Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T08:15:29.984Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Comprehensive elucidation of resting-state functional connectivity in anorexia nervosa by a multicenter cross-sectional study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 March 2024

Yusuke Sudo
Affiliation:
Research Center for Child Mental Development, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan Department of Cognitive Behavioral Physiology, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan Department of Psychiatry, Chiba University Hospital, Chiba, Japan
Junko Ota
Affiliation:
Research Center for Child Mental Development, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan Applied MRI Research, Department of Molecular Imaging and Theranostics, Institute for Quantum Medical Science, National Institutes for Quantum Science and Technology, Chiba, Japan United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Suita, Japan
Tsunehiko Takamura
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioral Medicine, National Institute of Mental Health, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Kodaira, Japan
Rio Kamashita
Affiliation:
Research Center for Child Mental Development, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Suita, Japan
Sayo Hamatani
Affiliation:
Research Center for Child Mental Development, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Suita, Japan Research Center for Child Mental Development, Fukui University, Eiheizi, Japan
Noriko Numata
Affiliation:
Research Center for Child Mental Development, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Suita, Japan
Ritu Bhusal Chhatkuli
Affiliation:
Research Center for Child Mental Development, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan Applied MRI Research, Department of Molecular Imaging and Theranostics, Institute for Quantum Medical Science, National Institutes for Quantum Science and Technology, Chiba, Japan United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Suita, Japan
Tokiko Yoshida
Affiliation:
Research Center for Child Mental Development, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan
Jumpei Takahashi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Chiba Aoba Municipal Hospital, Chiba, Japan
Hitomi Kitagawa
Affiliation:
Research Center for Child Mental Development, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan
Koji Matsumoto
Affiliation:
Department of Radiology, Chiba University Hospital, Chiba, Japan
Yoshitada Masuda
Affiliation:
Department of Radiology, Chiba University Hospital, Chiba, Japan
Michiko Nakazato
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, International University of Health and Welfare, Narita, Japan
Yasuhiro Sato
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, Tohoku University Hospital, Sendai, Japan
Yumi Hamamoto
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Northumbria University, Newcastle-upon-Tyne, UK Department of Human Brain Science, Institute of Development, Aging, and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
Tomotaka Shoji
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, Tohoku University Hospital, Sendai, Japan Department of Internal Medicine, Nagamachi Hospital, Sendai, Japan Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, Tohoku University School of Medicine, Sendai, Japan
Tomohiko Muratsubaki
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, Tohoku University Graduate School of Medicine, Sendai, Japan
Motoaki Sugiura
Affiliation:
Department of Human Brain Science, Institute of Development, Aging, and Cancer, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan Cognitive Sciences Lab, International Research Institute of Disaster Science, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan
Shin Fukudo
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, Tohoku University Hospital, Sendai, Japan Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, Tohoku University Graduate School of Medicine, Sendai, Japan
Michiko Kawabata
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Kyoto University Graduate School of Medicine, Kyoto, Japan
Momo Sunada
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Kyoto University Graduate School of Medicine, Kyoto, Japan
Tomomi Noda
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Kyoto University Graduate School of Medicine, Kyoto, Japan
Keima Tose
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Kyoto University Graduate School of Medicine, Kyoto, Japan
Masanori Isobe
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Kyoto University Graduate School of Medicine, Kyoto, Japan
Naoki Kodama
Affiliation:
Division of Psychosomatic Medicine, Department of Neurology, University of Occupational and Environmental Health, Kitakyushu, Japan
Shingo Kakeda
Affiliation:
Department of Radiology, Hirosaki University Graduate School of Medicine, Hirosaki, Japan
Masatoshi Takahashi
Affiliation:
Division of Psychosomatic Medicine, Department of Neurology, University of Occupational and Environmental Health, Kitakyushu, Japan
Shu Takakura
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, Kyushu University Hospital, Fukuoka, Japan
Motoharu Gondo
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, Kyushu University Hospital, Fukuoka, Japan
Kazufumi Yoshihara
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, Kyushu University Hospital, Fukuoka, Japan
Yoshiya Moriguchi
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioral Medicine, National Institute of Mental Health, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Kodaira, Japan Department of Sleep-Wake Disorders, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Kodaira, Japan
Eiji Shimizu
Affiliation:
Research Center for Child Mental Development, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan Department of Cognitive Behavioral Physiology, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Suita, Japan
Atsushi Sekiguchi
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioral Medicine, National Institute of Mental Health, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Kodaira, Japan Center for Eating Disorder Research and Information, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Kodaira, Japan Department of Advanced Neuroimaging, Integrative Brain Imaging Center, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Kodaira, Japan
Yoshiyuki Hirano*
Affiliation:
Research Center for Child Mental Development, Chiba University, Chiba, Japan Applied MRI Research, Department of Molecular Imaging and Theranostics, Institute for Quantum Medical Science, National Institutes for Quantum Science and Technology, Chiba, Japan United Graduate School of Child Development, Osaka University, Kanazawa University, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Chiba University and University of Fukui, Suita, Japan
*
Corresponding author: Yoshiyuki Hirano; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Previous research on the changes in resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) in anorexia nervosa (AN) has been limited by an insufficient sample size, which reduced the reliability of the results and made it difficult to set the whole brain as regions of interest (ROIs).

Methods

We analyzed functional magnetic resonance imaging data from 114 female AN patients and 135 healthy controls (HC) and obtained self-reported psychological scales, including eating disorder examination questionnaire 6.0. One hundred sixty-four cortical, subcortical, cerebellar, and network parcellation regions were considered as ROIs. We calculated the ROI-to-ROI rsFCs and performed group comparisons.

Results

Compared to HC, AN patients showed 12 stronger rsFCs mainly in regions containing dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), and 33 weaker rsFCs primarily in regions containing cerebellum, within temporal lobe, between posterior fusiform cortex and lateral part of visual network, and between anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and thalamus (p < 0.01, false discovery rate [FDR] correction). Comparisons between AN subtypes showed that there were stronger rsFCs between right lingual gyrus and right supracalcarine cortex and between left temporal occipital fusiform cortex and medial part of visual network in the restricting type compared to the binge/purging type (p < 0.01, FDR correction).

Conclusion

Stronger rsFCs in regions containing mainly DLPFC, and weaker rsFCs in regions containing primarily cerebellum, within temporal lobe, between posterior fusiform cortex and lateral part of visual network, and between ACC and thalamus, may represent categorical diagnostic markers discriminating AN patients from HC.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Anorexia nervosa (AN) is a psychiatric disorder characterized by food intake restriction leading to significantly lower body weight, intense fear of weight gain, and distorted body image (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The standard mortality rate of AN is as high as 5.86, which is the highest rate among mental disorders (Arcelus, Mitchell, Wales, & Nielsen, Reference Arcelus, Mitchell, Wales and Nielsen2011; Harris & Barraclough, Reference Harris and Barraclough1998). The rate of patients fully recovering from AN is only 46%, and the rate of chronicity is ~20%, indicating that the long-term prognosis of AN can be poor (Steinhausen, Reference Steinhausen2009). Although the etiology of AN is unknown, neurobiological factors for the development of AN are considered because AN patients have common personality traits, such as perfectionism, inflexibility, and obsession (Kaye, Wierenga, Bailer, Simmons, & Bischoff-Grethe, Reference Kaye, Wierenga, Bailer, Simmons and Bischoff-Grethe2013; Zipfel, Giel, Bulik, Hay, & Schmidt, Reference Zipfel, Giel, Bulik, Hay and Schmidt2015).

Many studies investigating resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) have been conducted since the 2010s to elucidate changes in brain function occurring in AN; connectomes based on rsFCs are stable and act as a ‘fingerprint’ that can accurately identify subjects from a large group. Therefore, rsFCs represent a promising predictor of cognitive behavior (Finn et al., Reference Finn, Shen, Scheinost, Rosenberg, Huang, Chun and Constable2015; Horien, Shen, Scheinost, & Constable, Reference Horien, Shen, Scheinost and Constable2019). Brain regions and networks identified as having altered rsFCs in two or more previous studies of AN include insula (Amianto et al., Reference Amianto, D'Agata, Lavagnino, Caroppo, Abbate-Daga, Righi and Fassino2013; Boehm et al., Reference Boehm, Geisler, King, Ritschel, Seidel, Deza Araujo and Ehrlich2014; Ehrlich et al., Reference Ehrlich, Lord, Geisler, Borchardt, Boehm, Seidel and Walter2015; Gaudio, Olivo, Beomonte Zobel, & Schiöth, Reference Gaudio, Olivo, Beomonte Zobel and Schiöth2018; Geisler et al., Reference Geisler, Borchardt, Lord, Boehm, Ritschel, Zwipp and Ehrlich2016; Kullmann et al., Reference Kullmann, Giel, Teufel, Thiel, Zipfel and Preissl2014; Lord et al., Reference Lord, Ehrlich, Borchardt, Geisler, Seidel, Huber and Walter2016), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Gaudio et al., Reference Gaudio, Olivo, Beomonte Zobel and Schiöth2018; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Ran Kim, Ku, Lee, Namkoong and Jung2014), thalamus (Biezonski, Cha, Steinglass, & Posner, Reference Biezonski, Cha, Steinglass and Posner2016; Ehrlich et al., Reference Ehrlich, Lord, Geisler, Borchardt, Boehm, Seidel and Walter2015; Geisler et al., Reference Geisler, Borchardt, Lord, Boehm, Ritschel, Zwipp and Ehrlich2016; Lord et al., Reference Lord, Ehrlich, Borchardt, Geisler, Seidel, Huber and Walter2016), inferior frontal gyrus (Collantoni et al., Reference Collantoni, Michelon, Tenconi, Degortes, Titton, Manara and Favaro2016; Cowdrey, Filippini, Park, Smith, & McCabe, Reference Cowdrey, Filippini, Park, Smith and McCabe2014; Kullmann et al., Reference Kullmann, Giel, Teufel, Thiel, Zipfel and Preissl2014), parietal cortex (Amianto et al., Reference Amianto, D'Agata, Lavagnino, Caroppo, Abbate-Daga, Righi and Fassino2013; Favaro et al., Reference Favaro, Santonastaso, Manara, Bosello, Bommarito, Tenconi and Di Salle2012; Olivo et al., Reference Olivo, Swenne, Zhukovsky, Tuunainen, Salonen-Ros, Larsson and Schiöth2018), cerebellum (Amianto et al., Reference Amianto, D'Agata, Lavagnino, Caroppo, Abbate-Daga, Righi and Fassino2013; Gaudio et al., Reference Gaudio, Olivo, Beomonte Zobel and Schiöth2018; Olivo et al., Reference Olivo, Swenne, Zhukovsky, Tuunainen, Salonen-Ros, Larsson and Schiöth2018), precuneus (Cowdrey et al., Reference Cowdrey, Filippini, Park, Smith and McCabe2014; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Ran Kim, Ku, Lee, Namkoong and Jung2014), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) (Biezonski et al., Reference Biezonski, Cha, Steinglass and Posner2016; Cowdrey et al., Reference Cowdrey, Filippini, Park, Smith and McCabe2014), visual network (Favaro et al., Reference Favaro, Santonastaso, Manara, Bosello, Bommarito, Tenconi and Di Salle2012; Phillipou et al., Reference Phillipou, Abel, Castle, Hughes, Nibbs, Gurvich and Rossell2016; Scaife, Godier, Filippini, Harmer, & Park, Reference Scaife, Godier, Filippini, Harmer and Park2017), and default mode network (DMN) (Boehm et al., Reference Boehm, Geisler, King, Ritschel, Seidel, Deza Araujo and Ehrlich2014; Cowdrey et al., Reference Cowdrey, Filippini, Park, Smith and McCabe2014).

However, the sample sizes of previous studies were small, ranging from 12 to 36 participants per group (Alfano, Mele, Cotugno, & Longarzo, Reference Alfano, Mele, Cotugno and Longarzo2020; Gaudio, Wiemerslage, Brooks, & Schiöth, Reference Gaudio, Wiemerslage, Brooks and Schiöth2016). This limited the reliability of the results, leading to a major source of inconsistency in study results among previous studies (Marek et al., Reference Marek, Tervo-Clemmens, Calabro, Montez, Kay, Hatoum and Dosenbach2022). For example, stronger rsFCs were found in the DMN in certain studies (Boehm et al., Reference Boehm, Geisler, King, Ritschel, Seidel, Deza Araujo and Ehrlich2014; Cowdrey et al., Reference Cowdrey, Filippini, Park, Smith and McCabe2014), whereas in other studies, no differences were found in the DMN between AN patients and healthy controls (HC) (Phillipou et al., Reference Phillipou, Abel, Castle, Hughes, Nibbs, Gurvich and Rossell2016). The lack of sample size also prevents extending the region of interest (ROI) to the whole brain in studies adopting seed-based analysis, which has the advantage of a clarity of meaning when quantifying functional connectivity compared to independent component analysis (ICA) or graph analysis. This is because the greater the number of ROIs used, the greater the number of statistical tests, the stricter the significance level, and the more difficult it is to obtain significant results with small sample size. No meta-analysis exists of studies examining rsFC changes in AN partially due to the insufficient sample size; only meta-analyses of positron emission tomography (PET), single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), arterial spin labeling (ASL), amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (ALFF), and fractional ALFF (fALFF) have been reported, showing bilateral anterior to middle cingulate cortex hypofunction and right parahippocampal gyrus hyperfunction (Su et al., Reference Su, Gong, Tang, Qiu, Chen, Chen and Wang2021). Furthermore, no previous studies have identified differences in rsFCs between AN subtypes (AN restricting type: AN-R and AN binge/purge type: AN-BP), which may also be partly due to the lack of AN participants. Due to these problems stemming from insufficient sample sizes, there is no unified view regarding the brain dysfunctions that may underlie the neurobiology of AN, and many aspects have remained unexplored.

Therefore, in this study, we secured a sample size of more than 100 subjects in each AN and HC group via a multicenter study, and we set whole brain regions as ROIs. The objective of this study was to comprehensively elucidate the resting-state rsFC changes occurring in AN with a high degree of reliability.

Methods

Participants

This study was conducted at five Japanese hospitals: (a) Chiba University Hospital; (b) Hospital of the University of Occupational and Environmental Health (UOEH); (c) Tohoku University Hospital; (d) Kyushu University Hospital; and (e) Kyoto University Hospital. AN patients were recruited from outpatients of these hospitals, as well as from those who applied through the participant recruitment websites and recruitment notices of these hospitals. HC participants were recruited from those who had applied via the participant recruitment websites and recruitment notices in each hospital. Participants for sites (a) through (d) were recruited between July 2015 and March 2021 as part of A Multi-Site Study on the Brain of Eating Disorder Patients, and participants for site (e) were recruited between March 2014 and February 2019. An appropriate sample size for a single group in fMRI studies is reported to be 21 when the type I error rate α is set at 0.002, and twice that number, 42, when multiple comparisons are made (Desmond & Glover, Reference Desmond and Glover2002)., Therefore, we tried to secure a minimum of 50 participants in each group, assuming a dropout rate of about 20%. The number of participants per site is shown in online Supplementary Table S1. Information on race was not collected, but almost all participants were Japanese females. Eligibility criteria for the study required that AN participants met the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, 5th Edition (DSM-5) diagnostic criteria, and HC participants were required to be at least 12 years of age. Diagnosis and AN subtypes were determined by a structured interview based on DSM-5 conducted by psychosomatic physicians or psychiatrists. HC participants had no history of mental disorders and were confirmed to be free of current mental disorders by psychosomatic physicians or psychiatrists. Any subjects with claustrophobia, head trauma, neurological disorders, or substance abuse were excluded from the study. AN participants who had imminent thoughts of death or with a medical history of psychiatric disorders other than depression, bipolar disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, anxiety, and personality disorder were also excluded. Due to their rarity, we could recruit only two male AN participants. Therefore, the two male AN patients and 36 male HC were excluded from the study, and only women were included in the analysis. The final number of participants was 114 in the AN group and 135 in the HC group. In the AN group, 61 patients had AN-R and 53 had AN-BP.

Physical and psychological assessment

Participants' height and weight were measured on the same day as their MRI scans. Participants were also evaluated by Japanese adult reading test: Japanese version of National Adult Reading Test (JART), Stait-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II), and Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire (EDE-Q) within two weeks before and after MRI. EDE-Q consists of four subscales (restraint, eating concern, shape concern, and weight concern), and global score is the average of the subscales' scores. These self-reported psychological scales were collected to characterize the participants and perform a correlation analysis between these scales and rsFCs that showed group differences.

Dataset

The dataset used for the analysis was the secondary release (pilot version), but studies analyzing the primary release dataset will be published. The secondary release dataset (pilot version) includes the following data not included in the primary release dataset: (1) data on duration of illness, psychotropic medication, JART, BDI-II, and STAI for all participants; and (2) data on MRI, JART, EDE-Q, BDI-II, STAI, age, height and weight of 12 AN patients and 15 HC subjects collected at Chiba University.

MRI acquisition and preprocessing

Brain MRI scans were obtained using 3.0 Tesla scanners at all sites (Chiba and UOEH: GE Discovery MR750; Tohoku and Kyushu: Philips Achieva; Kyoto: Siemens MAGNETOM TrioTim). The parameters including total scan time used to acquire T1-weighed images and resting-state fMRI (rsfMRI) are shown in online Supplementary Table S2. Preprocessing and quality control were performed using Statistical Parametric Mapping-12 (SPM12) and NITRC Functional Connectivity Toolbox (CONN) 18b. To stabilize the magnetic field, we excluded the first five volumes from rsfMRI data taken at repetition time (TR) = 3000 ms and the first six volumes from rsfMRI data taken at TR = 2500 ms. Then, using the default preprocessing pipeline of CONN, the functional realignment and unwarp, functional center to coordinates, functional slice-timing correction, functional outlier detection, functional direct segmentation and normalization, structural center to coordinates, structural segmentation and normalization, and functional smoothing were executed. For functional outlier detection, outlier scans were identified from the observed global BOLD signal and the amount of subject motion in the scanner. Acquisitions with a framewise displacement above 0.9 mm or global BOLD signal changes above five standard deviations were flagged as outliers.

Functional network construction

Calculations of region-to-region rsFCs were computed using CONN and the aCompCor (anatomical component-based noise correction procedure) method in MATLAB R2022a. Confounding factors (white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, six realignment parameters, first-order temporal derivatives of motion, and outlier scans) were removed using the regression of CONN's default denoising pipeline which implements aCompCor (Behzadi, Restom, Liau, & Liu, Reference Behzadi, Restom, Liau and Liu2007). Then we applied band pass filtering (0.008–0.09 Hz) to fMRI data to reduce both high- and low-frequency noise. Seed ROIs and target ROIs were defined by CONN atlas consisting of the Harvard–Oxford cortical and subcortical atlas (106 ROIs), AAL cerebellum atlas (26 ROIs), and network parcellation from ICA analysis of the HCP dataset (32 ROIs). In other words, to make the whole brain into seed ROIs and target ROIs, all regions including network parcellation were selected as ROIs rather than artificially selecting only part of the regions registered in CONN. In the ROI-to-ROI connectivity analysis, we calculated a bivariate correlation separately on the individual BOLD time series between each pair of ROIs, and the correlation coefficients were converted to Fisher's Z-scores. The output of ROI-to-ROI rsFCs for each participant was a 164 × 164 matrix of the Fisher-transformed correlation coefficient.

Statistical analysis

Demographic and clinical characteristics

Demographic variables were analyzed and compared by SPSS (Statistical Package for Social Science version 25.0. IBM Corp., Armonk, New York). Demographical variables (age, body mass index [BMI], psychological test results: JART, EDE-Q, STAI, BDI-II) that were found not to be normally distributed as a result of the Shapiro-Wilk test of normality were compared between groups by the Kruskal–Wallis test followed by multiple comparisons by the Dunn's test. Duration of illness was compared between groups by Mann–Whitney U test. The chi-square test was used to compare groups with and without psychotropic medication. Normally distributed total intracranial volume (TIV) was compared between groups by one-way ANOVA, followed by multiple comparisons by Bonferroni's multiple comparison test. All the above statistical tests were two-tailed, and a p-value <0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Combat harmonization and group comparison of functional network

To correct for the site effects of multi-site databases, we applied Combat (Combining Batches) harmonization, which adjusts for batch effects in datasets by an empirical Bayes framework to the ROI-to-ROI rsFCs of each participant using the neuroCombat Python package (Fortin et al., Reference Fortin, Parker, Tunç, Watanabe, Elliott, Ruparel and Shinohara2017). The numbers of AN patients in this study at the Hospital of UOEH and Kyushu University Hospital were considerably lower than at other sites. Moreover, the site effects of multi-site databases are mostly derived from differences in the phase encoding direction and differences in fMRI manufacturers (Yamashita et al., Reference Yamashita, Yahata, Itahashi, Lisi, Yamada, Ichikawa and Imamizu2019). Therefore, we configured the batches in Combat harmonization according to fMRI manufacturer, which governs the phase encoding direction. As shown in online Supplementary Table S2, fMRI was performed while participants kept their eyes closed at Tohoku University Hospital, unlike the other sites. However, the influence of the difference in imaging conditions due to eye-opening and closing on rsFC is considered to be limited (Patriat et al., Reference Patriat, Molloy, Meier, Kirk, Nair, Meyerand and Birn2013), so harmonization based on the difference in phase encoding direction was given priority. In this harmonization, the data acquired by GE Discovery MR750 were input as batch 1, the data obtained by a Philips Achieva were input as batch 2, and the data imaged by Siemens MAGNETOM TrioTim were input as batch 3. In addition to age and group (AN-R or AN-BP or HC), the BMI and TIV measured by Brain Anatomical Analysis using Diffeomorphic Deformation version 4.3.2 (BAAD; Shiga University of Medical Science, Otsu, Japan) were also entered as covariates. After applying Combat harmonization, the ROI-to-ROI rsFCs of each participant were re-entered into CONN. Then, we performed group-level analysis using the general linear model. Group comparisons of ROI-to-ROI rsFCs were conducted by analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with age as a covariate. The significance of group comparisons was determined by the two-sided false discovery rate corrected p-value (p-FDR) <0.01, seed-level correction, which applies FDR separately for each seed ROI.

Correlation analysis of psychological scales and rsFCs with significant differences between AN and HC

We performed correlation analysis with self-reported psychological scales (EDE-Q, BDI-II, STAI) to identify significantly different connectivities between HC and AN patients. Specifically, Spearman's rank correlation coefficient was calculated between the ROI-to-ROI rsFCs values of AN patients in these connectivities and their scores on the self-reported psychological scales using SPSS version 25.0. The significance of the correlation was determined by two-sided p-FDR <0.05.

Results

Demographics and clinical characteristics

The demographics and psychological test results of the participants are shown in Table 1. Age was slightly higher in AN-BP, and BMI was higher in HC. Duration of illness was longer in AN-BP than in AN-R. JART score was higher in HC than in AN. EDE-Q scores were higher in AN-BP, AN-R, HC, in that order for both global and subscale scores. STAI and BDI-II scores were all higher in the AN group than in HC.

Table 1. Study demographics and clinical behavioral measures

N, number of participants; s.d., standard deviation; DF, degrees of freedom; AN, anorexia nervosa; HC, healthy control; AN-R anorexia nervosa restricting type; AN-BP, anorexia nervosa binge/purge type; JART, Japanese adult reading test; EDE-Q, eating disorder examination questionnaire; STAI, state-trait anxiety inventory; BDI-II, beck depression inventory-II.

*F values, χ2 values, statistics without a symbol are H values.

Comparison of rsFCs between groups (HC v. AN)

The rsFCs that were significantly stronger or weaker in AN compared to HC are listed in Table 2, Figs 1 and 2 (p-FDR <0.01 level). AN showed 12 significantly stronger rsFCs and 33 significantly weaker rsFCs than HC. Significantly stronger rsFCs in AN were observed mainly in rostral prefrontal cortex; stronger rsFCs were observed between this region and hippocampus, amygdala, superior temporal gyrus, anterior middle temporal gyrus, and temporal pole. rsFCs were also stronger between frontal operculum cortex and temporal pole, and between frontal operculum cortex and anterior middle temporal gyrus. Significantly weaker rsFCs in AN were found mainly within temporal lobe, within cerebellum, between ACC and thalamus, temporal occipital fusiform cortex and lateral part of visual network, supramarginal gyrus and vermis, posterior parietal lobe and cerebellar hemisphere, cerebellum and frontal pole. In particular, weaker rsFCs were significant even at the p-FDR <0.001 level between left temporal pole and hippocampus, left temporal pole and anterior parahippocampal gyrus, right temporal occipital fusiform cortex and right lateral part of visual network, hippocampus and right anterior middle temporal gyrus, and vermis VI and the anterior part of cerebellar network.

Table 2. Difference of resting-state functional connectivity in AN v. HC and AN-R v. AN-BP

p-FDR, false discovery rate corrected p; HC, healthy control; AN, anorexia nervosa; AN-R AN, restricting type; AN-BP AN, binge/purge type; SN, salience network; FPN, Frontoparietal network; VN, visual network; LN, language network; CN, cerebellar network.

Figure 1. Connectome showing rsFCs altered in AN patients relative to HC. This figure presents the connectome showing rsFCs altered in AN patients relative to HC. The red lines indicate significantly stronger rsFCs and the blue lines indicate significantly weaker rsFCs in AN patients (114 persons) relative to HC (135 persons). Group comparison of ROI-to-ROI rsFCs was done by ANCOVA and using age as covariate. The significance of the group comparison was determined by two-sided p-FDR <0.01, seed-level correction, which applies FDR separately for each seed ROI. The ‘p’ before each region indicates posterior division, and ‘a’ means anterior division. The ‘r’ after each region indicates right, and the ‘l’ shows left. AG angular gyrus, SMG supramarginal gyrus, ITG inferior temporal gyrus, MTG middle temporal gyrus, STG superior temporal gyrus, TP temporal pole, FP frontal pole, CN cerebellar network, LN language network, FPN Frontoparietal network, PPC posterior parietal cortex, SN salience network, RPFC rostral prefrontal cortex, VN visual network, Ver vermis, Cereb cerebellar, AMG amygdala, HPC hippocampus, THA thalamus, FO frontal operculum cortex, TOFusC temporal occipital fusiform cortex, TFusC temporal fusiform cortex, PaHC parahippocampal gyrus, PaHC parahippocampal gyrus, AC anterior division of cingulate gyrus, Subcal subcallosal cortex.

Figure 2. Schematic diagram showing rsFCs with change in AN patients compared to HC. This figure presents a schematic diagram showing the rsFC changes in AN patients (114 persons) relative to HC (135 persons), based on a sagittal section of the brain. The left side of the figure corresponds to the frontal region, and the right side corresponds to the occipital region. The left and right sides of the brain are not clearly shown in this figure. The red lines indicate significantly stronger rsFCs, and the blue lines indicate significantly weaker rsFCs in AN relative to HC. Group comparison of ROI-to-ROI rsFCs was done by ANCOVA and using age as covariate. The significance of the group comparison was determined by two-sided p-FDR < 0.01, seed-level correction, which applies FDR separately for each seed ROI. The ‘p’ before each region indicates posterior division, and ‘a’ means anterior division. STG superior temporal gyrus, MTG middle temporal gyrus, ITG inferior temporal gyrus, SMG supramarginal gyrus, PPC posterior parietal cortex.

Comparison of rsFCs between AN subtypes(AN-R v. AN-BP)

The significantly stronger rsFCs in AN-R compared to AN-BP are listed in Table 2 and Fig. 3 (p-FDR <0.01). Stronger rsFCs were found between right lingual gyrus and right supracalcarine cortex, and between left temporal occipital fusiform cortex and medial part of visual network in AN-R compared to AN-BP.

Figure 3. Connectome showing rsFCs altered in AN-R compared to AN-BP. (a) This figure presents the connectome showing rsFCs altered in AN-R compared to AN-BP. The red line indicates significantly stronger rsFCs in AN-R (61 persons) relative to AN-BP (53 persons). A group comparison of ROI-to-ROI rsFCs was done by ANCOVA and using age as covariate. The significance of the group comparison was determined by two-sided p-FDR < 0.01, seed-level correction, which applies FDR separately for each seed ROI. The ‘r’ at the end of SCC indicates right, and the ‘l’ at the end of LG and TOFusC means left. LG lingual gyrus, TOFusC temporal occipital fusiform cortex, SCC supracalcarine cortex, VN visual network. (b) This figure shows rsFCs that were significantly stronger in AN-R relative to AN-BP in a horizontal brain section.

Correlations between rsFCs with group differences and self-reported psychological scales

Correlation coefficients between rsFCs, which showed group differences between HC and AN, and scores on self-reported psychological scales (EDE-Q, BDI-II, STAI) are shown in online Supplementary Tables S3 and S4. There was no significant correlation between rsFCs, which showed group differences between HC and AN, and the scores on each psychological scale at the level of p-FDR <0.05.

Discussion

This study showed that 45 rsFCs were significantly changed in AN compared to HC. The rsFC changes were elucidated in greater detail for regions such as ACC, thalamus, DLPFC, fusiform cortex, posterior parietal lobes, cerebellum, and visual network, where changes in rsFCs had already been noted in previous studies. Temporal pole, superior temporal gyrus, hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, amygdala, supramarginal gyrus, and frontal pole were newly identified as important regions involved in multiple rsFC changes in AN. Furthermore, this study revealed the differences in rsFCs among AN subtypes. The following paragraphs discuss the rsFCs that differed between AN and HC, followed by those that differed between AN-R and AN-BP. Finally, the implications of the lack of correlation between rsFCs showing group differences and psychological scales are discussed.

Rostral prefrontal cortex, which showed stronger rsFCs in multiple regions in AN, corresponds to DLPFC, and the stronger rsFCs of the network including DLPFC may be the neurological basis for the suppression of excessive eating behavior in AN. The MNI coordinates of rostral prefrontal cortex in CONN are X = −32, Y = 45, Z = 27 on the left side and X = 32, Y = 46, Z = 27 on the right side, corresponding to DLPFC (Lacadie, Fulbright, Constable, & Papademetris, Reference Lacadie, Fulbright, Constable and Papademetris2008). The prefrontal cortex is the main locus of biological executive control processes of eating behavior (Dohle, Diel, & Hofmann, Reference Dohle, Diel and Hofmann2018; Hall, Reference Hall2016; Hofmann, Friese, & Strack, Reference Hofmann, Friese and Strack2009). DLPFC is particularly involved in self-regulation (Hofmann, Schmeichel, & Baddeley, Reference Hofmann, Schmeichel and Baddeley2012; Miller & Cohen, Reference Miller and Cohen2001), controlling cravings and consuming food (Lowe, Vincent, & Hall, Reference Lowe, Vincent and Hall2017). In healthy adults, DLPFC activation during dietary self-regulation tasks is negatively correlated with BMI (Han, Boachie, Garcia-Garcia, Michaud, & Dagher, Reference Han, Boachie, Garcia-Garcia, Michaud and Dagher2018), and people with obesity have lower DLPFC activity in response to diet stimulation than those with leaner bodies (Gautier et al., Reference Gautier, Chen, Salbe, Bandy, Pratley, Heiman and Tataranni2000, Reference Gautier, Del Parigi, Chen, Salbe, Bandy, Pratley and Tataranni2001; Gluck, Viswanath, & Stinson, Reference Gluck, Viswanath and Stinson2017; Le et al., Reference Le, Pannacciulli, Chen, Del Parigi, Salbe, Reiman and Krakoff2006). It has also been found that high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the right DLPFC in AN patients reduces fat avoidance on a food choice task (Muratore et al., Reference Muratore, Bershad, Steinglass, Foerde, Gianini, Broft and Attia2021).

Stronger rsFCs between DLPFC and amygdala or hippocampus indicates that AN patients may cope with emotion through excessive cognitive control. DLPFC is activated during rational decision-making (Greene, Nystrom, Engell, Darley, & Cohen, Reference Greene, Nystrom, Engell, Darley and Cohen2004) and inhibits emotional expression (Lévesque et al., Reference Lévesque, Eugène, Joanette, Paquette, Mensour, Beaudoin and Beauregard2003). DLPFC is more active in AN patients when they are presented with unpleasant vocabulary (Miyake et al., Reference Miyake, Okamoto, Onoda, Shirao, Okamoto and Yamawaki2012) or food images (Sanders et al., Reference Sanders, Smeets, van Elburg, Danner, van Meer, Hoek and Adan2015). These previous studies proposed the hypothesis that AN patients cope with negative emotional reactions through excessive cognitive control by DLPFC (Sato & Fukudo, Reference Sato and Fukudo2017). The stronger rsFCs between DLPFC and the loci of emotion (i.e. amygdala or hippocampus) are consistent with this hypothesis.

Stronger rsFCs between DLPFC and superior temporal gyrus or temporal pole suggest that theory of mind (ToM) impairment, as in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), may be occurring in AN. Several studies have provided collateral evidence that ToM, the ability to reason about the mental states of others, is impaired in AN. One meta-analysis found that AN patients have lower cognitive empathy than HC (Kerr-Gaffney, Harrison, & Tchanturia, Reference Kerr-Gaffney, Harrison and Tchanturia2019). Another meta-analysis showed that AN patients have lower results in the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test than HC (Preti, Siddi, Marzola, & Abbate Daga, Reference Preti, Siddi, Marzola and Abbate Daga2022). ToM is realized by consistent activation of the posterior superior temporal gyrus, temporal pole, and medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) (Frith & Frith, Reference Frith and Frith2003). However, in ASD, where ToM is impaired, there is stronger functional connectivity between DLPFC and superior temporal gyrus, but not MPFC, and this change has been found to correlate with ASD severity (Ma et al., Reference Ma, Yuan, Li, Guo, Zhu, Wang and Wang2021).

The weaker rsFC between temporal pole and parahippocampal gyrus may contribute to the alexithymia tendency in AN, and the weaker rsFC between temporal pole and hippocampus may contribute to deficits in cognitive empathy in AN. The tendency toward alexithymia in AN has been well studied, and validated by a systematic review (Tauro, Wearne, Belevski, Filipčíková, & Francis, Reference Tauro, Wearne, Belevski, Filipčíková and Francis2022). Temporal pole is activated when evaluating one's own emotions in response to stimuli (Terasawa, Fukushima, & Umeda, Reference Terasawa, Fukushima and Umeda2011), and it was found that the alexithymia tendency is higher when temporal pole is impaired in response to unpleasant emotional experiences (Aust et al., Reference Aust, Alkan Härtwig, Koelsch, Heekeren, Heuser and Bajbouj2014). Parahippocampal gyrus has also been found to be inversely correlated with alexithymia when subjects are presented with emotional facial stimuli (Reker et al., Reference Reker, Ohrmann, Rauch, Kugel, Bauer, Dannlowski and Suslow2010). Regarding rsFC between temporal pole and hippocampus, it has been reported that the strength of rsFC between right temporal pole and left anterior hippocampus was associated with greater empathic interest in a person based on episodic information (Pehrs, Zaki, Taruffi, Kuchinke, & Koelsch, Reference Pehrs, Zaki, Taruffi, Kuchinke and Koelsch2018).

The weaker rsFC between posterior division of temporal fusiform cortex and posterior inferior temporal gyrus suggests that anomalies in visual perception of food are occurring in AN. This is because the temporal fusiform cortex and inferior temporal gyrus are both key regions in the visual processing of food (Chen, Papies, & Barsalou, Reference Chen, Papies and Barsalou2016).

The weaker rsFC between temporal occipital fusiform cortex and lateral part of a visual network may be responsible for body image distortion in AN. Temporal occipital fusiform cortex is known as a spindle gyrus body area that responds strongly to human body shape (Schwarzlose, Baker, & Kanwisher, Reference Schwarzlose, Baker and Kanwisher2005), and it is considered a region associated with body image distortion because its activity is reduced during body shape task identification in AN (Suda et al., Reference Suda, Brooks, Giampietro, Friederich, Uher, Brammer and Treasure2013). On the other hand, lateral occipitotemporal lobe, which contains a lateral part of the visual network, is also a region that selectively responds to images of the human body and is known as the extrastriate body area (Downing, Jiang, Shuman, & Kanwisher, Reference Downing, Jiang, Shuman and Kanwisher2001).

The weaker rsFC between ACC and thalamus may lead to impaired set-shifting in AN. ACC exerts cognitive control over behavior by monitoring conflicts (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, Reference Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter and Cohen2001) and errors in information processing and is activated during the performance of behavioral set-shifting tasks (Shafritz, Kartheiser, & Belger, Reference Shafritz, Kartheiser and Belger2005). Basal ganglia-thalamocortical circuits, including the thalamus, are also involved in maintaining and switching behavioral sets by their regulation of frontal lobe activity (Alexander, Crutcher, & DeLong, Reference Alexander, Crutcher, DeLong, Uylings, Van Eden, De Bruin, Corner and Feenstra1991). Furthermore, in AN, rsFCs of the network including ACC and thalamus have been found to be weaker during the performance of behavioral set-shifting tasks (Zastrow et al., Reference Zastrow, Kaiser, Stippich, Walther, Herzog, Tchanturia and Friederich2009).

Weaker rsFCs within the cerebellum and between the cerebellum and multiple regions of the brain may be associated with abnormal eating behavior in AN. The cerebellum is in direct bidirectional communication with the hypothalamus (Haines, Dietrichs, Mihailoff, & McDonald, Reference Haines, Dietrichs, Mihailoff and McDonald1997) and is responsible for motivating and regulating feeding behavior by sensing blood glucose levels, visceral stimulation, gastrointestinal hormones, taste, and smell (Zhu & Wang, Reference Zhu and Wang2008). The size of the right cerebellar hemisphere has also been found to be a prognostic factor regarding inpatient treatment of AN (Milos et al., Reference Milos, Kaufmann, Jäncke, Piccirelli, Blatow, Martin-Soelch and Baur2021).

The weaker rsFC between cerebellar Crus II and frontal pole may be involved in cognitive dysfunction. The weaker rsFC between cerebellar Crus II and posterior parietal cortex may contribute to visuospatial dysfunction, and weaker rsFCs between vermis and supramarginal gyrus or temporal pole may be involved in emotion regulation disorder in AN. Cerebellum receives higher-order information from the prefrontal cortex, posterior parietal lobe, and temporal lobe regarding motivation, emotion, etc (Schmahmann, Reference Schmahmann2010). Indeed, damage to Crus I or II of cerebellum has been found to produce cognitive dysfunction, damage to posterior cerebellar lobes including Crus II to produce visuospatial impairment, and damage to vermis to result in emotional dysregulation (Schmahmann, Reference Schmahmann2010).

For the regions with strengthened rsFCs in AN-R relative to AN-BP, the stronger rsFC between supracalcarine cortex and lingual gyrus may indicate that internally directed attention is more active in AN-R than in AN-BP. This is because the cuneus and lingual gyrus, both of which contain supracalcarine cortex, are responsible for the maintenance of internally oriented attention (Benedek et al., Reference Benedek, Jauk, Beaty, Fink, Koschutnig and Neubauer2016), are thought to be sites that are significantly activated during go/no-go tasks in AN, and are involved in the reduction of external attention (Noda et al., Reference Noda, Isobe, Ueda, Aso, Murao, Kawabata and Murai2021). The medial part of the visual network corresponds to the combined cuneus and lingual gyrus. The stronger rsFC between that region and temporal occipital fusiform cortex, known as the spindle gyrus body area may suggest that the internally oriented attention, which is heightened in AN-R, is especially directed to one's own body. In fact, AN-R patients have been found to show significantly more attention to their own unattractive body parts compared to AN-BP (Bauer et al., Reference Bauer, Schneider, Waldorf, Braks, Huber, Adolph and Vocks2017).

Finally, the lack of correlation between rsFCs, which differed between AN and HC groups, and psychological scales (EDE-Q, BDI-II, STAI) is discussed. The rsFCs, which showed significant differences between AN patients and HC in this study, include rsFCs that were suggested in previous studies to be associated with depression and anxiety. For example, rsFCs within the executive control network, with DLPFC as the main region, have been reported to be correlated with BDI-II scores in AN patients (Gaudio et al., Reference Gaudio, Piervincenzi, Beomonte Zobel, Romana Montecchi, Riva, Carducci and Cosimo Quattrocchi2015). Therefore, there is concern that rsFCs affected by depression and anxiety were included in rsFCs that showed differences between groups. However, in the present study, the lack of correlation between the rsFCs showing group differences and BDI-II or STAI suggests that there was no confounding of rsFCs involved in depression and anxiety, which are common but non-specific psychiatric symptoms in AN. Furthermore, no correlation was found with EDE-Q, suggesting that the rsFCs that showed group differences do not vary continuously with AN severity. Given the above, the rsFCs that differed significantly between AN and HC in this study are considered to be categorical diagnostic markers for AN.

A limitation of this cross-sectional study was that it could not determine whether the rsFCs that showed group differences were related to AN development or reflected temporal changes in brain function due to starvation. However, previous studies in participants recovering from AN have shown changes in the frontoparietal network (FPN), which consists of brain regions such as DLPFC and posterior parietal cortex (Boehm et al., Reference Boehm, Geisler, Tam, King, Ritschel, Seidel and Ehrlich2016), and the present study showed multiple changes in rsFCs in the FPN component regions, suggesting that at least some of the altered rsFCs in this study may be involved in the development of AN. Moreover, because weight loss is essential for diagnosing AN, the inability to distinguish whether the altered rsFCs were the causes or the effects of AN may not undermine the significance of the rsFC changes shown in this study as a categorical diagnostic marker. It should be noted, however, that atypical AN, AN without significant weight loss, exists, and psychiatric symptoms sometimes persist in AN after weight regain. Therefore, to better elucidate the brain dysfunctions involved in the development of AN, we are currently carrying out a cohort study investigating changes in the brain before and after treatment (Hamatani et al., Reference Hamatani, Hirano, Sugawara, Isobe, Kodama, Yoshihara and Sekiguchi2021). We are also collecting fMRI data on non-AN healthy, skinny women, taking advantage of the fact that as much as 17% of young Japanese women have a BMI below 18.5 (Yamamoto et al., Reference Yamamoto, Furukawa, Watanabe, Kato, Kusumoto, Takeshita and Hiasa2022). As for demographic data, there were two apparent concerns. The first was the lack of data on participants' comorbidities. Concerning depression and anxiety, the most common comorbidities in AN (Swinbourne & Touyz, Reference Swinbourne and Touyz2007; Ulfvebrand, Birgegård, Norring, Högdahl, & von Hausswolff-Juhlin, Reference Ulfvebrand, Birgegård, Norring, Högdahl and von Hausswolff-Juhlin2015), particularly the distribution among participants and the impact of comorbidities on study results should have been carefully considered. Although participants were carefully scrutinized in structured interviews based on the DSM-5 for eligibility and exclusion criteria, each participant's comorbidities were not recorded. However, this study found no significant correlation between the changes in rsFCs observed in the AN group and the depression and anxiety scores (i.e. BDI-II and STAI). Therefore, the observed differences in rsFCs were specific to AN, and the impact of comorbid depression and anxiety was likely to be relatively small. The second concern was that the mean EDE-Q global score of the AN-R group was lower than 2.3, which is generally considered the cut-off for eating disorders (Mond, Hay, Rodgers, Owen, & Beumont, Reference Mond, Hay, Rodgers, Owen and Beumont2004). However, because the mean BMI of the AN-R group was sufficiently low (14.3 kg/m2), it may be inferred that this did not indicate a lower severity of the AN-R group in this study but rather a lack of awareness of the disease. Other limitations of this study were as follows. Because this study included only Japanese women, it is unclear to what extent the findings can be generalized to other races, men, and gender-diverse populations. Because we could not collect self-report psychological scales from all participants, missing values may have affected the results of the correlation analysis between rsFCs and the scales. Although rsFC changes have been noted in many previous studies of the insular region, no significant rsFC changes in insula were observed in the present study. If insula had been divided into multiple regions based on functional localization as in previous studies (Ehrlich et al., Reference Ehrlich, Lord, Geisler, Borchardt, Boehm, Seidel and Walter2015; Kullmann et al., Reference Kullmann, Giel, Teufel, Thiel, Zipfel and Preissl2014; Lord et al., Reference Lord, Ehrlich, Borchardt, Geisler, Seidel, Huber and Walter2016), significant rsFC changes might have been observed in this study. In this study, we used the widely used atlas and network parcellation registered in CONN, but in the future, more detailed parcellation should be used not only for the insular cortex but also for other regions.

Conclusion

This study of setting whole brain regions as ROIs, with a sample size of more than 100 subjects per group, revealed that 12 rsFCs were stronger and 33 rsFCs were weakened in AN. Stronger rsFCs occurred mainly in the regions containing rostral prefrontal cortex (corresponding to DLPFC), and weaker rsFCs were found mainly in regions containing cerebellum, within temporal lobe, between posterior fusiform cortex and lateral part of visual network, and between ACC and thalamus. Furthermore, AN-R showed stronger rsFCs between right lingual gyrus and right supracalcarine cortex and between left temporal occipital fusiform cortex and medial part of visual network compared to AN-BP. These rsFC changes can represent categorical diagnostic markers discriminating AN from HC and AN-R from AN-BP.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724000485.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, YHi, YSu., YSa, MI and AS; data cuation, YSu, YHi, HK, TT, MK, MS, TN, KT, MI, NK and AS; formal analysis, YSu, YHi and JO; funding acquisition, YSa and AS; investigation, YHi, RK, SH, TY, HK, KM, YMa, YSa, YHa, TS, TM, MK, MSun, TN, KT, MI, NK, SK, MT, ST, MG, KY and AS; project administration, AS; resources, KM, YM, MN, NN, ES, JT, NK, SK, MT, ST, MG and KY; supervision, YHi, JO, MSug, SF and YMo; visualization, YSu and YHi; writing – original draft, YSu; writing – review and editing, YHi, RC, YSa, MI, MK, MSu, TN, KT and AS.

Funding statement

This research was supported by the Agency for Medical Research and Development (AS, grant number JP19dm0307104) and Japan Society for the Promotion of Science KAKENHI (AS, grant number JP25460884), (YSa, grant number JP17K09286) and grants from the Japanese Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare (AS, H29-nanbyo-ippan).

Competing interests

YM is employed by Lundbeck Japan, KK. All other authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Alexander, G. E., Crutcher, M. D., & DeLong, M. R. (1991). Chapter 6 basal ganglia-thalamocortical circuits: Parallel substrates for motor, oculomotor, “prefrontal” and “limbic” functions. In Uylings, H. B. M., Van Eden, C. G., De Bruin, J. P. C., Corner, M. A. & Feenstra, M. G. P. (Eds.), Progress in brain research (pp. 119146). Amsterdam: Elsevier. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)62678-3Google Scholar
Alfano, V., Mele, G., Cotugno, A., & Longarzo, M. (2020). Multimodal neuroimaging in anorexia nervosa. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 98(11), 21782207. doi: 10.1002/jnr.24674CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
Amianto, F., D'Agata, F., Lavagnino, L., Caroppo, P., Abbate-Daga, G., Righi, D., … Fassino, S. (2013). Intrinsic connectivity networks within cerebellum and beyond in eating disorders. The Cerebellum, 12(5), 623631. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0471-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arcelus, J., Mitchell, A. J., Wales, J., & Nielsen, S. (2011). Mortality rates in patients with anorexia nervosa and other eating disorders. A meta-analysis of 36 studies. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(7), 724731. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.74CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aust, S., Alkan Härtwig, E., Koelsch, S., Heekeren, H. R., Heuser, I., & Bajbouj, M. (2014). How emotional abilities modulate the influence of early life stress on hippocampal functioning. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(7), 10381045. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst078CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, A., Schneider, S., Waldorf, M., Braks, K., Huber, T. J., Adolph, D., & Vocks, S. (2017). Selective visual attention towards oneself and associated state body satisfaction: An eye-tracking study in adolescents with different types of eating disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45(8), 16471661. doi: 10.1007/s10802-017-0263-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Behzadi, Y., Restom, K., Liau, J., & Liu, T. T. (2007). A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. NeuroImage, 37(1), 90101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Beaty, R. E., Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., & Neubauer, A. C. (2016). Brain mechanisms associated with internally directed attention and self-generated thought. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 22959. doi: 10.1038/srep22959CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biezonski, D., Cha, J., Steinglass, J., & Posner, J. (2016). Evidence for thalamocortical circuit abnormalities and associated cognitive dysfunctions in underweight individuals with anorexia Nervosa. Neuropsychopharmacology, 41(6), 15601568. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.314CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boehm, I., Geisler, D., King, J. A., Ritschel, F., Seidel, M., Deza Araujo, Y., … Ehrlich, S. (2014). Increased resting state functional connectivity in the fronto-parietal and default mode network in anorexia nervosa. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 346. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00346CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boehm, I., Geisler, D., Tam, F., King, J. A., Ritschel, F., Seidel, M., … Ehrlich, S. (2016). Partially restored resting-state functional connectivity in women recovered from anorexia nervosa. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 41(6), 377385. doi: 10.1503/jpn.150259CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624652. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.3.624CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J., Papies, E. K., & Barsalou, L. W. (2016). A core eating network and its modulations underlie diverse eating phenomena. Brain and Cognition, 110, 2042. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.04.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collantoni, E., Michelon, S., Tenconi, E., Degortes, D., Titton, F., Manara, R., … Favaro, A. (2016). Functional connectivity correlates of response inhibition impairment in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 247, 916. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.11.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowdrey, F. A., Filippini, N., Park, R. J., Smith, S. M., & McCabe, C. (2014). Increased resting state functional connectivity in the default mode network in recovered anorexia nervosa: Resting state functional connectivity in the DMN in recovered AN. Human Brain Mapping, 35(2), 483491. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Desmond, J. E., & Glover, G. H. (2002). Estimating sample size in functional MRI (fMRI) neuroimaging studies: Statistical power analyses. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 118(2), 115128. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(02)00121-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dohle, S., Diel, K., & Hofmann, W. (2018). Executive functions and the self-regulation of eating behavior: A review. Appetite, 124, 49. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.05.041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Downing, P. E., Jiang, Y., Shuman, M., & Kanwisher, N. (2001). A cortical area selective for visual processing of the human body. Science (New York, N.Y.), 293(5539), 24702473. doi: 10.1126/science.1063414CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehrlich, S., Lord, A. R., Geisler, D., Borchardt, V., Boehm, I., Seidel, M., … Walter, M. (2015). Reduced functional connectivity in the thalamo-insular subnetwork in patients with acute anorexia nervosa. Human Brain Mapping, 36(5), 17721781. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22736CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Favaro, A., Santonastaso, P., Manara, R., Bosello, R., Bommarito, G., Tenconi, E., & Di Salle, F. (2012). Disruption of visuospatial and somatosensory functional connectivity in anorexia Nervosa. Biological Psychiatry, 72(10), 864870. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.04.025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finn, E. S., Shen, X., Scheinost, D., Rosenberg, M. D., Huang, J., Chun, M. M., … Constable, R. T. (2015). Functional connectome fingerprinting: Identifying individuals using patterns of brain connectivity. Nature Neuroscience, 18(11), 16641671. doi: 10.1038/nn.4135CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fortin, J.-P., Parker, D., Tunç, B., Watanabe, T., Elliott, M. A., Ruparel, K., … Shinohara, R. T. (2017). Harmonization of multi-site diffusion tensor imaging data. NeuroImage, 161, 149170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.047CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 459473. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1218CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaudio, S., Olivo, G., Beomonte Zobel, B., & Schiöth, H. B. (2018). Altered cerebellar–insular–parietal–cingular subnetwork in adolescents in the earliest stages of anorexia nervosa: A network-based statistic analysis. Translational Psychiatry, 8, 127. doi: 10.1038/s41398-018-0173-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaudio, S., Piervincenzi, C., Beomonte Zobel, B., Romana Montecchi, F., Riva, G., Carducci, F., & Cosimo Quattrocchi, C. (2015). Altered resting state functional connectivity of anterior cingulate cortex in drug naïve adolescents at the earliest stages of anorexia nervosa. Scientific Reports, 5(1), 10818. doi: 10.1038/srep10818CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaudio, S., Wiemerslage, L., Brooks, S. J., & Schiöth, H. B. (2016). A systematic review of resting-state functional-MRI studies in anorexia nervosa: Evidence for functional connectivity impairment in cognitive control and visuospatial and body-signal integration. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 71, 578589. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gautier, J. F., Chen, K., Salbe, A. D., Bandy, D., Pratley, R. E., Heiman, M., … Tataranni, P. A. (2000). Differential brain responses to satiation in obese and lean men. Diabetes, 49(5), 838846. doi: 10.2337/diabetes.49.5.838CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gautier, J. F., Del Parigi, A., Chen, K., Salbe, A. D., Bandy, D., Pratley, R. E., … Tataranni, P. A. (2001). Effect of satiation on brain activity in obese and lean women. Obesity Research, 9(11), 676684. doi: 10.1038/oby.2001.92CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geisler, D., Borchardt, V., Lord, A. R., Boehm, I., Ritschel, F., Zwipp, J., … Ehrlich, S. (2016). Abnormal functional global and local brain connectivity in female patients with anorexia nervosa. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience: JPN, 41(1), 615. doi: 10.1503/jpn.140310CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gluck, M. E., Viswanath, P., & Stinson, E. J. (2017). Obesity, appetite, and the prefrontal cortex. Current Obesity Reports, 6(4), 380388. doi: 10.1007/s13679-017-0289-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Engell, A. D., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron, 44(2), 389400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haines, D. E., Dietrichs, E., Mihailoff, G. A., & McDonald, E. F. (1997). The cerebellar-hypothalamic axis: Basic circuits and clinical observations. International Review of Neurobiology, 41, 83107. doi: 10.1016/s0074-7742(08)60348-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, P. A. (2016). Executive-control processes in high-calorie food consumption. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(2), 9198. doi: 10.1177/0963721415625049CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamatani, S., Hirano, Y., Sugawara, A., Isobe, M., Kodama, N., Yoshihara, K., … Sekiguchi, A. (2021). Eating disorder neuroimaging initiative (EDNI): A multicentre prospective cohort study protocol for elucidating the neural effects of cognitive-behavioural therapy for eating disorders. BMJ Open, 11(1), e042685. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2020-042685CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Han, J. E., Boachie, N., Garcia-Garcia, I., Michaud, A., & Dagher, A. (2018). Neural correlates of dietary self-control in healthy adults: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Physiology & Behavior, 192, 98108. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.02.037CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, C., & Barraclough, B. (1998). Excess mortality of mental disorder. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 173(1), 1153. doi: 10.1192/bjp.173.1.11CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, W., Friese, M., & Strack, F. (2009). Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 4(2), 162176. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01116.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., & Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(3), 174180. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horien, C., Shen, X., Scheinost, D., & Constable, R. T. (2019). The individual functional connectome is unique and stable over months to years. NeuroImage, 189, 676687. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaye, W. H., Wierenga, C. E., Bailer, U. F., Simmons, A. N., & Bischoff-Grethe, A. (2013). Nothing tastes as good as skinny feels: The neurobiology of anorexia Nervosa. Trends in Neurosciences, 36(2), 110120. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.01.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerr-Gaffney, J., Harrison, A., & Tchanturia, K. (2019). Cognitive and affective empathy in eating disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 102. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kullmann, S., Giel, K. E., Teufel, M., Thiel, A., Zipfel, S., & Preissl, H. (2014). Aberrant network integrity of the inferior frontal cortex in women with anorexia nervosa. NeuroImage: Clinical, 4, 615622. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.04.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lacadie, C. M., Fulbright, R. K., Constable, R. T., & Papademetris, X. (2008). More accurate talairach coordinates for Neuroimaging using nonlinear registration. NeuroImage, 42(2), 717725. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.240CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Le, D. S. N. T., Pannacciulli, N., Chen, K., Del Parigi, A., Salbe, A. D., Reiman, E. M., & Krakoff, J. (2006). Less activation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in response to a meal: A feature of obesity. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 84(4), 725731. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/84.4.725CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, S., Ran Kim, K., Ku, J., Lee, J.-H., Namkoong, K., & Jung, Y.-C. (2014). Resting-state synchrony between anterior cingulate cortex and precuneus relates to body shape concern in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 221(1), 4348. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.11.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lévesque, J., Eugène, F., Joanette, Y., Paquette, V., Mensour, B., Beaudoin, G., … Beauregard, M. (2003). Neural circuitry underlying voluntary suppression of sadness. Biological Psychiatry, 53(6), 502510. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01817-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lord, A., Ehrlich, S., Borchardt, V., Geisler, D., Seidel, M., Huber, S., … Walter, M. (2016). Brain parcellation choice affects disease-related topology differences increasingly from global to local network levels. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 249, 1219. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.02.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lowe, C. J., Vincent, C., & Hall, P. A. (2017). Effects of noninvasive brain stimulation on food cravings and consumption: A meta-analytic review. Psychosomatic Medicine, 79(1), 213. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000368CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ma, L., Yuan, T., Li, W., Guo, L., Zhu, D., Wang, Z., … Wang, J. (2021). Dynamic functional connectivity alterations and their associated gene expression pattern in autism spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 15, 794151. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.794151CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marek, S., Tervo-Clemmens, B., Calabro, F. J., Montez, D. F., Kay, B. P., Hatoum, A. S., … Dosenbach, N. U. F. (2022). Reproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals. Nature, 603(7902), 654660. doi: 10.1038/s41586-022-04492-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24(1), 167202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milos, G., Kaufmann, L.-K., Jäncke, L., Piccirelli, M., Blatow, M., Martin-Soelch, C., … Baur, V. (2021). Does local cerebellar volume predict treatment success in anorexia nervosa? Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 317, 111355. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2021.111355CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, Y., Okamoto, Y., Onoda, K., Shirao, N., Okamoto, Y., & Yamawaki, S. (2012). Brain activation during the perception of stressful word stimuli concerning interpersonal relationships in anorexia nervosa patients with high degrees of alexithymia in an fMRI paradigm. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 201(2), 113119. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.07.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mond, J. M., Hay, P. J., Rodgers, B., Owen, C., & Beumont, P. J. V. (2004). Validity of the eating disorder examination questionnaire (EDE-Q) in screening for eating disorders in community samples. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(5), 551567. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00161-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muratore, A. F., Bershad, M., Steinglass, J. E., Foerde, K. E., Gianini, L., Broft, A., & Attia, E. (2021). Use of high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to probe the neural circuitry of food choice in anorexia nervosa: A proof-of-concept study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 54(11), 20312036. doi: 10.1002/eat.23597CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noda, T., Isobe, M., Ueda, K., Aso, T., Murao, E., Kawabata, M., … Murai, T. (2021). The relationship between attention and avoidance coping in anorexia nervosa: Functional magnetic resonance imaging study. BJPsych Open, 7(4), e130. doi: 10.1192/bjo.2021.963CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olivo, G., Swenne, I., Zhukovsky, C., Tuunainen, A.-K., Salonen-Ros, H., Larsson, E.-M., … Schiöth, H. B. (2018). Reduced resting-state connectivity in areas involved in processing of face-related social cues in female adolescents with atypical anorexia nervosa. Translational Psychiatry, 8, 275. doi: 10.1038/s41398-018-0333-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patriat, R., Molloy, E. K., Meier, T. B., Kirk, G. R., Nair, V. A., Meyerand, M. E., … Birn, R. M. (2013). The effect of resting condition on resting-state fMRI reliability and consistency: A comparison between resting with eyes open, closed, and fixated. NeuroImage, 78, 463473. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pehrs, C., Zaki, J., Taruffi, L., Kuchinke, L., & Koelsch, S. (2018). Hippocampal-temporopolar connectivity contributes to episodic simulation during social cognition. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 9409. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-24557-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillipou, A., Abel, L. A., Castle, D. J., Hughes, M. E., Nibbs, R. G., Gurvich, C., & Rossell, S. L. (2016). Resting state functional connectivity in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 251, 4552. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.04.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preti, A., Siddi, S., Marzola, E., & Abbate Daga, G. (2022). Affective cognition in eating disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis of the performance on the “reading the mind in the eyes” test. Eating and Weight Disorders, 27(7), 22912307. doi: 10.1007/s40519-022-01393-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reker, M., Ohrmann, P., Rauch, A. V., Kugel, H., Bauer, J., Dannlowski, U., … Suslow, T. (2010). Individual differences in alexithymia and brain response to masked emotion faces. Cortex, 46(5), 658667. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.05.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanders, N., Smeets, P. A. M., van Elburg, A. A., Danner, U. N., van Meer, F., Hoek, H. W., & Adan, R. A. H. (2015). Altered food-cue processing in chronically ill and recovered women with anorexia nervosa. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 46. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00046CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sato, Y., & Fukudo, S. (2017). Eating disorders as stress related disorders: An approach with neuroimaging studies. Japanese Journal of Psychosomatic Medicine, 57, 790796. doi: 10.15064/jjpm.57.8_790Google Scholar
Scaife, J. C., Godier, L. R., Filippini, N., Harmer, C. J., & Park, R. J. (2017). Reduced resting-state functional connectivity in current and recovered restrictive anorexia Nervosa. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8, 30. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmahmann, J. D. (2010). The role of the cerebellum in cognition and emotion: Personal reflections since 1982 on the dysmetria of thought hypothesis, and Its historical evolution from theory to therapy. Neuropsychology Review, 20(3), 236260. doi: 10.1007/s11065-010-9142-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwarzlose, R. F., Baker, C. I., & Kanwisher, N. (2005). Separate face and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(47), 1105511059. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2621-05.2005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shafritz, K. M., Kartheiser, P., & Belger, A. (2005). Dissociation of neural systems mediating shifts in behavioral response and cognitive set. NeuroImage, 25(2), 600606. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.054CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinhausen, H.-C. (2009). Outcome of eating disorders. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 18(1), 225242. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2008.07.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Su, T., Gong, J., Tang, G., Qiu, S., Chen, P., Chen, G., … Wang, Y. (2021). Structural and functional brain alterations in anorexia nervosa: A multimodal meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 42(15), 51545169. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25602CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suda, M., Brooks, S. J., Giampietro, V., Friederich, H.-C., Uher, R., Brammer, M. J., … Treasure, J. (2013). Functional neuroanatomy of body checking in people with anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 46(7), 653662. doi: 10.1002/eat.22150CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swinbourne, J. M., & Touyz, S. W. (2007). The co-morbidity of eating disorders and anxiety disorders: A review. European Eating Disorders Review, 15(4), 253274. doi: 10.1002/erv.784CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tauro, J. L., Wearne, T. A., Belevski, B., Filipčíková, M., & Francis, H. M. (2022). Social cognition in female adults with anorexia nervosa: A systematic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 132, 197210. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.11.035CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terasawa, Y., Fukushima, H., & Umeda, S. (2011). How does interoceptive awareness interact with the subjective experience of emotion? An fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 34(3), 598612. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21458CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ulfvebrand, S., Birgegård, A., Norring, C., Högdahl, L., & von Hausswolff-Juhlin, Y. (2015). Psychiatric comorbidity in women and men with eating disorders results from a large clinical database. Psychiatry Research, 230(2), 294299. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.09.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamamoto, Y., Furukawa, S., Watanabe, J., Kato, A., Kusumoto, K., Takeshita, E., … Hiasa, Y. (2022). Association between body mass index and functional dyspepsia in young Japanese people. Journal of Neurogastroenterology and Motility, 28(2), 276282. doi: 10.5056/jnm21076CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamashita, A., Yahata, N., Itahashi, T., Lisi, G., Yamada, T., Ichikawa, N., … Imamizu, H. (2019). Harmonization of resting-state functional MRI data across multiple imaging sites via the separation of site differences into sampling bias and measurement bias. PLOS Biology, 17(4), e3000042. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000042CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zastrow, A., Kaiser, S., Stippich, C., Walther, S., Herzog, W., Tchanturia, K., … Friederich, H.-C. (2009). Neural correlates of impaired cognitive-behavioral flexibility in anorexia nervosa. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(5), 608616. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050775CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhu, J.-N., & Wang, J.-J. (2008). The cerebellum in feeding control: Possible function and mechanism. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology, 28(4), 469478. doi: 10.1007/s10571-007-9236-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zipfel, S., Giel, K. E., Bulik, C. M., Hay, P., & Schmidt, U. (2015). Anorexia nervosa: Aetiology, assessment, and treatment. The Lancet Psychiatry, 2(12), 10991111. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00356-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Study demographics and clinical behavioral measures

Figure 1

Table 2. Difference of resting-state functional connectivity in AN v. HC and AN-R v. AN-BP

Figure 2

Figure 1. Connectome showing rsFCs altered in AN patients relative to HC. This figure presents the connectome showing rsFCs altered in AN patients relative to HC. The red lines indicate significantly stronger rsFCs and the blue lines indicate significantly weaker rsFCs in AN patients (114 persons) relative to HC (135 persons). Group comparison of ROI-to-ROI rsFCs was done by ANCOVA and using age as covariate. The significance of the group comparison was determined by two-sided p-FDR <0.01, seed-level correction, which applies FDR separately for each seed ROI. The ‘p’ before each region indicates posterior division, and ‘a’ means anterior division. The ‘r’ after each region indicates right, and the ‘l’ shows left. AG angular gyrus, SMG supramarginal gyrus, ITG inferior temporal gyrus, MTG middle temporal gyrus, STG superior temporal gyrus, TP temporal pole, FP frontal pole, CN cerebellar network, LN language network, FPN Frontoparietal network, PPC posterior parietal cortex, SN salience network, RPFC rostral prefrontal cortex, VN visual network, Ver vermis, Cereb cerebellar, AMG amygdala, HPC hippocampus, THA thalamus, FO frontal operculum cortex, TOFusC temporal occipital fusiform cortex, TFusC temporal fusiform cortex, PaHC parahippocampal gyrus, PaHC parahippocampal gyrus, AC anterior division of cingulate gyrus, Subcal subcallosal cortex.

Figure 3

Figure 2. Schematic diagram showing rsFCs with change in AN patients compared to HC. This figure presents a schematic diagram showing the rsFC changes in AN patients (114 persons) relative to HC (135 persons), based on a sagittal section of the brain. The left side of the figure corresponds to the frontal region, and the right side corresponds to the occipital region. The left and right sides of the brain are not clearly shown in this figure. The red lines indicate significantly stronger rsFCs, and the blue lines indicate significantly weaker rsFCs in AN relative to HC. Group comparison of ROI-to-ROI rsFCs was done by ANCOVA and using age as covariate. The significance of the group comparison was determined by two-sided p-FDR < 0.01, seed-level correction, which applies FDR separately for each seed ROI. The ‘p’ before each region indicates posterior division, and ‘a’ means anterior division. STG superior temporal gyrus, MTG middle temporal gyrus, ITG inferior temporal gyrus, SMG supramarginal gyrus, PPC posterior parietal cortex.

Figure 4

Figure 3. Connectome showing rsFCs altered in AN-R compared to AN-BP. (a) This figure presents the connectome showing rsFCs altered in AN-R compared to AN-BP. The red line indicates significantly stronger rsFCs in AN-R (61 persons) relative to AN-BP (53 persons). A group comparison of ROI-to-ROI rsFCs was done by ANCOVA and using age as covariate. The significance of the group comparison was determined by two-sided p-FDR < 0.01, seed-level correction, which applies FDR separately for each seed ROI. The ‘r’ at the end of SCC indicates right, and the ‘l’ at the end of LG and TOFusC means left. LG lingual gyrus, TOFusC temporal occipital fusiform cortex, SCC supracalcarine cortex, VN visual network. (b) This figure shows rsFCs that were significantly stronger in AN-R relative to AN-BP in a horizontal brain section.

Supplementary material: File

Sudo et al. supplementary material

Sudo et al. supplementary material
Download Sudo et al. supplementary material(File)
File 58.5 KB