g. ‘I was sure someone wanted to hurt me’). Items are rated on a 5-point scale of 0 – not at all to 4 – totally. Sum-scores for ideas of reference (range: 0–32) and persecutory thoughts (range: 0–40) are usually calculated and provide the base for categorization from ‘average’ to ‘very severe’. For the persecutory thoughts subscale, a sum-score of 18 or more is indicative of persecutory delusions. In the current sample, Cronbach's alpha was excellent for both subscales (ideas of reference: α = 0.94; persecutory thoughts: α = 0.96).

For the UK, USA, and Australia, we used the original English version of the R-GPTS. For the German site, we used an existing translation of the GPTS as a starting point (Watzke & Schwenke, Reference Watzke and Schwenke2014) for the translation of the R-GPTS. Translated and original items were compared for any discrepancies and changes to the wording of the German version were added based on consensus between TML and a graduate level psychologist from the German site. Reliability of the German R-GPTS has been tested in two concurrent, independent studies that are as yet unpublished (see Schönig, Krkovic, & Lincoln, Reference Schönig, Krkovic and Lincoln2022a, Reference Schönig, Krkovic and Lincoln2022b for corresponding pre-registrations). Preliminary results from these studies (sample sizes n = 50, and n = 31, respectively) showed good reliability for both ideas of reference (0.83 ⩽ α ⩽ 0.89), persecutory thoughts (0.87 ⩽ α = 0.91), and the full scale (0.93 ⩽ α ⩽ 0.93) as well as high correlations with the PSYRATS delusion subscale (n = 31, 0.55 ⩽ r ⩽ 0.61). For the Hong Kong site, we translated the R-GPTS to Traditional Chinese (i.e. the writing system that is used in both HK and Taiwan): First, the English scale was translated into Chinese, followed by a back-translation of the Chinese version into English, and a check for consistency between the back-translated version and the original English version. After a discussion of discrepancies, fine-tuning of the wording for the translated version resulted in the final Chinese version of the R-GPTS. The translation and back-translation were conducted independently by bilingual, graduate-level psychologists. Comparison of the original v. back-translated version and decisions for final changes were based on consensus between the two translating psychologists, with SHS acting as consultant if needed. The traditional Chinese version of the R-GPTS has been used in another study (Chau et al., Reference Chau, So, Sun, Zhu, Chiu, Chan and Leung2022), where it showed an excellent Cronbach's alpha (α = 0.95). The German and Traditional Chinese versions of the R-GPTS have been added as an online Supplement to this article.

Strategy for data analyses

Analyses were conducted using R 4.2.2 and the R-package lavaan (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012). For all analyses, we calculated confirmatory factor analyses with Satorra-Bentler scaled test statistic. We tested measurement invariance in the following three steps: (1) configural invariance, (2a) invariance of loading (i.e. metric invariance), and (2b) invariance of loadings and intercepts (i.e. scalar invariance). Configural invariance was determined by the three indicators CFI (good fit CFI > 0.95, sufficient fit CFI > 0.90), RMSEA (good fit RMSEA < 0.06, sufficient fit RMSEA < 0.08), and SRMR (SRMR < 0.08; for information of thresholds on all indicators, see Chen, Reference Chen2007). Metric invariance was assessed on the basis of differences in fit indices in comparison to the configural invariance model, using the cutoffs ΔCFI > −0.010, ΔRMSEA < 0.015, and ΔSRMR < 0.030 as an indication of metric invariance (Meade, Johnson, & Braddy, Reference Meade, Johnson and Braddy2008). For the assessment of scalar invariance, differences in fit indices in comparison to the metric invariance model were used, with the cut-offs ΔCFI > −0.010, ΔRMSEA < 0.015, and ΔSRMR < 0.015 indicating scalar invariance (Meade et al., Reference Meade, Johnson and Braddy2008).

First, we tested invariance for the full 18-item R-GPTS with the full sample. Second, if indicators of non-invariance were found, we followed up with secondary analyses of invariance of the R-GPTS ideas of reference and R-GPTS persecutory thoughts subscales to determine the source of invariance in relation to specific subscales.

Finally, (3) we repeated the invariance analyses in language and culture specific subsamples. We performed a measurement invariance analyses across the English speaking countries (UK, USA, Australia) to explore whether the R-GPTS shows invariance across countries with the same (English) version of the scale. Next, we followed up by adding either the German sample or the Hong Kong sample in two additional invariance analyses to explore if and how invariance differs across different language/culture-dyads.

Results

Full sample invariance analyses

Results of the invariance analyses of the full sample (N = 2510) are summarized in Table 2. Measurement invariance analyses of the two-factor model (1. ideas of reference, 2. persecutory thoughts) yielded sufficient fit for the configural and metric invariance model. For scalar invariance, invariance was supported by RMSEA (RMSEA = 0.62, ΔRMSEA = 0.006) and SRMR (SRMR = 0.066, ΔSRMR = 0.005), but not by CFI (CFI = 0.930, ΔCFI = 0.018).

Table 2. Measurement invariance confirmatory factor analyses on the full five-site sample (N = 2510)

Note. Cells printed in bold denote indicators of non-invariance.

When analyzing the subscales of the R-GPTS separately, we again found sufficient fit for configural and metric invariance for both subscales (see Table 2). There was insufficient fit for scalar invariance, however, with one indicator showing insufficient fit for the persecutory thoughts subscale (CFI = 0.953, ΔCFI = 0.011) and all three indicators showing insufficient fit for the ideas of reference subscale (CFI = 0.937, ΔCFI = 0.034; RMSEA = 0.087, ΔRMSEA = 0.022; SRMR = 0.073, ΔSRMR = 0.017).

Language subsample invariance analyses

When analyzing the subsamples from the English speaking countries (UK, USA, and Australia), invariance analysis yielded sufficient fit for configural, metric, and scalar invariance (see Table 3). These results were similar, when the two subscales (ideas of reference and persecutory thoughts) were analyzed separately (see online Supplement).

Table 3. Measurement invariance results for the full R-GPTS two-factor model in language and culture subsamples

Note. Cells printed in bold denote indicators of non-invariance.

When analyzing the English and German subsamples without the Hong Kong sample, the full scale analyses yielded configural, metric, or scalar invariance (see Table 3). When analyzing the subscales separately, we found invariance for the persecutory thoughts subscale, whereas scalar invariance was not found for the ideas of reference subscale (χ2(122) = 658.83, χ2(122)scaled = 375.12, CFI = 0.967, ΔCFI = 0.013, RMSEA = 0.063, ΔRMSEA = 0.008, SRMR = 0.056, ΔSRMR = 0.006, see online Supplement for more details).

Analyses of the English and Hong Kong subsamples yielded configural and metric invariance, but difference in CFI did not support scalar invariance for the full scale (CFI = 0.909, ΔCFI = 0.026). The separate analyses of the subscales yielded non-invariance for the ideas of reference subscale (χ2(122) = 944.96, χ2(122)scaled = 584.61, CFI = 0.939, ΔCFI = 0.029, RMSEA = 0.087, ΔRMSEA = 0.018, SRMR = 0.074, ΔSRMR = 0.015). For the persecutory thoughts subscale, all indicators support scalar invariance (χ2(194) = 1282.21, χ2(194)scaled = 554.27, CFI = 0.960, ΔCFI = 0.008, RMSEA = 0.061, ΔRMSEA = 0.003, SRMR = 0.057, ΔSRMR = 0.002, see online Supplement).

To better understand the nature of the invariance, we repeated the factor analyses while freeing one item intercept, respectively, and comparing the pooled estimate for the item intercept with the freely estimated intercept for the Hong Kong sample. The resulting comparisons yielded substantially higher intercept estimates in the Hong Kong v. pooled sample the items 7 (‘I believed that certain people were not what they seemed’, Δintercept = 0.57) and 3 (‘I have been upset by friends and colleagues judging me critically’, Δintercept = 0.31) and a lower estimate for item 5 (‘I have been thinking a lot about people avoiding me’, Δintercept = −0.28).

Discussion

In this study, we explored the measurement invariance of the Revised Green at al. Paranoid Thoughts Scale (R-GPTS). We found configural invariance, supporting the notion that paranoia can be differentiated in milder, more common symptoms that encompass ideas of reference and other forms of social evaluative concerns on the one hand, and more severe persecutory thoughts on the other hand (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Loe, Kingdon, Startup, Molodynski, Rosebrock and Bird2021). These findings are consistent with continuum models (Strauss, Reference Strauss1969; Verdoux & van Os, Reference Verdoux and van Os2002) and provide further support for a multifaceted model of paranoid beliefs (Green et al., Reference Green, Freeman, Kuipers, Bebbington, Fowler, Dunn and Garety2008; Moritz et al., Reference Moritz, Van Quaquebeke and Lincoln2012). We add to this by showing empirically that a two factor model of paranoid beliefs comprising social evaluative beliefs and ideas of reference on the one hand and persecutory beliefs on the other hand appears to be applicable across cultures.

Regarding the more severe form of paranoid beliefs, persecutory thoughts, we further found substantial evidence for scalar invariance for the corresponding subscale of the R-GPTS. This is no small feat, since prior tests of paranoia measures across cultures showed no scalar invariance for paranoia (sub)scales (Heuvelman, Nazroo, & Rai, Reference Heuvelman, Nazroo and Rai2018; Jaya et al., Reference Jaya, Wüsten, Alizadeh, Van Amelsvoort, Bartels-Velthuis, Van Beveren and Lincoln2022), which Jaya et al. (Reference Jaya, Wüsten, Alizadeh, Van Amelsvoort, Bartels-Velthuis, Van Beveren and Lincoln2022) highlight as an indication of the ‘general difficulty of constructing an invariant measure of paranoia’. Scalar invariance for the persecutory thoughts subscale in particular is of substantial practical importance, as persecutory thoughts scores have previously been established as the basis for cut-offs in screening for clinical levels of paranoid delusions (Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Loe, Kingdon, Startup, Molodynski, Rosebrock and Bird2021). With evidence for scalar invariance for this subscale, future studies can now test for differences in mean latent construct values across languages/cultures in order to formally verify the validity of the existing cut-off (sum score ⩾ 18) cross-culturally and in other populations than the UK. Assuming that the same level of (the latent of construct of) persecutory beliefs is indicative of the same risk for a psychotic disorder across cultures, researchers and practitioners may provisionally work with the established UK cut-off. However, it needs noting that this equivalence has not been directly tested. Rather, our initial results regarding the invariance of the R-GPTS persecutory thoughts subscale are a promising first step toward cross-culturally valid cut-offs for this self-report questionnaire. What is needed to complete validation of the cut-offs is a comparison of mean latent scores between samples that are optimized for the testing of cut-offs (i.e. samples stratified for mental health status).

Regarding the less severe form of paranoid beliefs, ideas of reference, we found metric but not scalar invariance. In other words, agreeing more with the R-GPTS ideas of reference items corresponds with equal gradual increases of the latent construct of ideas of reference across cultures. However, the intercept of these items varies across cultures, meaning that individual items can add an over- or underestimation of the latent construct of ideas of reference. In practice, this means that comparison of the latent mean scores of the R-GPTS ideas of reference factor between samples from different cultures is at risk for biased results. Interestingly, this scalar invariance was limited to the comparison of the three English-speaking samples (UK, USA, and Australia) and the Hong Kong sample. When only the English-speaking or the English-speaking and German-speaking samples were used for invariance analyses, we found no indication of non-invariance for ideas of reference. Possibly, cultural differences in trust-formation in Western v. Eastern societies (Yuki, Maddux, Brewer, & Takemura, Reference Yuki, Maddux, Brewer and Takemura2005) that may be correlated with the degree of collectivism of the respective culture (Westjohn, Magnusson, Franke, & Peng, Reference Westjohn, Magnusson, Franke and Peng2022) influence what constitutes the norm/absence of elevated social-evaluative concerns, though this would need to be established in future research. In line with this, Jaya et al. (Reference Jaya, Wüsten, Alizadeh, Van Amelsvoort, Bartels-Velthuis, Van Beveren and Lincoln2022) already pointed out that the assessment of paranoia in self-report is intertwined with objective levels of (social) threat. These levels may greatly differ as a function of geographical location, culture-specific social norms, and/or differences in the social status of certain minorities that consequently face higher or lower levels of threat in different places. The combination of non-invariance for ideas of reference and scalar invariance for persecutory thoughts could point toward qualitative differences in the building blocks of the hierarchy of paranoia. In particular, what constitutes an elevated level of more mundane forms of mistrust may vary with cultural standards, whereas the starting point for what can be considered more severe or pathological forms of paranoia is universal and appears to be largely unaffected by cultural differences. In the case of ideas of reference, we found overall more items with an increased than a lower intercept when comparing with the Hong Kong sample. Specifically, participants from Hong Kong show higher scores in items that deal with a not directly disclosed social judgment by others (i.e. ‘believing others are not what they seem’ and ‘being upset by being critically judged’). This sample-specific tendency is unrelated to the ideas of reference. Instead, it may be a result of a culture-specific difference in norms and expectation in social interactions. Thus, a person from Hong Kong is likely to score higher in ideas of reference than a person from the UK, USA, or Australia, even when both participants have the same true value. At the same time, a minority of items with a lower intercept in Hong Kong v. the other samples (for the item dealing with ‘concerns regarding other people avoiding the respondent’) make it difficult to predict the exact direction of the biased estimate for an individual participant.

Strengths and limitations

While the recruitment with stratified quota sampling constitutes a major strength of this study in terms of generalizability, it needs noting that the sampling was conducted via recruitment in the Panel samples provided by Qualtrics, impacting the representativeness of the sample to some degree. We purposefully sampled region-specific representative samples, yet there are groups that could nonetheless be under-represented, such as those with limited access to a computer or the internet. It needs noting though that a study comparing different online-recruitment procedures found Qualtrics to be the best option in terms of achieving approximate demographic representativeness and geographic representativeness in a high-income country (when compared to equally common commercial providers such as MTurk or Facebook-advertisement; Boas, Christenson, & Glick, Reference Boas, Christenson and Glick2020). Divergences from representativeness are gradual, and indications of some limitations regarding the participant pool have been documented for people with high income and people of higher age (i.e. aged 50 and older, Miller, Guidry, Dahman, & Thomson, Reference Miller, Guidry, Dahman and Thomson2020). To our knowledge, however, there is no existing research detailing potential selection bias in terms of clinical variables. Related to this, it needs noting that invariance-assessments of non-clinical and clinical samples (i.e. patients with psychosis) might show diverging results, necessitating future tests in clinical samples to further verify our initial findings. Furthermore, the lack of a more objective criterion to validate the R-GPTS, in particular other self-report- or interview-based assessment of paranoid delusions, prevented us from further criterion validation with the present data. Further, our data may have been impacted by the Covid-19 pandemic, given the timing of data collection. With COVID-19 affecting the five international sites differently at the time of recruitment, it is possible that the mean scores of persecutory thoughts may have temporarily shifted to different degrees in the sample, preventing us from disentangling permanent mean differences in persecutory thoughts across the five sites from transitory fluctuations due to the Covid-19 pandemic. Ideally, future research should collect data at several assessment points in different cultures to disentangle fluctuations in latent means due to, for example public or political crises, from stable intercultural differences in mean persecutory thoughts. Such an effort would allow for either the generalization of the established UK cut-offs or the generation of improved cross-cultural or culture-specific cut-offs. Finally, while there was some cultural variation in the samples analyzed, all five sites constitute high-income regions, so a more extensive follow-up to this study is needed to further verify invariance across high, middle, and low income countries.

Conclusion

This analysis of measurement invariance of the R-GPTS showed that its persecutory thoughts subscale is a reliable and valid self-report measure of severe levels of paranoia, which provides the first step toward an unbiased assessment across cultures and cross-cultural verification of cut-off criteria. At the same time, our analyses revealed that a direct comparison of the ideas of reference subscale sum-scores between cultures may lead to an over-estimation of these milder forms of paranoia in some (non-western) cultures. In sum, the R-GPTS constitutes a valuable tool for researchers and practitioners assessing, treating, and exploring the phenomenon of paranoid beliefs.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724000072

References

Barnby, J. M., Mehta, M. A., & Moutoussis, M. (2022). The computational relationship between reinforcement learning, social inference, and paranoia. PLOS Computational Biology, 18(7), e1010326. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1010326CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boas, T. C., Christenson, D. P., & Glick, D. M. (2020). Recruiting large online samples in the United States and India: Facebook, mechanical turk, and qualtrics. Political Science Research and Methods, 8(2), 232250. https://doi.org/10.1017/psrm.2018.28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, P., Waite, F., Rovira, A., Nickless, A., & Freeman, D. (2020). Virtual reality clinical-experimental tests of compassion treatment techniques to reduce paranoia. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 8547. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64957-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chau, A. K. C., So, S. H. W., Sun, X., Zhu, C., Chiu, C. D., Chan, R. C., & Leung, P. W. (2022). The co-occurrence of multidimensional loneliness with depression, social anxiety and paranoia in non-clinical young adults: A latent profile analysis. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13, 931558. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.931558CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14(3), 464504. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510701301834CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellett, L., Lopes, B., & Chadwick, P. (2003). Paranoia in a nonclinical population of college students. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 191(7), 425430. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.NMD.0000081646.33030.EFCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellett, L., Schlier, B., Kingston, J. L., Zhu, C., So, S. H., Lincoln, T. M., … Gaudiano, B. A. (2023a). Pandemic paranoia in the general population: International prevalence and sociodemographic profile. Psychological Medicine, 53(12), 57485755. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291722002975.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellett, L., Varese, F., Owens, J., Rafiq, S., Penn, G., & Berry, K. (2023b). Experimental studies of paranoid thinking in clinical and nonclinical populations: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 52, 59335944. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723001708CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenigstein, A., & Vanable, P. A. (1992). Paranoia and self-consciousness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(1), 129138. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.62.1.129CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D., Garety, P. A., Bebbington, P. E., Smith, B., Rollinson, R., Kuipers, E., … Katarzyna, R. (2005). Psychological investigation of the structure of paranoia in a non-clinical population psychological investigation of the structure of paranoia in a non-clinical population. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 186(5), 427435. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.186.5.427CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, D., Lambe, S., Kabir, T., Petit, A., Rosebrock, L., Yu, L.-M., … West, J. (2022). Automated virtual reality therapy to treat agoraphobic avoidance and distress in patients with psychosis (gameChange): A multicentre, parallel-group, single-blind, randomised, controlled trial in England with mediation and moderation analyses. The Lancet Psychiatry, 9(5), 375388. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(22)00060-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D., Loe, B. S., Kingdon, D., Startup, H., Molodynski, A., Rosebrock, L., … Bird, J. C. (2021). The revised Green et al., Paranoid Thoughts Scale (R-GPTS): Psychometric properties, severity ranges, and clinical cut-offs. Psychological Medicine, 51(2), 244253. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719003155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, C. E. L., Freeman, D., Kuipers, E., Bebbington, P., Fowler, D., Dunn, G., Garety, P. A. (2008). Measuring ideas of persecution and social reference: The Green et al. paranoid thought scales (GPTS). Psychological Medicine, 38(1), 101111. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291707001638CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heuvelman, H., Nazroo, J., & Rai, D. (2018). Investigating ethnic variations in reporting of psychotic symptoms: A multiple-group confirmatory factor analysis of the psychosis screening questionnaire. Psychological Medicine, 48(16), 27572765. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291718000399CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaya, E. S., Wüsten, C., Alizadeh, B. Z., Van Amelsvoort, T., Bartels-Velthuis, A. A., Van Beveren, N. J.Lincoln, T. M. (2022). Comparing psychotic experiences in low-and-middle-income-countries and high-income-countries with a focus on measurement invariance. Psychological Medicine, 52(8), 15091516. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720003323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kingston, J. L., Schlier, B., Ellett, L., So, S. H., Gaudiano, B. A., Morris, E. M. J., & Lincoln, T. M. (2023a). The pandemic paranoia scale (PPS): Factor structure and measurement invariance across languages. Psychological Medicine, 53(6), 26522661. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721004633CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kingston, J. L., Schlier, B., Lincoln, T., So, S. H., Gaudiano, B. A., Morris, E. M. J., … Ellett, L. (2023b). Paranoid thinking as a function of minority group Status and intersectionality: An international examination of the role of negative beliefs. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 49(4), 10781087. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbad027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kowalski, J., Marchlewska, M., Molenda, Z., Górska, P., & Gawęda, Ł (2020). Adherence to safety and self-isolation guidelines, conspiracy and paranoia-like beliefs during COVID-19 pandemic in Poland - associations and moderators. Psychiatry Research, 294, 113540. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113540CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Latteur, A., Larøi, F., Bortolon, C. (2022). Translation and validation of the French version of the revised Green et al., paranoid thoughts scale (R-GPTS) in two samples: Non-clinical and clinical adults. Psychologica Belgica, 62(1), 208217. https://doi.org/10.5334/pb.1134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lincoln, T. M., Schlier, B., Strakeljahn, F., Gaudiano, B. A., So, S. H., Kingston, J., … Ellett, L. (2022). Taking a machine learning approach to optimize prediction of vaccine hesitancy in high income countries. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 2055. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-05915-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meade, A. W., Johnson, E. C., & Braddy, P. W. (2008). Power and sensitivity of alternative fit indices in tests of measurement invariance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(3), 568592. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.93.3.568CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, C. A., Guidry, J. P., Dahman, B., & Thomson, M. D. (2020). A tale of two diverse qualtrics samples: Information for online survey researchers. Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention, 29(4), 731735. https://doi.org/10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-19-0846CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moritz, S., Van Quaquebeke, N., & Lincoln, T. M. (2012). Jumping to conclusions is associated with paranoia but not general suspiciousness: A comparison of two versions of the probabilistic reasoning paradigm. Schizophrenia Research and Treatment, 2012, 19. https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/384039CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, E. R., Joseph, S. A., Garety, P. A. (1999). Measurement of delusional ideation in the normal population: Introducing the PDI (Peters et al. delusions inventory). Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25(3), 553576. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnick, D. L., & Bornstein, M. H. (2016). Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: The state of the art and future directions for psychological research. Developmental Review, 41, 7190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2016.06.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rek, S. V., Freeman, D., Reinhard, M. A., Bühner, M., Grosen, S., Falkai, P., … Padberg, F. (2022). Differential psychological response to the COVID-19 pandemic in psychiatric inpatients compared to a non-clinical population from Germany. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 272(1), 6779. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-021-01291-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 136.10.18637/jss.v048.i02CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schönig, S., Krkovic, K., & Lincoln, T. M. (2022a). Safety behaviours in paranoid individuals' daily lives. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/8TCHNCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schönig, S., Krkovic, K., & Lincoln, T. M. (2022b). The role of safety behaviours for the maintenance of paranoid beliefs. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/ZGX92CrossRefGoogle Scholar
So, S. H., Zhu, C., Lincoln, T. M., Gaudiano, B. A., Kingston, J. L., Ellett, L., & Morris, E. M. J. (2022). Pandemic paranoia, general paranoia, and their relationships with worry and beliefs about self/others – A multi-site latent class analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 241, 122129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2022.01.045CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Statham, V., Emerson, L.-M., & Rowse, G. (2019). A systematic review of self-report measures of paranoia. Psychological Assessment, 31(2), 139158. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000645CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, J. S. (1969). Hallucinations and delusions as points on continua function. Archives of General Psychiatry, 21(5), 581. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1969.01740230069010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verdoux, H., & van Os, J. (2002). Psychotic symptoms in non-clinical populations and the continuum of psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 54(1–2), 5965. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00352-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watzke, S., & Schwenke, J. (2014). Irrationale Überzeugungen und paranoia. Nervenheilkunde, 33(09), 617625. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0038-1627720Google Scholar
Westjohn, S. A., Magnusson, P., Franke, G. R., & Peng, Y. (2022). Trust propensity across cultures: The role of collectivism. Journal of International Marketing, 30(1), 117. https://doi.org/10.1177/1069031X211036688CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yuki, M., Maddux, W. W., Brewer, M. B., & Takemura, K. (2005). Cross-cultural differences in relationship- and group-based trust. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(1), 4862. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167204271305CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and clinical sample characteristics by site

Figure 1

Table 2. Measurement invariance confirmatory factor analyses on the full five-site sample (N = 2510)

Figure 2

Table 3. Measurement invariance results for the full R-GPTS two-factor model in language and culture subsamples

Supplementary material: File

Schlier et al. supplementary material

Schlier et al. supplementary material
Download Schlier et al. supplementary material(File)
File 34.1 KB