Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-21T19:48:42.883Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive performance and brain structural connectome alterations in major depressive disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2023

Marius Gruber
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Frankfurt, Goethe University, 60528 Frankfurt, Germany
Marco Mauritz
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany
Susanne Meinert
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany Institute of Translational Neuroscience, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany
Dominik Grotegerd
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany
Siemon C. de Lange
Affiliation:
Connectome Lab, Department of Complex Trait Genetics, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam Neuroscience, 1081 HV Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Sleep and Cognition, Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, an institute of the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, 1105 BA Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Pascal Grumbach
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany
Janik Goltermann
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany
Nils Ralf Winter
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany
Lena Waltemate
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany
Hannah Lemke
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany
Katharina Thiel
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany
Alexandra Winter
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany
Fabian Breuer
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany
Tiana Borgers
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany
Verena Enneking
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany
Melissa Klug
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany
Katharina Brosch
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Marburg, 35039 Marburg, Germany Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior, University of Marburg, 35032 Marburg, Germany
Tina Meller
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Marburg, 35039 Marburg, Germany Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior, University of Marburg, 35032 Marburg, Germany
Julia-Katharina Pfarr
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Marburg, 35039 Marburg, Germany Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior, University of Marburg, 35032 Marburg, Germany
Kai Gustav Ringwald
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Marburg, 35039 Marburg, Germany Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior, University of Marburg, 35032 Marburg, Germany
Frederike Stein
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Marburg, 35039 Marburg, Germany Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior, University of Marburg, 35032 Marburg, Germany
Nils Opel
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany Department of Psychiatry, Jena University Hospital/Friedrich-Schiller-University Jena, 07743 Jena, Germany
Ronny Redlich
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany Institute of Psychology, University of Halle, 06108 Halle (Saale), Germany
Tim Hahn
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany
Elisabeth J. Leehr
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany
Jochen Bauer
Affiliation:
Department of Radiology, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany
Igor Nenadić
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Marburg, 35039 Marburg, Germany Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior, University of Marburg, 35032 Marburg, Germany
Tilo Kircher
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Marburg, 35039 Marburg, Germany Center for Mind, Brain and Behavior, University of Marburg, 35032 Marburg, Germany
Martijn P. van den Heuvel
Affiliation:
Connectome Lab, Department of Complex Trait Genetics, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam Neuroscience, 1081 HV Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Child Psychiatry, Amsterdam University Medical Center, Amsterdam Neuroscience, 1105 AZ Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Udo Dannlowski
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany
Jonathan Repple*
Affiliation:
Institute for Translational Psychiatry, University of Münster, 48149 Münster, Germany Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Frankfurt, Goethe University, 60528 Frankfurt, Germany
*
Author for correspondence: Jonathan Repple, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Cognitive dysfunction and brain structural connectivity alterations have been observed in major depressive disorder (MDD). However, little is known about their interrelation. The present study follows a network approach to evaluate alterations in cognition-related brain structural networks.

Methods

Cognitive performance of n = 805 healthy and n = 679 acutely depressed or remitted individuals was assessed using 14 cognitive tests aggregated into cognitive factors. The structural connectome was reconstructed from structural and diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging. Associations between global connectivity strength and cognitive factors were established using linear regressions. Network-based statistics were applied to identify subnetworks of connections underlying these global-level associations. In exploratory analyses, effects of depression were assessed by evaluating remission status-related group differences in subnetwork-specific connectivity. Partial correlations were employed to directly test the complete triad of cognitive factors, depressive symptom severity, and subnetwork-specific connectivity strength.

Results

All cognitive factors were associated with global connectivity strength. For each cognitive factor, network-based statistics identified a subnetwork of connections, revealing, for example, a subnetwork positively associated with processing speed. Within that subnetwork, acutely depressed patients showed significantly reduced connectivity strength compared to healthy controls. Moreover, connectivity strength in that subnetwork was associated to current depressive symptom severity independent of the previous disease course.

Conclusions

Our study is the first to identify cognition-related structural brain networks in MDD patients, thereby revealing associations between cognitive deficits, depressive symptoms, and reduced structural connectivity. This supports the hypothesis that structural connectome alterations may mediate the association of cognitive deficits and depression severity.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided that no alterations are made and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use and/or adaptation of the article.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is an affective disorder that has been considered one of the most debilitating diseases for the past three decades (James et al., Reference James, Abate, Abate, Abay, Abbafati, Abbasi and Murray2018). Cognitive deficits, such as impaired concentration, attention, or decision-making contribute to this debilitation and are prevalent among MDD patients: In studies, up to 90% of patients report cognitive deficits (Conradi, Ormel, & De Jonge, Reference Conradi, Ormel and De Jonge2011), and up to 48% of patients are diagnosed with cognitive deficits (Gualtieri & Morgan, Reference Gualtieri and Morgan2008; Schwert, Stohrer, Aschenbrenner, Weisbrod, & Schröder, Reference Schwert, Stohrer, Aschenbrenner, Weisbrod and Schröder2019). In acute depression, patients exhibit impairments in the cognitive domains of executive functioning, processing speed, memory, and attention (Baune, Fuhr, Air, & Hering, Reference Baune, Fuhr, Air and Hering2014; Rock, Roiser, Riedel, & Blackwell, Reference Rock, Roiser, Riedel and Blackwell2014), which negatively affect daily psychosocial functioning (Buist-Bouwman et al., Reference Buist-Bouwman, Ormel, De Graaf, De Jonge, Van Sonderen, Vollebergh and Vilagut2008; Fried & Nesse, Reference Fried and Nesse2014; McIntyre et al., Reference McIntyre, Cha, Soczynska, Woldeyohannes, Gallaugher, Kudlow and Baskaran2013) and treatment outcome (McLennan & Mathias, Reference McLennan and Mathias2010). Even after remission, moderate impairments in the domains of executive functioning and attention persist (Baune et al., Reference Baune, Fuhr, Air and Hering2014; Rock et al., Reference Rock, Roiser, Riedel and Blackwell2014), causing impairments, for example, in occupational contexts (Baune et al., Reference Baune, Miller, McAfoose, Johnson, Quirk and Mitchell2010; Jaeger, Berns, Uzelac, & Davis-Conway, Reference Jaeger, Berns, Uzelac and Davis-Conway2006). Due to these adverse effects, cognitive deficits represent a symptom of outstanding importance within the symptom profile of MDD. It is, thus, crucial to investigate the factors underlying their emergence. Aiming to identify one of these factors, in the present study, we investigated the relationship between neurobiological alterations and cognitive deficits in MDD. More specifically, we explored the association of alterations in structural connectivity, i.e. white matter fiber tracts, and cognitive performance in a large sample of MDD patients and healthy controls (HC).

Structural connectivity within the human brain can be investigated using neuroimaging techniques such as diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI). DWI captures the diffusion of hydrogen molecules (Basser, Mattiello, & LeBihan, Reference Basser, Mattiello and LeBihan1994), allowing researchers to reconstruct white matter fiber tracts. The entirety of these fiber tracts, i.e. the brain's connectome, can then be analyzed using network analyses (van den Heuvel & Sporns, Reference van den Heuvel and Sporns2013). In these analyses, white matter fiber tracts are considered edges that connect nodes, i.e. gray matter brain regions. Moreover, researchers can infer the connectome's topology using graph metrics or identify subnetworks of edges associated with a given effect.

Previous studies that applied network analyses on the structural connectome identified subnetworks of edges characterized by reduced structural connectivity in MDD patients (Korgaonkar et al., Reference Korgaonkar, Grieve, Koslow, Gabrieli, Gordon and Williams2011; Myung et al., Reference Myung, Han, Fava, Mischoulon, Papakostas, Heo and Jeon2016; Repple et al., Reference Repple, Mauritz, Meinert, de Lange, Grotegerd, Opel and van den Heuvel2020; Sacchet, Prasad, Foland-Ross, Thompson, & Gotlib, Reference Sacchet, Prasad, Foland-Ross, Thompson and Gotlib2015). These findings are supported by studies showing widespread microstructural white matter alterations in MDD patients (Han et al., Reference Han, Choi, Jung, Na, Yoon, Lee and Ham2014; Jiang et al., Reference Jiang, Zhao, Hu, Du, Chen, Wu and Gong2017; Murphy & Frodl, Reference Murphy and Frodl2011; van Velzen et al., Reference van Velzen, Kelly, Isaev, Aleman, Aftanas, Bauer and Schmaal2020). Results from both network and microstructural analyses suggest that structural connectivity alterations are especially pronounced in acute compared to remitted depression (Repple et al., Reference Repple, Mauritz, Meinert, de Lange, Grotegerd, Opel and van den Heuvel2020, Reference Repple, Zaremba, Meinert, Grotegerd, Redlich, Förster and Dannlowski2019).

Given all these findings, we hypothesize a link between cognitive deficits and structural connectome alterations in MDD. This hypothesis is based on the theory that cognitive functions emerge from a network of interacting brain regions rather than from individual brain regions alone (Lim & Helpern, Reference Lim and Helpern2002). Adopting this theory, cognitive deficits would likely be associated with alterations within cognition-related networks rather than alterations in individual brain structures. Empirically, our hypothesis is supported by studies showing associations between structural connectivity and cognitive performance in HC (Dhamala, Jamison, Jaywant, Dennis, & Kuceyeski, Reference Dhamala, Jamison, Jaywant, Dennis and Kuceyeski2021; Wiseman et al., Reference Wiseman, Booth, Ritchie, Cox, Muñoz Maniega, Valdés Hernández and Bastin2018; Zimmermann, Griffiths, & McIntosh, Reference Zimmermann, Griffiths and McIntosh2018) and patients suffering, for example, from multiple sclerosis, HIV, or traumatic brain injury (Jolly, Scott, Sharp, & Hampshire, Reference Jolly, Scott, Sharp and Hampshire2020; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Hassanzadeh-Behbahani, Bronshteyn, Dawson, Kumar, Moore and Jiang2021; Zhang, Cortese, De Stefano, & Giorgio, Reference Zhang, Cortese, De Stefano and Giorgio2021). Beyond results from network analyses, our hypothesis is supported by results from analyses of white matter microstructure in HC (Bolandzadeh, Davis, Tam, Handy, & Liu-Ambrose, Reference Bolandzadeh, Davis, Tam, Handy and Liu-Ambrose2012; Grumbach et al., Reference Grumbach, Opel, Martin, Meinert, Leehr, Redlich and Repple2020) and neurological or psychiatric patients (Eijlers et al., Reference Eijlers, Van Geest, Dekker, Steenwijk, Meijer, Hulst and Geurts2018; Karlsgodt, Reference Karlsgodt2016; Mettenburg, Benzinger, Shimony, Snyder, & Sheline, Reference Mettenburg, Benzinger, Shimony, Snyder and Sheline2012; Mollison et al., Reference Mollison, Sellar, Bastin, Mollison, Chandran, Wardlaw and Connick2017; Rizk et al., Reference Rizk, Rubin-Falcone, Keilp, Miller, Sublette, Burke and Mann2017; Welton, Kent, Constantinescu, Auer, & Dineen, Reference Welton, Kent, Constantinescu, Auer and Dineen2015; Yamada et al., Reference Yamada, Takahashi, Ukai, Tsuji, Iwatani, Tsuda and Shinosaki2015). However, to our knowledge, no study utilized network analyses to investigate the association between cognitive performance and the structural connectome in MDD. Following our above hypothesis, network analyses could provide valuable insights into this relationship by identifying both the networks associated with cognitive performance and MDD-related alterations within these networks. In the present study, we, thus, compared the cognitive performance of MDD patients and HC and employed network analysis to investigate associations between cognitive performance and the structural connectome. We expected (1) significantly reduced cognitive performance in MDD patients compared to HC, especially in the domains of processing speed and executive functioning, which is particularly pronounced in acutely depressed patients; and (2) significant associations between brain structural networks and cognitive performance across the whole sample. In exploratory analyses, we evaluated the effects of acute depressive symptoms within these cognition-related subnetworks.

Materials and methods

Participants and behavioral measures

A total of N = 1484 participants were included in our analysis. Details on participants' demographic and clinical characteristics can be found in Table 1. The sample was part of the Marburg–Münster Affective Disorders Cohort Study [see Kircher et al. (Reference Kircher, Wöhr, Nenadic, Schwarting, Schratt, Alferink and Dannlowski2019) for a general study protocol and Vogelbacher et al. (Reference Vogelbacher, Möbius, Sommer, Schuster, Dannlowski, Kircher and Bopp2018) for an MRI quality assurance protocol]. Study procedures were approved by the ethics committees of the medical faculties of the universities of Marburg and Münster. Participants aged 18–65 were recruited in one of the two cities (Marburg or Münster, Germany) via newspaper advertisements or local psychiatric hospitals. All participants gave written informed consent before participation. The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR [Axis I: Mental Disorders, SCID-I (Wittchen, Wunderlich, Gruschwitz, & Zaudig, Reference Wittchen, Wunderlich, Gruschwitz and Zaudig1997)] was used by trained personnel to diagnose psychiatric disorders. Patients were included in our analysis if they were diagnosed with a current depressive episode (MDDa) or a history of depressive episodes in partial or complete remission (MDDr) according to SCID-I. HC were included if they had no current or history of psychiatric or neurological diseases. See our previous work (Repple et al., Reference Repple, Mauritz, Meinert, de Lange, Grotegerd, Opel and van den Heuvel2020) for details on exclusion criteria and online Supplement S1 for information regarding patients' medication and comorbidities.

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample

Note. Except for sex, mean values and standard deviations are shown. Test statistics and p values were derived from ANCOVA or χ2 tests. HAMD, Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (total score); HC, healthy controls; MDDa, patients with an acute episode of major depressive disorder; MDDr, MDD patients in symptomatic remission.

1 Letters indicate significant (i.e. p FDR < 0.05) differences in post-hoc t tests between HC and MDDa (A), HC and MDDr (B), or MDDa and MDDr (C).

Scores from 14 cognitive tests (see Fig. 1a) were included to assess the participants' cognitive performance. Please refer to online Supplement S2 for details on the cognitive tests. Current depressive symptom severity was assessed in all participants using the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (Hamilton, Reference Hamilton, Sartorius and Ban1986).

Fig. 1. Results from analyses on cognitive performance. (a) Factor loadings of cognitive test scores resulting from exploratory factor analysis. According to this analysis, three factors represent the variance within the cognitive performance of the sample: CF-PS, cognitive factor representing processing speed; CF-VLM, cognitive factor representing verbal learning and memory; CF-VF, cognitive factor representing verbal fluency; VLMT 1–5, verbal learning; VLMT 6, immediate verbal memory; VLMT 7, delayed verbal memory; VLMT 8, recognition; DSST, Digit Symbol Substitution Test; TMT, Trail Making Test; D2, D2 Test of attention; LNST, Letter Number Sequencing Test; Corsi fwd., Crosi Block-Tapping Test forward; Crosi bw., Corsi Block-Tapping Test backwards; VF category, semantic verbal fluency; VF letter, phenomic verbal fluency; VF altern., cognitive flexibility. (b) Differences in cognitive factors between healthy controls (HC) and patients with acute (MDDa) or (partially) remitted (MDDr) episode of major depressive disorder. *p FDR ≤ 0.05; **p FDR ≤  0.01; ***p FDR ≤  0.001. Boxes indicate the lower and upper quartile of the CF. Black lines within the boxes indicate the mean of the CF. Whiskers indicate the 1.5 inter-quartile range of the CF's lower and upper quartile. Diamonds indicate observations that fall outside this range. (c) Effect sizes (Cohen's d) representing these between-group differences. See online version for colored figures.

Acquisition and processing of MRI data

MRI data acquisition

Two MR scanners at the Universities of Münster and Marburg were used for MRI data acquisition (see online Supplement S3 and S4 for details on MRI data acquisition and preprocessing, respectively). We included a scanner-site variable in all connectome analyses to account for scanner differences (Vogelbacher et al., Reference Vogelbacher, Möbius, Sommer, Schuster, Dannlowski, Kircher and Bopp2018).

Anatomical connectome reconstruction

Details on the reconstruction and quality control of the anatomical connectome are provided in online Supplements S5 and S6, respectively. Briefly, white matter connectivity strength between 114 cortical brain regions [depicted by the Cammoun subdivision of the Desikan-Killiany atlas (Cammoun et al., Reference Cammoun, Gigandet, Meskaldji, Thiran, Sporns, Do and Hagmann2012; Desikan et al., Reference Desikan, Ségonne, Fischl, Quinn, Dickerson, Blacker and Killiany2006)] was reconstructed using CATO (de Lange & van den Heuvel, Reference de Lange and van den Heuvel2021). Each participant's network was stored in a connectivity matrix with rows and columns representing nodes, i.e. brain regions, and matrix entries representing edges, i.e. white matter fiber tracts. Edge connectivity strength was measured as the number of reconstructed streamlines between two nodes. Edges were included if they incorporated at least three reconstructed streamlines to balance the sensitivity and specificity of the resulting connectivity matrices (de Reus & van den Heuvel, Reference de Reus and van den Heuvel2013; Zalesky et al., Reference Zalesky, Fornito, Cocchi, Gollo, van den Heuvel and Breakspear2016).

Estimation of global connectivity strength and connectome topology

We calculated three measures to assess the connectome's global connectivity strength: (1) the number of edges present based on the binarized connectivity matrix; (2) the mean connectivity strength within the connectome as the mean number of streamlines per edge present (i.e. based on the weighted connectivity matrix); (3) the total number of streamlines within a connectome. In addition, we included seven standard measures describing the topology of the unweighted connectome in our analysis, such as shortest path length, global efficiency, clustering, or small-worldness (Rubinov & Sporns, Reference Rubinov and Sporns2010). For details on these measures, we refer to online Supplement S7.

Statistical analysis

Python 3.7.9 (Van Rossum & Drake, Reference Van Rossum and Drake2019) was used to analyze the data and create the figures (see online Supplement S8 for all Python packages employed). Additionally, Matlab 2019b (MATLAB, 2019) was used to apply network-based statistics (NBS) (Zalesky, Fornito, & Bullmore, Reference Zalesky, Fornito and Bullmore2010). Network figures were created using Brain Data Viewer (Dwyer et al., Reference Dwyer, Fornito, Pham, Shi, Smith, Manley and Klapperstueck2017). If not stated otherwise, statistical tests were conducted at a two-sided significance level of α = 0.05.

Analyses of cognitive performance

We calculated cognitive factors by applying an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) on the test scores to abstract from cognitive tests to cognitive domains. Details on the EFA can be found in online Supplement S9. In short, the adequacy of the data for conducting an EFA was evaluated using Bartlett's test and the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin test. Then, the test scores of all participants were entered into the EFA, and the number of factors to be extracted was determined according on their eigenvalues (Kaiser criterion, Scree plot) and a parallel analysis (Horn, Reference Horn1965). The cognitive factors were interpreted according to the factor loadings of the test scores. All subsequent analyses were conducted using these cognitive factors.

Analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) were employed to test our first hypothesis that there are cognitive deficits in MDD patients, entering the respective cognitive factor as the dependent variable and remission status (HC v. MDDr v. MDDa) as independent variable. Further, age, sex, and years of education were included as covariates of no interest as those are key variables for evaluating an individual's cognitive performance. In case of a significant remission status effect, FDR (Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995) corrected post-hoc t tests were applied to assess which of the groups drove the effect.

Analyses of associations between cognitive performance and the structural connectome

To test our second hypothesis that cognitive performance is associated with structural brain networks across the whole sample, we first conducted global-level analyses. To this end, we employed linear regressions testing the bivariate association between a global connectome measure and a given cognitive factor while correcting for age, sex, and scanner-site. The p values assigned to the cognitive factors were extracted and corrected for multiple comparisons based on the FDR. To evaluate whether HC, acutely depressed, and remitted MDD patients differed in these associations, in a second step, we additionally entered the remission status and the remission status × cognitive factor interaction effects into the above regressions. The respective p values were again corrected based on the FDR.

Next, to identify the subnetworks of edges underlying the cognition–connectome associations found at the global level, we employed NBS analyses in the whole sample (see online Supplement S10 for details). As for our global-level analyses, we chose a two-step approach for these local-level analyses: First, for each cognitive factor, an NBS analysis was conducted, which used family-wise error (FWE) corrected linear models to test the association between edge-wise connectivity strength (i.e. number of streamlines) and the respective cognitive factor while correcting for age, sex, and scanner-site. The significance of an identified subnetwork was assessed through permutation testing. To correct for multiple testing, we set the significance level to α = 0.05/number of NBS analyses = 0.05/3 cognitive factors = 0.0167 in these analyses. Second, to evaluate whether remission status groups differed in these local-level associations, for each cognitive factor, an additional NBS analysis was conducted, testing the interaction effect of remission status and cognitive performance while correcting for the main effects of remission status, age, sex, and scanner-site. To correct for multiple testing, the significance level was again set to α = 0.0167. Note that all subnetworks were obtained when applying an NBS F-threshold of F = 5.8. We chose this threshold because it is the highest at which subnetworks could be identified for all cognitive factors, allowing identification and comparison of the most specific subnetworks. See online Supplement S11 for analyses based on a more lenient threshold and online Supplement S12 for information on how different F-thresholds result in differing subnetwork sizes.

Analyses of effects of depression on identified subnetworks

Our analyses followed the hypothesis that comparable subnetworks are associated with cognitive performance in healthy and depressed individuals but that depressed individuals show alterations within these subnetworks. After identifying these subnetworks in our second analysis, in our third analysis, we assessed the effects of depression within the identified subnetworks by employing two complementary analyses that were based on the subnetwork-specific connectivity strength, i.e. the total number of streamlines in an identified subnetwork.

First, to evaluate whether depressed individuals show altered connectivity patterns within the identified subnetworks compared to HC, we entered the subnetwork-specific connectivity strength as the dependent variable into an ANCOVA with remission status (HC v. MDDr v. MDDa), age, sex, and scanner-site as independent variables. In case of a significant remission status effect, we applied post-hoc t tests to assess which of the groups drove the effect.

Second, we performed a correlation analysis to simultaneously test all associations forming the triad of acute depressive symptoms, cognition-related networks, and cognitive performance and to examine the influence of disease course characteristics on this triad. More specifically, we calculated correlations between (1) depressive symptom severity as measured by the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D), (2) subnetwork-specific connectivity strength, and (3) cognitive performance as measured by the cognitive factor used to identify the respective subnetwork. Correlations were calculated as partial correlations between two variables while holding the third constant and correcting for age, sex, and scanner-site. All p values were corrected for multiple comparisons based on the FDR. Next, to assess the influence of disease course characteristics on the triad, we repeated the correlational analyses while adjusting for age of disease onset, the number of depressive episodes or hospitalizations, or medication load (Opel et al., Reference Opel, Redlich, Dohm, Zaremba, Goltermann, Repple and Dannlowski2019; Redlich et al., Reference Redlich, Almeida, Grotegerd, Opel, Kugel, Heindel and Dannlowski2014; Repple et al., Reference Repple, Mauritz, Meinert, de Lange, Grotegerd, Opel and van den Heuvel2020, Reference Repple, Meinert, Grotegerd, Kugel, Redlich, Dohm and Dannlowski2017). Note that we focused this analysis on the MDD subgroup since most HC had no depressive symptoms (HAMD sum score ≦2 in 79% of HCs), resulting in a heavily skewed distribution of HAMD sum scores in HC. Moreover, all HC would have been excluded due to missing values in disease course characteristics. We opted for this correlational approach instead of mediation analysis as the assumption of causality necessary for mediation analyses cannot be verified using cross-sectional data (for exploratory mediation analysis with results in line with our approach, see online Supplement S13).

Results

Analyses of cognitive performance

Adequacy of the data for conducting an EFA was confirmed by Bartlett's test (χ2 = 10 567.97, p < 0.001) and Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin test (KMO = 0.90). The EFA yielded three cognitive factors (see Fig. 1a for factor loadings). The first factor captured processing speed and lower executive functioning (CF-PS, explaining 41% of the variance), the second factor captured verbal learning and memory (CF-VLM, 13%), and the third factor captured higher executive functions, especially verbal fluency (CF-VF, 9%).

There were significant differences in cognitive performance between HC, MDDr, and MDDa in all three cognitive factors [CF-PS: F (2, 1478) = 79.09, p FDR < 0.001; CF-VLM: F (2, 1478) = 20.62, p FDR < 0.001; CF-VF: F (2, 1478) = 40.21, p FDR < 0.001]. Post-hoc t tests (see Fig. 1; see online Supplement S14 for test statistics) revealed that both MDDa and MDDr performed significantly worse than HC in processing speed and verbal fluency (all p FDR = 0.001, 0.277 ⩽ d ⩽ 0.517). In addition, MDDa performed significantly worse than HC in verbal learning and memory (p FDR = 0.019, d = 0.203) and significantly worse than MDDr in verbal fluency (p FDR = 0.001, d = 0.277). The latter remained significant when controlling for medication load, age of disease onset, or the number of depressive episodes or hospitalizations (see online Supplement S15). Besides, we tested for effects of patients' comorbidity on cognitive factors, which did not yield any significant results (see online Supplement S16). When outliers in cognitive factors were excluded, all but the differences between HC and MDDa in verbal learning and memory (p FDR = 0.067) remained significant (see online Supplement S17)

Associations between cognitive performance and the structural connectome

Linear regression models revealed six significant associations between cognitive factors and global connectome measures. More specifically, all three cognitive factors were associated with the mean number of streamlines per edge (CF-PS: t = 3.925, p FDR = 0.001, partial η 2 = 0.010; CF-VLM: t = −3.925, p FDR = 0.003, partial η 2 = 0.009; CF-VF: t = 3.047, p FDR = 0.012, partial η 2 = 0.006). Further, processing speed and verbal learning and memory were significantly associated with the total number of streamlines (t = 4.102, p FDR = 0.001, partial η 2 = 0.011 and t = −3.389, p FDR = 0.008, partial η 2 = 0.004) and verbal learning and memory was significantly associated with the non-normalized clustering coefficient (t = −3.620, p FDR = 0.003, partial η 2 = 0.009). HC, acutely depressed, and remitted MDD patients did not differ in these associations as indicated by non-significant remission status × cognitive factor interaction effects (all psFDR ⩾ 0.755, see online Supplement S18).

NBS analyses conducted to identify the subnetworks underlying the global-level associations yielded the following results: For each cognitive factor, NBS identified a subnetwork of connections related to the respective factor (NBS F-threshold = 5.8, all p FWE ⩽ 0.014, i.e. all p FWE ⩽ α = 0.0167, see Figs 2–4). These subnetworks comprised between 65 and 105 edges, which corresponds to 1.0% and 1.6% of the connectome. Connectivity strength within the subnetworks identified for processing speed and verbal fluency was positively associated with the respective factor (t = 8.939, p FDR < 0.001, partial η 2 = 0.051 and t = 2.654, p FDR = 0.008, partial η 2 = 0.005, respectively). In contrast, connectivity strength within the subnetwork identified for verbal learning and memory was negatively associated with the factor (t = −10.254, p FDR < 0.001, partial η 2 = 0.066). Notably, the pattern of structural connectivity differed across the subnetworks, as reflected by a low overlap of subnetwork edges (6–20%, see online Supplement S19). This is further supported by differences in the number of edges connecting frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital brain regions: For example, the CF-PS network comprised a large proportion of fronto-frontal edges (26%). In contrast, the CF-VLM network was characterized by a core of temporo-parietal edges (25%). All subnetworks were equally distributed across left and right hemispheres, indicating comparable involvement of both hemispheres in the cognitive processes captured in the cognitive factors. See online Supplement S19 for further details.

Fig. 2. Network of white matter tracts related to CF-PS performance. The figure shows the subnetwork of edges associated with the cognitive factor representing processing speed performance (CF-PS). The subnetwork was derived from network-based statistics (F-threshold = 5.8). Edges were positively (red) or negatively (blue) related to the cognitive factor (see online version for colored figures).

Fig. 3. Network of white matter tracts related to CF-VLM performance. The figure shows the subnetwork of edges associated with the cognitive factor representing verbal learning and memory (CF-VLM). The subnetwork was derived from network-based statistics (F-threshold = 5.8). Edges were positively (red) or negatively (blue) related to the cognitive factor (see online version for colored figures).

Fig. 4. Network of white matter tracts related to CF-VF performance. The figure shows the subnetwork of edges associated with the cognitive factor representing verbal fluency performance (CF-VF). The subnetwork was derived from network-based statistics (F-threshold = 5.8). Edges were positively (red) or negatively (blue) related to the cognitive factor (see online version for colored figures).

In line with our global-level results, we did not identify any significant subnetworks associated with remission status × cognitive factor interaction effects (all p FWE ⩾ 0.070, i.e. all p FWE > α = 0.0167, see online Supplement S18), suggesting that healthy and depressed individuals did not differ in the association of cognitive performance and local connectivity strength.

Robustness checks of our NBS analyses revealed no significant associations between identified subnetworks and participants' years of education or head motion during MRI acquisition (all ps ≥ 0.162). Moreover, neither the exclusion of outliers in cognitive factors nor the use of non-thresholded connectivity matrices for NBS analyses changed the overall pattern of our results (see online Supplement S20).

Effects of depression on identified subnetworks

ANCOVAs conducted to evaluate differences in structural connectivity patterns in acutely depressed and remitted MDD patients compared to HC yielded a significant remission status effect for the connectivity strength within the processing speed-related subnetwork [F (2, 1477) = 4.592, p FDR = 0.031]. Post-hoc t tests revealed a significantly reduced connectivity strength in MDDa compared to HC (t = −3.032, p FDR = 0.008, d = −0.200).

Correlational analyses performed to examine the triad of (1) acute depressive symptoms, (2) cognitive performance, and (3) subnetwork-specific connectivity strength within MDD patients revealed that when considering all three variables simultaneously, all partial correlations remained significant only within the processing speed-related triad (see online Supplement S21 for all results). According to these correlations, higher processing speed was associated with higher subnetwork-specific connectivity strength independent of depression severity (r CF−PS,NOS = 0.201, p FDR < 0.001), while increased depressive symptoms were associated with reductions in both variables (r HAMD,CF−PS = −0.126, p FDR = 0.002, and r HAMD,NOS = −0.105, p FDR = 0.006, respectively). When controlling for current medication load, the correlation between processing speed and acute depressive symptom severity did not remain significant, which might be due to a substantial correlation between acute depressive symptoms and current medication load (r = 0.390, p < 0.001, see online Supplement S23). In contrast, controlling for measures of the previous disease course, such as the age of disease onset or number of hospitalizations or depressive episodes, did not change the significance of the association. Post-hoc analyses did not reveal significant associations to specific symptoms (see online Supplement S22), indicating that the association is driven more by cumulative symptom severity rather than a specific set of symptoms.

Discussion

The present study investigated the association between cognitive performance and the structural connectome of the brain in a sample of acutely depressed or (partially) remitted MDD patients and HC. Our analyses yielded three main results: First, we replicate findings demonstrating substantial cognitive deficits across various cognitive domains in MDD. Second, based on network analyses, we show a link between cognitive performance and the structural connectome and present evidence for domain-specific properties of that link. Notably, we found no evidence for differential cognition–connectome associations in healthy and depressed individuals. Instead, our analyses demonstrate structural connectome alterations within cognition-related subnetworks that may be a neurobiological factor underlying cognitive deficits in MDD.

We found widespread cognitive deficits in both MDDa and MDDr. The most pronounced effects were found in processing speed and verbal fluency. These results confirm our first hypothesis and align with previous meta-analytic findings (Rock et al., Reference Rock, Roiser, Riedel and Blackwell2014). While our analyses are cross-sectional, they nevertheless support findings showing residual cognitive deficits in remitted MDD (Baune et al., Reference Baune, Fuhr, Air and Hering2014; Rock et al., Reference Rock, Roiser, Riedel and Blackwell2014; Semkovska et al., Reference Semkovska, Quinlivan, O'Grady, Johnson, Collins, O'Connor and Gload2019). Given the adverse effects of persistent cognitive deficits (Baune et al., Reference Baune, Fuhr, Air and Hering2014; Jaeger et al., Reference Jaeger, Berns, Uzelac and Davis-Conway2006), their treatment should be of high priority. Although available drugs and remediation programs tend to improve cognitive deficits, meta-analyses also criticize the small number of primary studies (Motter et al., Reference Motter, Pimontel, Rindskopf, Devanand, Doraiswamy and Sneed2016; Prado, Watt, & Crowe, Reference Prado, Watt and Crowe2018; Rosenblat, Kakar, & McIntyre, Reference Rosenblat, Kakar and McIntyre2015; Thérond et al., Reference Thérond, Pezzoli, Abbas, Howard, Bowie and Guimond2021). Therefore, future treatment research should consider the reduction of cognitive deficits as one of its primary goals.

Our analyses revealed associations between all three cognitive factors and the structural connectome, thus confirming our second hypothesis. Although we note the small effect sizes of the global-level associations, we also stress the more pronounced effects evident within the identified subnetworks. In our opinion, the fact that these effects are detectable even at the global level, albeit with small effect sizes, underscores the strength of the local-level effects rather than reducing the significance of our results. While few studies have analyzed the relationship between cognitive performance and the structural connectome (Jolly et al., Reference Jolly, Scott, Sharp and Hampshire2020; Ponsoda et al., Reference Ponsoda, Martínez, Pineda-Pardo, Abad, Olea, Román and Colom2017; Zimmermann et al., Reference Zimmermann, Griffiths and McIntosh2018), our study extends their findings in several ways. First, our study is based on the largest sample to date, as it doubles the sample sizes of previous studies. Second, while previous studies identified several edges associated with cognitive performance, our study is the first to identify complete networks, which is more consistent with the hypothesis that cognitive functions emerge from a network of interacting brain regions (Lim & Helpern, Reference Lim and Helpern2002). In addition, to our knowledge, this is the first study to identify domain-specific networks of white matter fiber tracts. While we acknowledge that our cognitive factors might not fully generalize to other datasets, our analysis may nevertheless provide preliminary evidence for the domain specificity of the cognition–connectome link and elicit future studies on this issue.

All subnetworks comprised edges that are positively or negatively associated with cognitive performance, which aligns with previous findings (Zimmermann et al., Reference Zimmermann, Griffiths and McIntosh2018). However, while mean connectivity strength within the respective subnetworks was positively associated with processing speed and verbal fluency, verbal learning and memory was negatively associated with both global and subnetwork-specific connectivity strength. An explanation might be found in the cognitive processes represented by our cognitive factors: The cognitive tests related to the first two factors require quick information processing and responses, whereas the tests related to the verbal learning and memory factor mainly require retrieval of stored information. While processing of new information relies on white matter, as reflected by reduced processing speed in individuals with white matter damage (Chiaravalloti & DeLuca, Reference Chiaravalloti and DeLuca2008; Filley & Fields, Reference Filley and Fields2016), retrieval of stored information rather depends on the gray matter (Hoffman et al., Reference Hoffman, Cox, Dykiert, Muñoz Maniega, Valdés Hernández, Bastin and Deary2017), as evidenced by memory deficits in individuals with gray matter atrophy (Filley & Fields, Reference Filley and Fields2016). Although both gray and white matter are essential for cognition in general, the divergence in the association of structural connectivity and cognition may, thus, reflect the differential contribution of white and gray matter to individual cognitive processes.

Our analyses provide evidence for substantial alterations within the processing speed-related subnetwork in depressed individuals. First, they reveal reduced connectivity strength in acutely depressed MDD patients compared to HC. This finding is complemented by analyses conducted within the MDD subgroup that demonstrate associations between structural connectivity, processing speed, and acute depressive symptom severity, independent of previous disease course characteristics. Of note, this relationship was not driven by a specific set of symptoms, but by the cumulative severity of all current depressive symptoms. To our knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate these reciprocal associations based on a network approach. While we acknowledge that this pattern was found only for one of our three cognitive factors, we note that this factor captured by far the largest proportion of variance of the cognitive tests and was associated with the most severe cognitive deficits in MDD patients. Although the cross-sectional study design limits causal inferences, our results suggest that altered brain network communication may underlie the link between depression severity and cognitive deficits. This would support our hypothesis that cognitive deficits emerge from alterations in cognition-related networks rather than from alterations of individual brain structures. While we cannot address the neurobiological mechanisms underlying these alterations in our study, previous research might support speculation about the implication of inflammatory or stress-related processes. For instance, a possible mechanism of this interplay might be found in increased cortisol levels that have been related to depression, cognitive performance, and white matter alterations (Gomez et al., Reference Gomez, Fleming, Keller, Flores, Kenna, DeBattista and Schatzberg2006; Keller et al., Reference Keller, Gomez, Williams, Lembke, Lazzeroni, Murphy and Schatzberg2017; Li, Ruan, Chen, & Fang, Reference Li, Ruan, Chen and Fang2021; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Watanabe, Kakeda, Yoshimura, Abe, Ide and Korogi2016). It could be hypothesized that increased cortisol levels during progressing depressive symptoms lead to alterations in brain networks responsible for cognitive functioning, which in turn results in cognitive deficits.

Our study has several strengths and limitations. The main strengths are the variety of cognitive tests and the large sample covering a broad spectrum of MDD patients. The main limitation is the cross-sectional study design: Although our sample includes various remission states and thus provides an approximation of the impact of disease progression, longitudinal studies are needed to draw definitive conclusions on this question. Besides, cognitive factor labels might be subjective in that other researchers may have chosen different labels for the factors. However, it is crucial to separate the subjectivity of the factor labels from the subjectivity of the factor scores. Since their calculation was purely data-driven, we consider the scores largely objective.

To conclude, our findings emphasize the importance of cognitive deficits in depression. Moreover, our study substantially extends previous findings of a relationship between cognitive performance and the structural connectome by performing network analyses in the largest sample to date. By including MDD patients in these network analyses for the first time, our study provides preliminary evidence for an association between cognitive deficits, reduced connectivity in cognition-related subnetworks, and depressive symptoms, supporting the hypothesis that cognitive deficits in depression are associated with structural connectome alterations. Future research should verify this relationship in longitudinal studies and investigate possible neurobiological mechanisms underlying this association.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291722004007

Acknowledgements

This work is part of the German multicenter consortium ‘Neurobiology of Affective Disorders. A translational perspective on brain structure and function’, funded by the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft DFG; Forschungsgruppe/Research Unit FOR2107). Principal investigators (PIs) with respective areas of responsibility in the FOR2107 consortium are: Work Package WP1, FOR2107/MACS cohort and brainimaging: Tilo Kircher (speaker FOR2107; DFG grant numbers KI 588/14-1, KI 588/14-2), Udo Dannlowski (co-speaker FOR2107; DA 1151/5-1, DA 1151/5-2), Axel Krug (KR 3822/5-1, KR 3822/7-2), Igor Nenadic (NE 2254/1-2), Carsten Konrad (KO 4291/3-1). CP1, biobank: Petra Pfefferle (PF 784/1-1, PF 784/1-2), Harald Renz (RE 737/20-1, 737/20-2). CP2, administration. Tilo Kircher (KI 588/15-1, KI 588/17-1), Udo Dannlowski (DA 1151/6-1), Carsten Konrad (KO 4291/4-1). Martijn van den Heuvel was supported by an ALW open (ALWOP.179) and VIDI (452-16-015) grant from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) and a Fellowship of MQ.

Financial support

This work was funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG, grant FOR2107 DA1151/5-1 and DA1151/5-2 to UD; SFB-TRR58, Projects C09 and Z02 to UD), the Interdisciplinary Center for Clinical Research (IZKF) of the medical faculty of Münster (grant Dan3/012/17 to UD), IMF Münster RE111604 to RR und RE111722 to RR, IMF Münster RE 22 17 07 to Jonathan Repple and the Deanery of the Medical Faculty of the University of Münster. The FOR2107 cohort project (WP1) was approved by the Ethics Committees of the Medical Faculties, University of Marburg (AZ: 07/14) and University of Münster (AZ: 2014-422-b-S). TH was supported by the German Research Foundation (DFG grants HA7070/2-2, HA7070/3, HA7070/4). MP was supported by an ERC Consolidator grant (ERC-COG 101001062) and a NWO VIDI grant of the Dutch Research Council (Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research Grant VIDI-452-16-015).

Conflict of interest

Tilo Kircher received unrestricted educational grants from Servier, Janssen, Recordati, Aristo, Otsuka, neuraxpharm. Markus Wöhr is a scientific advisor of Avisoft Bioacoustics. This cooperation has no relevance to the work that is covered in the manuscript. The other authors declare no financial interests or competing interests.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Data access and responsibility

All PIs take responsibility for the integrity of the respective study data and their components. All authors and coauthors had full access to all study data. Acknowledgements and members by Work Package (WP): WP1: Henrike Bröhl, Katharina Brosch, Bruno Dietsche, Rozbeh Elahi, Jennifer Engelen, Sabine Fischer, Jessica Heinen, Svenja Klingel, Felicitas Meier, Tina Meller, Julia-Katharina Pfarr, Kai Ringwald, Torsten Sauder, Simon Schmitt, Frederike Stein, Annette Tittmar, Dilara Yüksel (Dept. of Psychiatry, Marburg University). Mechthild Wallnig, Rita Werner (Core-Facility Brainimaging, Marburg University). Carmen Schade-Brittinger, Maik Hahmann (Coordinating Centre for Clinical Trials, Marburg). Michael Putzke (Psychiatric Hospital, Friedberg). Rolf Speier, Lutz Lenhard (Psychiatric Hospital, Haina). Birgit Köhnlein (Psychiatric Practice, Marburg). Peter Wulf, Jürgen Kleebach, Achim Becker (Psychiatric Hospital Hephata, Schwalmstadt-Treysa). Ruth Bär (Care facility Bischoff, Neunkirchen). Matthias Müller, Michael Franz, Siegfried Scharmann, Anja Haag, Kristina Spenner, Ulrich Ohlenschläger (Psychiatric Hospital Vitos, Marburg). Matthias Müller, Michael Franz, Bernd Kundermann (Psychiatric Hospital Vitos, Gießen). Christian Bürger, Katharina Dohm, Fanni Dzvonyar, Verena Enneking, Stella Fingas, Katharina Förster, Janik Goltermann, Dominik Grotegerd, Hannah Lemke, Susanne Meinert, Nils Opel, Ronny Redlich, Jonathan Repple, Katharina Thiel, Kordula Vorspohl, Bettina Walden, Lena Waltemate, Kira Flinkenflügel, Alexandra Winter, Dario Zaremba (Dept. of Psychiatry, University of Münster). Harald Kugel, Jochen Bauer, Walter Heindel, Birgit Vahrenkamp (Dept. of Clinical Radiology, University of Münster). Gereon Heuft, Gudrun Schneider (Dept. of Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University of Münster). Thomas Reker (LWL-Hospital Münster). Gisela Bartling (IPP Münster). Ulrike Buhlmann (Dept. of Clinical Psychology, University of Münster). WP2: Marco Bartz, Miriam Becker, Christine Blöcher, Annuska Berz, Moria Braun, Ingmar Conell, Debora dalla Vecchia, Darius Dietrich, Ezgi Esen, Sophia Estel, Jens Hensen, Ruhkshona Kayumova, Theresa Kisko, Rebekka Obermeier, Anika Pützer, Nivethini Sangarapillai, Özge Sungur, Clara Raithel, Tobias Redecker, Vanessa Sandermann, Finnja Schramm, Linda Tempel, Natalie Vermehren, Jakob Vörckel, Stephan Weingarten, Maria Willadsen, Cüneyt Yildiz (Faculty of Psychology, Marburg University). WP4: Jana Freff (Dept. of Psychiatry, University of Münster). Susanne Michels, Goutham Ganjam, Katharina Elsässer (Faculty of Pharmacy, Marburg University). Felix Ruben Picard, Nicole Löwer, Thomas Ruppersberg (Institute of Laboratory Medicine and Pathobiochemistry, Marburg University). WP5: Helene Dukal, Christine Hohmeyer, Lennard Stütz, Viola Lahr, Fabian Streit, Josef Frank, Lea Sirignano (Dept. of Genetic Epidemiology, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University). Stefanie Heilmann-Heimbach, Stefan Herms, Per Hoffmann (Institute of Human Genetics, University of Bonn, School of Medicine & University Hospital Bonn). Andreas J. Forstner (Institute of Human Genetics, University of Bonn, School of Medicine & University Hospital Bonn; Centre for Human Genetics, Marburg University). WP6: Anastasia Benedyk, Miriam Bopp, Roman Keßler, Maximilian Lückel, Verena Schuster, Christoph Vogelbacher (Dept. of Psychiatry, Marburg University). Jens Sommer, Olaf Steinsträter (Core-Facility Brainimaging, Marburg University). Thomas W.D. Möbius (Institute of Medical Informatics and Statistics, Kiel University). CP1: Julian Glandorf, Fabian Kormann, Arif Alkan, Fatana Wedi, Lea Henning, Alena Renker, Karina Schneider, Elisabeth Folwarczny, Dana Stenzel, Kai Wenk, Felix Picard, Alexandra Fischer, Sandra Blumenau, Beate Kleb, Doris Finholdt, Elisabeth Kinder, Tamara Wüst, Elvira Przypadlo, Corinna Brehm (Comprehensive Biomaterial Bank Marburg, Marburg University).

Footnotes

*

These authors contributed equally to this work.

References

Basser, P. J., Mattiello, J., & LeBihan, D. (1994). MR diffusion tensor spectroscopy and imaging. Biophysical Journal, 66(1), 259267. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3495(94)80775-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baune, B. T., Fuhr, M., Air, T., & Hering, C. (2014). Neuropsychological functioning in adolescents and young adults with major depressive disorder – A review. Psychiatry Research, 218, 261271. Elsevier Ireland Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.04.052.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baune, B. T., Miller, R., McAfoose, J., Johnson, M., Quirk, F., & Mitchell, D. (2010). The role of cognitive impairment in general functioning in major depression. Psychiatry Research, 176(2–3), 183189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.12.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x.Google Scholar
Bolandzadeh, N., Davis, J. C., Tam, R., Handy, T. C., & Liu-Ambrose, T. (2012). The association between cognitive function and white matter lesion location in older adults: A systematic review. BMC Neurology, 12(1), 126. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2377-12-126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buist-Bouwman, M. A., Ormel, J., De Graaf, R., De Jonge, P., Van Sonderen, E., Vollebergh, W. A. M., … Vilagut, G. (2008). Mediators of the association between depression and role functioning. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 118(6), 451458. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01285.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cammoun, L., Gigandet, X., Meskaldji, D., Thiran, J. P., Sporns, O., Do, K. Q., … Hagmann, P. (2012). Mapping the human connectome at multiple scales with diffusion spectrum MRI. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 203(2), 386397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.09.031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiaravalloti, N. D., & DeLuca, J. (2008). Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. The Lancet Neurology, 7(12), 11391151. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70259-X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conradi, H. J., Ormel, J., & De Jonge, P. (2011). Presence of individual (residual) symptoms during depressive episodes and periods of remission: A 3-year prospective study. Psychological Medicine, 41(6), 11651174. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291710001911.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Lange, S. C., & van den Heuvel, M. P. (2021). Structural and functional connectivity reconstruction with CATO – A Connectivity Analysis Toolbox. BioRxiv, 2021.05.31.446012. https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.05.31.446012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Reus, M. A., & van den Heuvel, M. P. (2013). Estimating false positives and negatives in brain networks. NeuroImage, 70, 402409. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.066.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desikan, R. S., Ségonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B. T., Dickerson, B. C., Blacker, D., … Killiany, R. J. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage, 31(3), 968980. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dhamala, E., Jamison, K. W., Jaywant, A., Dennis, S., & Kuceyeski, A. (2021). Distinct functional and structural connections predict crystallised and fluid cognition in healthy adults. Human Brain Mapping, 42(10), 31023118. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dwyer, T., Fornito, A., Pham, T. N., Shi, M., Smith, N., Manley, J., & Klapperstueck, M. (2017). Brain Data Viewer. Retrieved February 1, 2022, from Monash University website https://immersive.erc.monash.edu/neuromarvl/.Google Scholar
Eijlers, A. J. C., Van Geest, Q., Dekker, I., Steenwijk, M. D., Meijer, K. A., Hulst, H. E., … Geurts, J. J. G. (2018). Predicting cognitive decline in multiple sclerosis: A 5-year follow-up study. Brain, 141(9), 26052618. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awy202.Google ScholarPubMed
Filley, C. M., & Fields, R. D. (2016). White matter and cognition: Making the connection. Journal of Neurophysiology, 116(5), 20932104. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00221.2016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, E. I., & Nesse, R. M. (2014). The impact of individual depressive symptoms on impairment of psychosocial functioning. PLoS ONE, 9(2), e90311. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomez, R. G., Fleming, S. H., Keller, J., Flores, B., Kenna, H., DeBattista, C., … Schatzberg, A. F. (2006). The neuropsychological profile of psychotic major depression and its relation to cortisol. Biological Psychiatry, 60(5), 472478. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.11.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grumbach, P., Opel, N., Martin, S., Meinert, S., Leehr, E. J., Redlich, R., … Repple, J. (2020). Sleep duration is associated with white matter microstructure and cognitive performance in healthy adults. Human Brain Mapping, 41(15), 43974405. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gualtieri, C. T., & Morgan, D. W. (2008). The frequency of cognitive impairment in patients with anxiety, depression, and bipolar disorder: An unaccounted source of variance in clinical trials. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69(7), 11221130. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.v69n0712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, M. (1986). The Hamilton Rating Scale for depression. In Sartorius, N., & Ban, T. A. (Eds.), Assessment of Depression (pp. 143152). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-70486-4_14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Han, K. M., Choi, S., Jung, J., Na, K. S., Yoon, H. K., Lee, M. S., & Ham, B. J. (2014). Cortical thickness, cortical and subcortical volume, and white matter integrity in patients with their first episode of major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 155(1), 4248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.10.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, P., Cox, S. R., Dykiert, D., Muñoz Maniega, S., Valdés Hernández, M. C., Bastin, M. E., … Deary, I. J. (2017). Brain grey and white matter predictors of verbal ability traits in older age: The Lothian Birth Cohort 1936. NeuroImage, 156, 394402. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.052.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika, 30(2), 179185. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02289447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaeger, J., Berns, S., Uzelac, S., & Davis-Conway, S. (2006). Neurocognitive deficits and disability in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 145(1), 3948. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2005.11.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, S. L., Abate, D., Abate, K. H., Abay, S. M., Abbafati, C., Abbasi, N., … Murray, C. J. L. (2018). Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 354 diseases and injuries for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2017: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2017. The Lancet, 392(10159), 17891858. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(18)32279-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jiang, J., Zhao, Y. J., Hu, X. Y., Du, M. Y., Chen, Z. Q., Wu, M., … Gong, Q. Y. (2017). Microstructural brain abnormalities in medication-free patients with major depressive disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis of diffusion tensor imaging. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 42(3), 150163. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.150341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jolly, A. E., Scott, G. T., Sharp, D. J., & Hampshire, A. H. (2020). Distinct patterns of structural damage underlie working memory and reasoning deficits after traumatic brain injury. Brain, 143(3), 11581176. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awaa067.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karlsgodt, K. H. (2016). Diffusion imaging of white matter in schizophrenia: Progress and future directions. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 1(3), 209217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2015.12.001.Google ScholarPubMed
Keller, J., Gomez, R., Williams, G., Lembke, A., Lazzeroni, L., Murphy, G. M., & Schatzberg, A. F. (2017). HPA axis in major depression: Cortisol, clinical symptomatology and genetic variation predict cognition. Molecular Psychiatry, 22(4), 527536. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2016.120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kircher, T., Wöhr, M., Nenadic, I., Schwarting, R., Schratt, G., Alferink, J., … Dannlowski, U. (2019). Neurobiology of the major psychoses: A translational perspective on brain structure and function – The FOR2107 consortium. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 269(8), 949962. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-018-0943-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korgaonkar, M. S., Grieve, S. M., Koslow, S. H., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Gordon, E., & Williams, L. M. (2011). Loss of white matter integrity in major depressive disorder: Evidence using tract-based spatial statistical analysis of diffusion tensor imaging. Human Brain Mapping, 32(12), 21612171. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, Z., Ruan, M., Chen, J., & Fang, Y. (2021). Major depressive disorder: Advances in neuroscience research and translational applications. Neuroscience Bulletin, 37(6), 863880. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12264-021-00638-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lim, K. O., & Helpern, J. A. (2002). Neuropsychiatric applications of DTI – A review. NMR in Biomedicine, 15(7–8), 587593. https://doi.org/10.1002/nbm.789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, X., Watanabe, K., Kakeda, S., Yoshimura, R., Abe, O., Ide, S., … Korogi, Y. (2016). Relationship between white matter integrity and serum cortisol levels in drug-naive patients with major depressive disorder: Diffusion tensor imaging study using tract-based spatial statistics. British Journal of Psychiatry, 208(6), 585590. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.114.155689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MATLAB (2019). 9.7.0.1190202 (R2019b). Natick, Massachusetts: The MathWorks Inc.Google Scholar
McIntyre, R. S., Cha, D. S., Soczynska, J. K., Woldeyohannes, H. O., Gallaugher, L. A., Kudlow, P., … Baskaran, A. (2013). Cognitive deficits and functional outcomes in major depressive disorder: Determinants, substrates, and treatment interventions. Depression and Anxiety, 30(6), 515527. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22063.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLennan, S. N., & Mathias, J. L. (2010). The depression-executive dysfunction (DED) syndrome and response to antidepressants: A meta-analytic review. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 25, 933944. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.2431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mettenburg, J. M., Benzinger, T. L., Shimony, J. S., Snyder, A. Z., & Sheline, Y. I. (2012). Diminished performance on neuropsychological testing in late life depression is correlated with microstructural white matter abnormalities. NeuroImage, 60(4), 21822190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mollison, D., Sellar, R., Bastin, M., Mollison, D., Chandran, S., Wardlaw, J., … Connick, P. (2017). The clinico-radiological paradox of cognitive function and MRI burden of white matter lesions in people with multiple sclerosis: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE, 12, 116. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Motter, J. N., Pimontel, M. A., Rindskopf, D., Devanand, D. P., Doraiswamy, P. M., & Sneed, J. R. (2016). Computerized cognitive training and functional recovery in major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 189, 184191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, M. L., & Frodl, T. (2011). Meta-analysis of diffusion tensor imaging studies shows altered fractional anisotropy occurring in distinct brain areas in association with depression. Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders, 1(1), 112. https://doi.org/10.1186/2045-5380-1-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myung, W., Han, C. E., Fava, M., Mischoulon, D., Papakostas, G. I., Heo, J. Y., … Jeon, H. J. (2016). Reduced frontal-subcortical white matter connectivity in association with suicidal ideation in major depressive disorder. Translational Psychiatry, 6(6), e835. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2016.110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Opel, N., Redlich, R., Dohm, K., Zaremba, D., Goltermann, J., Repple, J., … Dannlowski, U. (2019). Mediation of the influence of childhood maltreatment on depression relapse by cortical structure: A 2-year longitudinal observational study. The Lancet Psychiatry, 6(4), 318326. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30044-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ponsoda, V., Martínez, K., Pineda-Pardo, J. A., Abad, F. J., Olea, J., Román, F. J., … Colom, R. (2017). Structural brain connectivity and cognitive ability differences: A multivariate distance matrix regression analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 38(2), 803816. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prado, C. E., Watt, S., & Crowe, S. F. (2018). A meta-analysis of the effects of antidepressants on cognitive functioning in depressed and non-depressed samples. Neuropsychology Review, 28(1), 3272. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-018-9369-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redlich, R., Almeida, J. R., Grotegerd, D., Opel, N., Kugel, H., Heindel, W., … Dannlowski, U. (2014). Brain morphometric biomarkers distinguishing unipolar and bipolar depression: A voxel-based morphometry-pattern classification approach. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(11), 12221230. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Repple, J., Mauritz, M., Meinert, S., de Lange, S. C., Grotegerd, D., Opel, N., … van den Heuvel, M. P. (2020). Severity of current depression and remission status are associated with structural connectome alterations in major depressive disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 25(7), 15501558. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-019-0603-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Repple, J., Meinert, S., Grotegerd, D., Kugel, H., Redlich, R., Dohm, K., … Dannlowski, U. (2017). A voxel-based diffusion tensor imaging study in unipolar and bipolar depression. Bipolar Disorders, 19(1), 2331. https://doi.org/10.1111/bdi.12465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Repple, J., Zaremba, D., Meinert, S., Grotegerd, D., Redlich, R., Förster, K., … Dannlowski, U. (2019). Time heals all wounds? A 2–year longitudinal diffusion tensor imaging study in major depressive disorder. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 44(6), 407413. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.180243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizk, M. M., Rubin-Falcone, H., Keilp, J., Miller, J. M., Sublette, M. E., Burke, A., … Mann, J. J. (2017). White matter correlates of impaired attention control in major depressive disorder and healthy volunteers. Journal of Affective Disorders, 222, 103111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.06.066.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rock, P. L., Roiser, J. P., Riedel, W. J., & Blackwell, A. D. (2014). Cognitive impairment in depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 44, 20292040. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291713002535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenblat, J. D., Kakar, R., & McIntyre, R. S. (2015). The cognitive effects of antidepressants in major depressive disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized clinical trials. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 19(2), 113. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijnp/pyv082.Google ScholarPubMed
Rubinov, M., & Sporns, O. (2010). Complex network measures of brain connectivity: Uses and interpretations. NeuroImage, 52(3), 10591069. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sacchet, M. D., Prasad, G., Foland-Ross, L. C., Thompson, P. M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2015). Support vector machine classification of major depressive disorder using diffusion-weighted neuroimaging and graph theory. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 6(Feb), 21. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwert, C., Stohrer, M., Aschenbrenner, S., Weisbrod, M., & Schröder, A. (2019). Neurocognitive profile of outpatients with unipolar depressive disorders. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 41(9), 913924. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2019.1634180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Semkovska, M., Quinlivan, L., O'Grady, T., Johnson, R., Collins, A., O'Connor, J., … Gload, T. (2019). Cognitive function following a major depressive episode: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 6(10), 851861. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30291-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thérond, A., Pezzoli, P., Abbas, M., Howard, A., Bowie, C. R., & Guimond, S. (2021). The efficacy of cognitive remediation in depression: A systematic literature review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 284, 238246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2021.02.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Heuvel, M. P., & Sporns, O. (2013). Network hubs in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(12), 683696. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Rossum, G., & Drake, F. L. (2019). Python 3 reference manual. Scotts Valley, CA: CreateSpace.Google Scholar
van Velzen, L. S., Kelly, S., Isaev, D., Aleman, A., Aftanas, L. I., Bauer, J., … Schmaal, L. (2020). White matter disturbances in major depressive disorder: A coordinated analysis across 20 international cohorts in the ENIGMA MDD working group. Molecular Psychiatry, 25(7), 15111525. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-019-0477-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vogelbacher, C., Möbius, T. W. D., Sommer, J., Schuster, V., Dannlowski, U., Kircher, T., … Bopp, M. H. A. (2018). The Marburg-Münster Affective Disorders Cohort Study (MACS): A quality assurance protocol for MR neuroimaging data. NeuroImage, 172, 450460. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.079.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Welton, T., Kent, D., Constantinescu, C. S., Auer, D. P., & Dineen, R. A. (2015). Functionally relevant white matter degradation in multiple sclerosis: A tract-based spatial meta-analysis. Radiology, 275(1), 8996. https://doi.org/10.1148/radiol.14140925.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiseman, S. J., Booth, T., Ritchie, S. J., Cox, S. R., Muñoz Maniega, S., Valdés Hernández, M. d. C., … Bastin, M. E. (2018). Cognitive abilities, brain white matter hyperintensity volume, and structural network connectivity in older age. Human Brain Mapping, 39(2), 622632. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23857.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wittchen, H., Wunderlich, U., Gruschwitz, S., & Zaudig, M. (1997). SKID I. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV. Achse I: Psychische Störungen. Interviewheft und Beurteilungsheft. Göttingen, Germany.Google Scholar
Yamada, S., Takahashi, S., Ukai, S., Tsuji, T., Iwatani, J., Tsuda, K., … Shinosaki, K. (2015). Microstructural abnormalities in anterior callosal fibers and their relationship with cognitive function in major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder: A tract-specific analysis study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 174, 542548. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.12.022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, F. N., Hassanzadeh-Behbahani, S., Bronshteyn, M., Dawson, M., Kumar, P., Moore, D. J., … Jiang, X. (2021). Connectome-based prediction of global cognitive performance in people with HIV. NeuroImage: Clinical, 30, 102677. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102677.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zalesky, A., Fornito, A., & Bullmore, E. T. (2010). Network-based statistic: Identifying differences in brain networks. NeuroImage, 53(4), 11971207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.041.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zalesky, A., Fornito, A., Cocchi, L., Gollo, L. L., van den Heuvel, M. P., & Breakspear, M. (2016). Connectome sensitivity or specificity: Which is more important? NeuroImage, 142, 407420. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2016.06.035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, J., Cortese, R., De Stefano, N., & Giorgio, A. (2021). Structural and functional connectivity substrates of cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Frontiers in Neurology, 12, 671894. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2021.671894.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmermann, J., Griffiths, J. D., & McIntosh, A. R. (2018). Unique mapping of structural and functional connectivity on cognition. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(45), 96589667. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0900-18.2018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Results from analyses on cognitive performance. (a) Factor loadings of cognitive test scores resulting from exploratory factor analysis. According to this analysis, three factors represent the variance within the cognitive performance of the sample: CF-PS, cognitive factor representing processing speed; CF-VLM, cognitive factor representing verbal learning and memory; CF-VF, cognitive factor representing verbal fluency; VLMT 1–5, verbal learning; VLMT 6, immediate verbal memory; VLMT 7, delayed verbal memory; VLMT 8, recognition; DSST, Digit Symbol Substitution Test; TMT, Trail Making Test; D2, D2 Test of attention; LNST, Letter Number Sequencing Test; Corsi fwd., Crosi Block-Tapping Test forward; Crosi bw., Corsi Block-Tapping Test backwards; VF category, semantic verbal fluency; VF letter, phenomic verbal fluency; VF altern., cognitive flexibility. (b) Differences in cognitive factors between healthy controls (HC) and patients with acute (MDDa) or (partially) remitted (MDDr) episode of major depressive disorder. *pFDR ≤ 0.05; **pFDR ≤  0.01; ***pFDR ≤  0.001. Boxes indicate the lower and upper quartile of the CF. Black lines within the boxes indicate the mean of the CF. Whiskers indicate the 1.5 inter-quartile range of the CF's lower and upper quartile. Diamonds indicate observations that fall outside this range. (c) Effect sizes (Cohen's d) representing these between-group differences. See online version for colored figures.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Network of white matter tracts related to CF-PS performance. The figure shows the subnetwork of edges associated with the cognitive factor representing processing speed performance (CF-PS). The subnetwork was derived from network-based statistics (F-threshold = 5.8). Edges were positively (red) or negatively (blue) related to the cognitive factor (see online version for colored figures).

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Network of white matter tracts related to CF-VLM performance. The figure shows the subnetwork of edges associated with the cognitive factor representing verbal learning and memory (CF-VLM). The subnetwork was derived from network-based statistics (F-threshold = 5.8). Edges were positively (red) or negatively (blue) related to the cognitive factor (see online version for colored figures).

Figure 4

Fig. 4. Network of white matter tracts related to CF-VF performance. The figure shows the subnetwork of edges associated with the cognitive factor representing verbal fluency performance (CF-VF). The subnetwork was derived from network-based statistics (F-threshold = 5.8). Edges were positively (red) or negatively (blue) related to the cognitive factor (see online version for colored figures).

Supplementary material: File

Gruber et al. supplementary material

Gruber et al. supplementary material

Download Gruber et al. supplementary material(File)
File 548.2 KB