Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T00:53:28.104Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A contextual approach to trauma experience: lessons from life events research

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 April 2019

Ruth Spence
Affiliation:
Centre for Abuse and Trauma Studies, Psychology Department, Middlesex University, London, UK
Lisa Kagan
Affiliation:
Centre for Abuse and Trauma Studies, Psychology Department, Middlesex University, London, UK
Antonia Bifulco*
Affiliation:
Centre for Abuse and Trauma Studies, Psychology Department, Middlesex University, London, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Antonia Bifulco, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Editorial
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Defining trauma

There has been much debate over what constitutes trauma experiences, how these are differentiated from other very negative events, and the importance of personal impact – for example, witnessing v. experiencing events. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.; DSM–5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) defines post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) trauma events specifically as those where the individual experiences, witnesses or is confronted with actual/threatened death, serious injury or sexual violence. Whilst experiencing a Criterion-A defined traumatic event is a necessary component of the PTSD diagnosis, it is clearly not a sufficient explanation of disorder risk. Studies show significant between- and within-event variation on disorder impact (Darves-Bornoz et al., Reference Darves-Bornoz, Alonso, de Girolamo, de Graaf, Haro, Kovess-Masfety, Lepine, Nachbaur, Negre-Pages, Vilagut and Gasquet2008; Carmassi et al., Reference Carmassi, Dell'Osso, Manni, Candini, Dagani, Iozzino, Koenen and de Girolamo2014) suggesting that (a) some trauma events are much more prone to provoking PTSD than others and (b) that after experiencing the same trauma event type, some individuals will go on to develop PTSD while others will remain resilient. Therefore, trauma event characteristics as well as individual vulnerability are required to explain disorder risk.

Currently, individual factors such as sex, age and psychiatric history are known to increase the risk for developing PTSD (Brewin et al., Reference Brewin, Andrews and Valentine2000). However, refining our understanding of trauma events could also further help explain these differences (Kilpatrick et al., Reference Kilpatrick, Resnick and Acierno2009). Indeed, further improving the specifics of trauma experiences might actually help reduce the discrepancy between the incidence of currently defined traumatic events and the much lower rate of PTSD cases found by epidemiological studies (Darves-Bornoz et al., Reference Darves-Bornoz, Alonso, de Girolamo, de Graaf, Haro, Kovess-Masfety, Lepine, Nachbaur, Negre-Pages, Vilagut and Gasquet2008; Digangi et al., Reference Digangi, Guffanti, McLaughlin and Koenen2013; Carmassi et al., Reference Carmassi, Dell'Osso, Manni, Candini, Dagani, Iozzino, Koenen and de Girolamo2014). Certainly, it is important to increase our knowledge of risk and resilience surrounding trauma, as PTSD is associated with some of the widest use of health care systems and a high associated cost per patient (Kessler, Reference Kessler2000; Boscarino, Reference Boscarino2004).

We believe the DSM implication that trauma events are uniquely different from other types of negative life events and are limited to a few specific types of predetermined event that can be identified categorically and of which only one or two may occur in a given case, hinders the utility of current trauma definitions in research and practice. Whilst we do not take issue with the standard definition of trauma events, a binary checklist-style approach to such events has a potential for missing relevant experience. Instead, we argue that trauma events should be considered to be at one end of a continuum of threat, with potential for more or less ‘threat to life’ in a range of domains. For example, a serious violent attack from a partner with injuries sustained could be differentiated from lesser conflict and verbal threats which may precede it. Yet both are likely to add to the duration of threat and impact, and individuals seeking treatment may be at different phases in a developing scenario. Similarly witnessing violence may be considered less threatening (or of different valence) than being the recipient in many cases. Taking a wider assessment of adverse experience around an identified trauma with the grading of ‘threat to life’ for the individual or another can help to contextualise the experience and predict or understand the impact. This might help explain why some negative life events which fall short of current trauma definitions, such as certain instances of infidelity and relationship disturbances, can be related to risk of PTSD (Gold et al., Reference Gold, Marx, Soler-Baillo and Sloan2005; Van Hooff et al., Reference Van Hooff, McFarlane, Baur, Abraham and Barnes2009; Catherall, Reference Catherall and Ardino2011) and why traumatic events can fail to demonstrate this association if experienced indirectly (Anders et al., Reference Anders, Frazier and Frankfurt2011). Furthermore, a wider assessment of events could elucidate a greater range of co-existing trauma events, or those sub-threshold experiences related to the phasing of trauma, and other unrelated negative life events which may contribute to context and impact (e.g. reducing resources).

A contextual approach to severity

Whilst there are a few intensive life events interview measures, largely developed in the 1980s and 1990s (Paykel, Reference Paykel1997), the Life Events and Difficulties Schedule (LEDS; Brown and Harris, Reference Brown and Harris1978) has been utilised in a body of research and is often considered a ‘gold standard’ of this type of approach. The LEDS uses intensive interview questioning to provide an objective assessment of an event's threat (i.e. where it sits on the spectrum of negativity) through close examination of the event itself, its context and the resulting inferred objective life change for the individual (Brown and Harris, Reference Brown and Harris1978). This occurs through precedent ratings and some panel reviews for reliability purposes.

A ‘severe’ life event is one that involves an enduring or relatively extensive negative life change (with high score of threat or unpleasantness), including those requiring substantial cognitive reappraisal, lasting at least 2 weeks post-event and impacting either solely or jointly on the individual/someone close (Brown and Harris, Reference Brown and Harris1978). The level of threat (scored on a four-point scale) is determined using information about both current circumstances and relevant past biography, as well as evidence for relevant plans, purposes and concerns held by the respondent at the time of the event (Brown, Reference Brown, Bennett and Freeman1991). Rating of ‘marked’ threat is relatively unusual and involves aspects such as bereavement of someone close (i.e. where loss is permanent) or violent attack (i.e. where real physical danger is involved). This is all rated free from the actual emotional response or disorder occasioned and based on the response of the ‘average person’ in similar circumstances. Thus, it reflects factual information independent of symptomatology and emotional response about change, focusing on potential negative impacts.

However, to our knowledge, the theoretical grounding and methodology used in life events research have not been specifically applied to trauma exposure. Using life events procedures for identifying a wider range of negatively life-changing events may serve to identify multiple trauma experiences wider than those restricted to current Criterion-A definitions, identifying related severe events or chronic problems which add to the trauma context, severity and ‘dosage’, or explicate the causes of comorbid disorder adding to both research prediction and treatment focus.

The research on trauma implies that for many individuals, traumatic events occur within a context of other negative experiences such as ongoing deprivation or substance abuse (Hamel and Pampalon, Reference Hamel and Pampalon2002; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Ford, Howard and Bradford2010). Moreover, severe life events tend to cluster within the same individuals (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Neale, Kessler, Heath and Eaves1993; Foley et al., Reference Foley, Neale and Kendler1996; Bifulco et al., Reference Bifulco, Brown, Moran, Ball and Campbell1998). Thus, the clustering of severe events and their likely effect on coping/support resources may help explain why PTSD is frequently comorbid with other psychiatric diagnoses (Grubaugh et al., Reference Grubaugh, Long, Elhai, Frueh and Magruder2010; Contractor et al., Reference Contractor, Durham, Brennan, Armour, Wutrick, Frueh and Elhai2014), as well as why events that do not meet Criterion-A can be related to PTSD (Bodkin et al., Reference Bodkin, Pope, Detke and Hudson2007; Van Hooff et al., Reference Van Hooff, McFarlane, Baur, Abraham and Barnes2009).

Severe events not reaching current trauma criterion evoke a range of emotional reactions such as anger, sadness and anxiety as well as negative cognitions around pessimism, self-doubt and hopelessness. This increase in general psychological distress can reduce an individual's available coping resources, which in turn may increase the likelihood of psychological disorder in general. Indeed, it is well documented that severe life events are related to onset of major depression (e.g. Brown et al., Reference Brown, Harris and Hepworth1995; Bifulco et al., Reference Bifulco, Brown, Moran, Ball and Campbell1998; McQuaid et al., Reference McQuaid, Monroe, Roberts, Kupfer and Frank2000) and other psychopathology such as schizophrenia, eating disorders and bipolar disorder (Hultman et al., Reference Hultman, Wieselgren and Öhman1997; Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Tiller, Blanchard, Andrews and Treasure1997; Hosang et al., Reference Hosang, Uher, Maughan, McGuffin and Farmer2012; Beards et al., Reference Beards, Gayer-Anderson, Borges, Dewey, Fisher and Morgan2013). Alternatively, a traumatic event may itself increase psychological distress reducing the ability to cope with other life events, further increasing distress and the likelihood of PTSD symptoms along with other disorders. Therefore, it should be unsurprising to conclude that the co-occurrence of severe events with present or past trauma(s) could be important for both PTSD and comorbid psychopathology.

This is borne out by research which illustrates that experiencing a severe negative life event can add to the impact of trauma experiences (Brewin et al., Reference Brewin, Andrews and Valentine2000). Studies show that delayed PTSD onset is often associated with experiencing severe life events, sometimes many years after the initial trauma (Andrews et al., Reference Andrews, Brewin, Rose and Kirk2000; Boscarino and Adams, Reference Boscarino and Adams2009; Horesh et al., Reference Horesh, Solomon, Zerach and Ein-Dor2011) and negative life events contribute to PTSD and depression comorbidity (Jin et al., Reference Jin, Sun, Wang, An and Xu2018). Additionally, PTSD symptomatology increases after experiencing a subsequent severe life event or trauma (Schock et al., Reference Schock, Böttche, Rosner, Wenk-Ansohn and Knaevelsrud2016).

This illustrates the importance for both clinicians and researchers to examine in detail not only the index trauma(s) but any stressful experiences surrounding the trauma. Those who fail to assess the impact of other contextual factors might misattribute the cause of distress or ignore their intersecting effects which could be decreasing the resilience and increasing the likelihood of disorder, greater symptomatology or comorbidity.

Possible dimensions of trauma events

Life events have also been analysed in terms of characteristics or dimensions which cut across the usual event categories (such as partner, housing, parenthood) (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Harris and Hepworth1995) and these may be usefully attributed to trauma events. One of these is loss, defined broadly as the loss of a person (attachment threat), role (identity threat), important plan (achievement threat) or cherished idea about the self (identity threat). The permanence of such loss denotes higher severity ratings with bereavement having particular prominence. Although PTSD Criterion-A does not specifically mention loss, its inclusion of death, serious injury and sexual assault may all involve aspects of loss, i.e. death breaks attachments, and injury or assault may lead to the loss of role and functioning central to self-concept. Events involving emotional loss have been found to be significantly associated with PTSD symptoms above and beyond Criterion-A stressors (Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Smith and Dalenberg2013). This indicates not only that loss events provoke severe emotional pain of the sort consistent with Criterion-A, but suggests that other loss events may additionally be considered traumatic. Certainly, bereavement is commonly associated with PTSD symptoms in the general population (Zisook et al., Reference Zisook, Chentsova-Dutton and Shuchter1998; O'Connor, Reference O'Connor2010) even when non-violent, but with sudden and untimely elements such as the death of a child from chronic illness.

Danger events are those clearly indicating a future loss or security threat, threats to plans (achievement) and threats to ideas about the self, particularly when they are associated with behavioural commitment (identity), with those threatening to life and safety having highest severity. Similar to loss, it could be implied that the Criterion-A events of threatened death, injury or assault carry high weightings of danger to security and potentially identity and attachments. Cognitive models of PTSD argue that trauma events violate formerly held beliefs and lead to cognitive restructuring around concepts of safety and self-assessment (Brewin, Reference Brewin2014). Indeed, individuals who experience threats to safety show a raised likelihood of PTSD, including war veterans and victims of stalking or natural disasters (Xu and Liao, Reference Xu and Liao2011; Norris and Slone, Reference Norris and Slone2013; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Benjet, Bromet, Cardoso, Degenhardt, de Girolamo, Ferry, Florescu, Gureje, Haro, Huang, Karam, Kawakami, Lee, Lepine, Levinson, Navarro-Mateu, Pennell, Piazza, Posada-Villa, Scott, Stein, Ten Have, Torres, Viana, Petukhova, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Koenen2017). The perception of life threat is significantly associated with PTSD (Larsen and Berenbaum, Reference Larsen and Berenbaum2017), even after adjusting for objective trauma exposure (Heir et al., Reference Heir, Blix and Knatten2016).

Another severe life event dimension, humiliation, involves rejection, devaluation and role failure (attachment and identity threats), leading to a sense of shame or devaluation with more public events and those in areas of high commitment being more severe. Feelings of anger, shame and guilt are often associated with trauma, and humiliation may underlie some of the association between traumatic events and PTSD (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Scragg and Turner2001). For example, experiencing shame and anger at others after a violent event is predictive of PTSD (Andrews et al., Reference Andrews, Brewin, Rose and Kirk2000). More broadly, research suggests that negative social events involving public humiliation, ridicule or rejection can be experienced as more distressing than those meeting Criterion-A (Carleton et al., Reference Carleton, Peluso, Collimore and Asmundson2011) and lead to PTSD symptoms (Erwin et al., Reference Erwin, Heimberg, Marx and Franklin2006; Guðmundsdóttir, Reference Guðmundsdóttir2016). There is also evidence that persistent humiliation can lead to significantly lower psychological functioning than periodic exposure to violence (Barber et al., Reference Barber, McNeely, Olsen, Belli and Doty2016).

Entrapment is a characteristic of both severe events and related long-term problems, where there is an erosion of hope with events confirming imprisonment in an ongoing, highly negative situation (security threat), with the most severe involving little chance of escape. Torture, hostage situations and domestic abuse are all cases which could easily fulfil Criterion-A but also have features of entrapment in the cultivation of hostile and punishing environments. Certainly, feelings of entrapment are significantly associated with PTSD (Griffiths et al., Reference Griffiths, Wood, Maltby, Taylor, Panagioti and Tai2015; Siddaway et al., Reference Siddaway, Taylor, Wood and Schulz2015) and are strongly related to suicidal behaviour in those with PTSD (Panagioti et al., Reference Panagioti, Gooding, Taylor and Tarrier2012). These can encompass events not in themselves traumatic, such as carer experience, which can be associated with PTSD when involving perceptions of entrapment (van den Born-van Zanten et al., Reference Van den Born-van Zanten, Dongelmans, Dettling-Ihnenfeldt, Vink and van der Schaaf2016). Similar findings hold for entraping experiences of parents of children with chronic illnesses (Cabizuca et al., Reference Cabizuca, Marques-Portella, Mendlowicz, Coutinho and Figueira2009) and victims of school and workplace bullying (Nielsen et al., Reference Nielsen, Tangen, Idsoe, Matthiesen and Magerøy2015).

Testing overlap of markedly severe events, their attributes and trauma events

In order to examine how attributes of severe events may overlap with trauma classifications, a secondary analysis of published data of LEDS events was undertaken. In a London community sample of 110 vulnerable women seen prospectively (Bifulco et al., Reference Bifulco, Brown, Moran, Ball and Campbell1998), the LEDS interview classified 1232 events. The analysis examined only those with the most ‘marked’ threat rating taken to be similar in severity to trauma events. A post hoc analysis applied the Life Events Checklist for DSM-5 (LEC-5; Weathers et al., Reference Weathers, Blake, Schnurr, Kaloupek, Marx and Keane2013) classification to summarised events (inter-rater reliability of κ = 0.90) and found 4.4% of events meeting Criterion-A Trauma across 40 respondents. Most were physical assault (39%) or death/life-threatening illness (28%). There were fewer sexual assaults (11%), accidents (13%) or severe human suffering (e.g. suicide attempt of other, stillbirth) (9%). Analysis showed that of those categorised as having LEC-5 trauma events, most (88%) had also experienced at least one other severe event as identified in the LEDS. Thus, trauma events and severe life events co-occur in the same respondents.

The overlap of ‘markedly’ severe life events and LEC-5 trauma events was further examined. This yielded 60 events across 22 respondents. There was a modest agreement between the two scorings (κ = 0.36, p < 0.0001). The LEDS ‘marked threat’ rating had a specificity of 97% (95% CI 95.60–97.71) but a sensitivity of only 41% (95% CI 27.57–54.97) with the trauma classification, and a PPV of 37% and an NPV of 97%. Thus LEDS ‘marked’ threat events failed to detect 59% of trauma events, but incorrectly identified only 3%. The two measures are overlapping but by no means identical.

The examination of pre-rated severe events features (i.e. loss, danger, humiliation, entrapment) and LEC-5 trauma classification for this group of ‘marked’ threat events are shown in Table 1. Nearly all those classified as trauma were scored additionally as having danger or loss with a further two categorised as humiliation. For non-trauma events, there was a wider spread of classifications including humiliation and entrapment.

Table 1. The number (%) of LEDS ‘marked’ severity and Criterion-A traumatic events classified by each threat type in a London sample (Bifulco et al., Reference Bifulco, Brown, Moran, Ball and Campbell1998)

This brief analysis suggests that trauma events commonly occur with severe life events, are a subset of events with marked threat and similarly have characteristics of loss, danger and humiliation.

The LEDS approach is thorough but very time consuming and therefore expensive. A new online approach – Computerised Life Events Assessment Record CLEAR – mimics aspects of the interview ratings with good reliability and validity and is able to provide ratings of the threat as well as the further characteristics in a large number of event categories (Bifulco et al., Reference Bifulco, Spence, Nunn, Kagan, Rodriguez, Hosang and Fisher2019). The overlap of trauma and other severe events and their characteristics have been investigated with some success in relation to depression (Bifulco et al., Reference Bifulco, Kagan, Spence, Nunn, Hosang, Taylor and Fisherunder review). Whilst these scores constitute reported rather than investigator-coded characteristics of events, aids to rating online through written and video instruction, and detailed labelling of rating points with examples potentially objectifies ratings. In addition, the potential for providing algorithms for comparing detailed and time-based demographic information to underpin severity ratings is present. Given trauma is a characteristic rated online, this has the potential for beneficial use in trauma-related research and services but requiring fewer resources than the intensive interview.

Treatment implications

There is already some support for the further specification of the trauma concept for PTSD along potential dimensions already used for severe life events research. Potentially, adopting both a dimensional approach to investigating what makes negative life events traumatic and what other attributes are particularly traumagenic would increase the breadth of experience encompassed but also the potential specific meaning of different subsets of trauma experience for the individual. Equally, this more qualitative or ‘meaning’ approach to understanding trauma would allow for a more considered approach when formulating treatment, placing the individual within their context at the time of the event but also in encompassing other related stressful events which impinge on the individual but may also have implications for managing trauma (e.g. through loss of other close support figures).

Using greater specification in assessing the characteristics of trauma events may aid not only in refining diagnosis but also in treatment or intervention. Thus, psychotherapeutic techniques appropriate to grief, over-vigilance, damaged self-concept or helplessness can be used to supplement those already available for less specified trauma. Whether or not the trauma situation is ended/resolved or still active is relevant for appropriate levels of vigilance experienced. Any of these approaches would also potentially benefit from a detailed knowledge of other severe life events or long-term problems still in evidence which may create further future risk or limit support or resource (escape from a violent partner also involving loss of home and financial support). Whilst intensive interviews may be ruled out for time and resource issues, an online assessment of severe life events and trauma, based on the LEDS and undertaken outside of clinician time may aid in improved treatment approaches as well as client understanding of their experience (Bifulco et al., Reference Bifulco, Spence, Nunn, Kagan, Rodriguez, Hosang and Fisher2019). Failing this, clinicians simply being aware of distinctions in trauma characteristics (regarding loss, danger, humiliation and entrapment) and applying these to the trauma situations described, may find new themes for treatment. This would not necessarily involve extensive questioning, rather a deeper understanding of components of an identified trauma experience.

Conclusions

We have argued that trauma events should be considered in more varied terms, for example, on a spectrum of magnitude, in relation to phasing and tied to contextualised life event threat approaches. Specifically, we have highlighted the potential significance of loss, danger, humiliation and entrapment when rating the traumatic nature and likely impact of events. These attributes may be crucial in refining our understanding of not only why some events are considered traumatic, but their specific impacts, for instance, the likely repercussions of a traumatic loss compared with traumatic danger such as ongoing violence.

In addition, we have argued the importance of both current and longer-term context is critical, especially as negative experiences including traumatic events tend to cluster within individuals and their effects can have multiple and cumulative impacts. Critically, traumatic experiences are rarely isolated events and the unique impact of any given trauma may be difficult to ascertain. We believe a dimensional approach to the characteristics of trauma taken from life events research could have the potential for greater clarification of trauma attributes and severity. This would have direct implications for a more person-focused treatment of trauma. It could also inform predictions of future life trajectories to distress and disorder.

Author ORCIDs

Ruth Spence, 0000-0002-6197-9975

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013) The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.; DSM–5). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Anders, S, Frazier, P and Frankfurt, S (2011) Variations in criterion A and PTSD rates in a community sample of women. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 25, 176184.Google Scholar
Andrews, B, Brewin, CR, Rose, S and Kirk, M (2000) Predicting PTSD symptoms in victims of violent crime: the role of shame, anger, and childhood abuse. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109, 6973.Google Scholar
Barber, BK, McNeely, C, Olsen, JA, Belli, RF and Doty, SB (2016) Long-term exposure to political violence: the particular injury of persistent humiliation. Social Science & Medicine 156, 154166.Google Scholar
Beards, S, Gayer-Anderson, C, Borges, S, Dewey, M, Fisher, H and Morgan, C (2013) Life events and psychosis: a review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 39, 740747.Google Scholar
Bifulco, A, Brown, G, Moran, P, Ball, C and Campbell, C (1998) Predicting depression in women: the role of past and present vulnerability. Psychological Medicine 28, 3950.Google Scholar
Bifulco, A, Kagan, L, Spence, R, Nunn, S, Hosang, GM, Taylor, M and Fisher, HL (under review) Characteristics of severe life events, attachment style and depression – using a new online approach.Google Scholar
Bifulco, A, Spence, R, Nunn, S, Kagan, L, Rodriguez, D, Hosang, GM and Fisher, HL (2019) The computerised life events and assessment record (CLEAR) online measure of life events: reliability, validity and association with depression. JMIR Mental Health 6, e10675.Google Scholar
Bodkin, JA, Pope, HG, Detke, MJ and Hudson, JI (2007) Is PTSD caused by traumatic stress? Journal of Anxiety Disorders 21, 176182.Google Scholar
Boscarino, J (2004) Posttraumatic stress disorder and physical illness: results from clinical and epidemiologic studies. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1032, 141153.Google Scholar
Boscarino, JA and Adams, RE (2009) PTSD onset and course following the World Trade Center disaster: findings and implications for future research. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 44, 887898.Google Scholar
Brewin, C (2014) Episodic memory, perceptual memory, and their interaction: foundations for a theory of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Bulletin 140, 6997.Google Scholar
Brewin, C, Andrews, B and Valentine, J (2000) Meta-analysis of risk factors for posttraumatic stress disorder in trauma-exposed adults. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 68, 748766.Google Scholar
Brown, GW (1991) A psychosocial view of depression. In Bennett, D and Freeman, H (ed.), Community Psychiatry. London: Churchill-Livingstone, pp. 71114.Google Scholar
Brown, GW and Harris, T (1978) Social Origins of Depression – A Study of Psychiatric Disorder in Women. London: Tavistock.Google Scholar
Brown, GW, Harris, T and Hepworth, C (1995) Loss, humiliation and entrapment among women developing depression: a patient and non-patient comparison. Psychological Medicine 25, 721.Google Scholar
Cabizuca, M, Marques-Portella, C, Mendlowicz, MV, Coutinho, ESF and Figueira, I (2009) Posttraumatic stress disorder in parents of children with chronic illnesses: a meta-analysis. Health Psychology 28, 379388.Google Scholar
Carleton, RN, Peluso, DL, Collimore, KC and Asmundson, GJ (2011) Social anxiety and posttraumatic stress symptoms: the impact of distressing social events. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 25, 4957.Google Scholar
Carlson, E, Smith, S and Dalenberg, C (2013) Can sudden, severe emotional loss be a traumatic stressor? Journal of Trauma & Dissociation 14, 519528.Google Scholar
Carmassi, C, Dell'Osso, L, Manni, C, Candini, V, Dagani, J, Iozzino, L, Koenen, KC and de Girolamo, G (2014) Frequency of trauma exposure and Post-Traumatic stress disorder in Italy: analysis from the World Mental Health Survey initiative. Journal of Psychiatric Research 59, 7784.Google Scholar
Catherall, D (2011) The impact of lower magnitude stressors on child and adolescent development: The Family Context. In Ardino, V (ed.), Post-Traumatic Syndromes in Childhood and Adolescence. A Handbook of Research and Practice. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd, pp. 6376.Google Scholar
Contractor, AA, Durham, TA, Brennan, JA, Armour, C, Wutrick, HR, Frueh, BC and Elhai, JD (2014) DSM-5 PTSD's symptom dimensions and relations with major depression's symptom dimensions in a primary care sample. Psychiatry Research 215, 146153.Google Scholar
Darves-Bornoz, JM, Alonso, J, de Girolamo, G, de Graaf, R, Haro, JM, Kovess-Masfety, V, Lepine, JP, Nachbaur, G, Negre-Pages, L, Vilagut, G and Gasquet, I (2008) Main traumatic events in Europe: PTSD in the European study of the epidemiology of mental disorders survey. Journal of Traumatic Stress 21, 455462.Google Scholar
Digangi, J, Guffanti, G, McLaughlin, K and Koenen, K (2013) Considering trauma exposure in the context of genetics studies of posttraumatic stress disorder: a systematic review. Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders 3, 2.Google Scholar
Erwin, BA, Heimberg, RG, Marx, BP and Franklin, ME (2006) Traumatic and socially stressful life events among persons with social anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 20, 896914.Google Scholar
Foley, D, Neale, M and Kendler, K (1996) A longitudinal study of stressful life events assessed at interview with an epidemiological sample of adult twins: the basis of individual variation in event exposure. Psychological Medicine 26, 12391252.Google Scholar
Gold, SD, Marx, BP, Soler-Baillo, J and Sloan, DM (2005) Is life stress more traumatic than traumatic stress? Journal of Anxiety Disorders 19, 687698.Google Scholar
Griffiths, AW, Wood, AM, Maltby, J, Taylor, PJ, Panagioti, M and Tai, S (2015) The development of the Short Defeat and Entrapment Scale (SDES). Psychological Assessment 27, 11821194.Google Scholar
Grubaugh, AL, Long, ME, Elhai, JD, Frueh, BC and Magruder, KM (2010) An examination of the construct validity of posttraumatic stress disorder with veterans using a revised criterion set. Behaviour Research and Therapy 48, 909914.Google Scholar
Guðmundsdóttir, K (2016) The Impact of Social Trauma among Outpatients with Social Anxiety Disorder Compared to Individuals with no Mental Disorders. (Unpublished Canadian psychology thesis). University of Iceland, Department of Psychology. Available at https://skemman.is/bitstream/1946/24878/1/Cand.psych%20lokaverkefni%202016.pdf (Accessed 27 September 2018).Google Scholar
Hamel, D and Pampalon, R (2002) Trauma and deprivation in Quebec. National Public Health Institute of Quebec.Google Scholar
Heir, T, Blix, I and Knatten, CK (2016) Thinking that one's life was in danger: perceived life threat in individuals directly or indirectly exposed to terror. The British Journal of Psychiatry 209, 306310.Google Scholar
Horesh, D, Solomon, Z, Zerach, G and Ein-Dor, T (2011) Delayed-onset PTSD among war veterans: the role of life events throughout the life cycle. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 46, 863870.Google Scholar
Hosang, G, Uher, R, Maughan, B, McGuffin, P and Farmer, A (2012) The role of loss and danger events in symptom exacerbation in bipolar disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research 46, 15841589.Google Scholar
Hultman, CM, Wieselgren, I and Öhman, A (1997) Relationships between social support, social coping and life events in the relapse of schizophrenic patients. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 38, 313.Google Scholar
Jin, Y, Sun, C, Wang, F, An, J and Xu, J (2018) The relationship between PTSD, depression and negative life events: Ya'an earthquake three years later. Psychiatry Research 259, 358363.Google Scholar
Kendler, K, Neale, M, Kessler, R, Heath, A and Eaves, L (1993) A twin study of recent life events and difficulties. Archives of General Psychiatry 50, 789796.Google Scholar
Kessler, R (2000) Posttraumatic stress disorder: the burden to the individual and to society. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 61, 412.Google Scholar
Kessler, R, Aguilar-Gaxiola, S, Alonso, J, Benjet, C, Bromet, EJ, Cardoso, G, Degenhardt, L, de Girolamo, RV, Ferry, F, Florescu, S, Gureje, O, Haro, JM, Huang, Y, Karam, EG, Kawakami, N, Lee, S, Lepine, JP, Levinson, D, Navarro-Mateu, F, Pennell, BE, Piazza, M, Posada-Villa, J, Scott, KM, Stein, DJ, Ten Have, M, Torres, Y, Viana, MC, Petukhova, MV, Sampson, NA, Zaslavsky, AM and Koenen, KC (2017) Trauma and PTSD in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. European Journal of Psychotraumatology 8. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2017.1353383.Google Scholar
Kilpatrick, D, Resnick, H and Acierno, R (2009) Should PTSD Criterion A be retained? Journal of Traumatic Stress 22, 374383.Google Scholar
Kim, MM, Ford, JD, Howard, DL and Bradford, DW (2010) Assessing trauma, substance abuse, and mental health in a sample of homeless men. Health & Social Work 35, 3948.Google Scholar
Larsen, SE and Berenbaum, H (2017) Did the DSM-5 improve the traumatic stressor criterion?: Association of DSM-IV and DSM-5 Criterion A with posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms. Psychopathology 50, 373378.Google Scholar
Lee, DA, Scragg, P and Turner, S (2001) The role of shame and guilt in traumatic events: a clinical model of shame-based and guilt-based PTSD. British Journal of Medical Psychology 74, 451466.Google Scholar
McQuaid, J, Monroe, S, Roberts, J, Kupfer, D and Frank, E (2000) A comparison of two life stress assessment approaches: prospective prediction of treatment outcome in recurrent depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109, 787791.Google Scholar
Nielsen, MB, Tangen, T, Idsoe, T, Matthiesen, SB and Magerøy, N (2015) Post-traumatic stress disorder as a consequence of bullying at work and at school. A literature review and meta-analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior 21, 1724.Google Scholar
Norris, FH and Slone, LB (2013) Understanding research on the epidemiology of trauma and PTSD. PTSD Research Quarterly 24, 113.Google Scholar
O'Connor, M (2010) PTSD in older bereaved people. Aging & Mental Health 14, 670678.Google Scholar
Panagioti, M, Gooding, P, Taylor, PJ and Tarrier, N (2012) Negative self-appraisals and suicidal behavior among trauma victims experiencing PTSD symptoms: the mediating role of defeat and entrapment. Depression and Anxiety 29, 187194.Google Scholar
Paykel, ES (1997) The interview for recent life events. Psychological Medicine 27, 301310.Google Scholar
Schmidt, U, Tiller, J, Blanchard, M, Andrews, B and Treasure, J (1997) Is there a specific trauma precipitating anorexia nervosa? Psychological Medicine 27, 523530.Google Scholar
Schock, K, Böttche, M, Rosner, R, Wenk-Ansohn, M and Knaevelsrud, C (2016) Impact of new traumatic or stressful life events on pre-existing PTSD in traumatized refugees: results of a longitudinal study. European Journal of Psychotraumatology 7, 32106. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v7.32106.Google Scholar
Siddaway, AP, Taylor, PJ, Wood, AM and Schulz, J (2015) A meta-analysis of perceptions of defeat and entrapment in depression, anxiety problems, posttraumatic stress disorder, and suicidality. Journal of Affective Disorders 184, 149159.Google Scholar
Van den Born-van Zanten, SA, Dongelmans, DA, Dettling-Ihnenfeldt, D, Vink, R and van der Schaaf, M (2016) Caregiver strain and posttraumatic stress symptoms of informal caregivers of intensive care unit survivors. Rehabilitation Psychology 61, 173178.Google Scholar
Van Hooff, M, McFarlane, A, Baur, J, Abraham, M and Barnes, D (2009) The stressor Criterion-A1 and PTSD: a matter of opinion? Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23, 7786.Google Scholar
Weathers, FW, Blake, DD, Schnurr, PP, Kaloupek, DG, Marx, BP and Keane, TM (2013) The Life Events Checklist for DSM-5 (LEC-5). Instrument available from the National Center for PTSD. Available at http://www.ptsd.va.govGoogle Scholar
Xu, J and Liao, Q (2011) Prevalence and predictors of posttraumatic growth among adult survivors one year following 2008 Sichuan earthquake. Journal of Affective Disorder 133, 274280.Google Scholar
Zisook, S, Chentsova-Dutton, Y and Shuchter, SR (1998) PTSD following bereavement. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry 10, 157163.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. The number (%) of LEDS ‘marked’ severity and Criterion-A traumatic events classified by each threat type in a London sample (Bifulco et al., 1998)