Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T00:47:07.693Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Association between prenatal maternal infection and disordered eating behaviours in adolescence: a UK population-based prospective birth cohort study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 May 2019

Francesca Solmi*
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University College London, London, UK
Bianca L. De Stavola
Affiliation:
Great Ormond Street Institute of Child Health, UCL, London, UK
Golam M. Khandaker
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, UK
Cynthia M. Bulik
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, USA Department of Nutrition, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, USA Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
Christina Dalman
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
Glyn Lewis
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University College London, London, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Francesca Solmi, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Prenatal infections have been proposed as a putative risk factor for a number of psychiatric outcomes across a continuum of severity. Evidence on eating disorders is scarce. We investigated whether exposure to prenatal maternal infections is associated with an increased risk of disordered eating and weight and shape concerns in adolescence in a large UK birth cohort.

Methods

We used data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. The primary exposure was maternal experience of infections at any time in pregnancy. Study outcomes were presence of any, monthly or weekly disordered eating at 14 and 16 years of age, and weight and shape concerns at 14 years. We defined the causal effect of the exposure on these outcomes using a counterfactual framework adjusting our analyses for a number of hypothesised confounders, and imputing missing confounder data using multiple imputation.

Results

In total, 4884 children had complete exposure and outcome data at age 14 years, and 4124 at 16 years. Exposed children had a greater risk of reporting weekly disordered eating at both age 14 [risk difference (RD) 0.9%, 95% confidence interval (CI) −0.01 to 1.9, p = 0.08] and 16 (RD 2.3%, 95% CI 0.6–3.9, p < 0.01), though evidence of an association was weak at age 14 years. Exposed children also had greater weight and shape concerns at age 14 years (mean difference 0.15, 95% CI 0.05–0.26, p < 0.01).

Conclusions

Exposure to prenatal maternal infection is associated with greater risk of disordered eating in adolescence. This association could be explained by in utero processes leading to impaired neurodevelopment or altered immunological profiles. Residual confounding cannot be excluded.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Introduction

A growing body of literature suggests that exposure to prenatal infections might have a role in the aetiology of a number of psychiatric conditions (Flinkkilä et al., Reference Flinkkilä, Keski-Rahkonen, Marttunen and Raevuori2016). Prospective studies using large general population samples have shown that incidence of non-affective psychoses (Buka et al., Reference Buka, Tsuang, Torrey, Klebanoff, Bernstein and Yolken2001; Brown et al., Reference Brown, Begg, Gravenstein, Schaefer, Wyatt, Bresnahan, Babulas and Susser2004; Mortensen et al., Reference Mortensen, Nørgaard-Pedersen, Waltoft, Sørensen, Hougaard, Torrey and Yolken2007; Xiao et al., Reference Xiao, Buka, Cannon, Suzuki, Viscidi, Torrey and Yolken2009; Brown and Derkits, Reference Brown and Derkits2010; Khandaker et al., Reference Khandaker, Zimbron, Lewis and Jones2013), bipolar disorder (Parboosing et al., Reference Parboosing, Bao, Shen, Schaefer and Brown2013) and autism spectrum disorders (Atladóttir et al., Reference Atladóttir, Thorsen, Østergaard, Schendel, Lemcke, Abdallah and Parner2010; Canetta et al., Reference Canetta, Sourander, Surcel, Hinkka-Yli-Salomäki, Leiviskä, Kellendonk, McKeague and Brown2014; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Magnusson, Gardner, Blomström, Newschaffer, Burstyn, Karlsson and Dalman2015; Hornig et al., Reference Hornig, Bresnahan, Che, Schultz, Ukaigwe, Eddy, Hirtz, Gunnes, Lie, Magnus, Mjaaland, Reichborn-Kjennerud, Schjølberg, Øyen, Levin, Susser, Stoltenberg and Lipkin2018) is greater among individuals whose mother had an infection during pregnancy (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Begg, Gravenstein, Schaefer, Wyatt, Bresnahan, Babulas and Susser2004; Xiao et al., Reference Xiao, Buka, Cannon, Suzuki, Viscidi, Torrey and Yolken2009; Parboosing et al., Reference Parboosing, Bao, Shen, Schaefer and Brown2013). In comparison, the study of eating disorders has received remarkably little attention in the epidemiological literature (Favaro et al., Reference Favaro, Tenconi, Ceschin, Zanetti, Bosello and Santonastaso2011; Lydholm et al., Reference Lydholm, Köhler-Forsberg, Nordentoft, Yolken, Mortensen, Petersen and Benros2018).

An association between exposure to prenatal infections and eating disorders is plausible for several reasons. Individuals with eating disorders often have co-morbid autistic traits (Gesi et al., Reference Gesi, Carmassi, Luciano, Bossini, Ricca, Fagiolini, Maj and Dell'Osso2017; Christensen et al., Reference Christensen, Bentz, Clemmensen, Strandberg-Larsen and Olsen2019), psychotic symptoms (Koyanagi et al., Reference Koyanagi, Stickley and Haro2016; Solmi et al., Reference Solmi, Melamed, Lewis and Kirkbride2018) and mood disorders (Hudson et al., Reference Hudson, Hiripi, Pope and Kessler2007). All of these conditions have been shown to be more common among people exposed to infections in utero (Zammit et al., Reference Zammit, Odd, Horwood, Thompson, Thomas, Menezes, Gunnell, Hollis, Wolke, Lewis and Harrison2009; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Trevena, Simpson, Barratt, Nutbeam and McCaffery2010; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Magnusson, Gardner, Blomström, Newschaffer, Burstyn, Karlsson and Dalman2015; Simanek and Meier, Reference Simanek and Meier2015). Depression and psychosis are associated with dysregulated immunological profiles and, particularly, with increased inflammatory marker levels in peripheral blood (Upthegrove et al., Reference Upthegrove, Manzanares-Teson and Barnes2014; Goldsmith et al., Reference Goldsmith, Rapaport and Miller2016). A meta-analysis of clinical studies has shown that similar immunological abnormalities are also observed in people with anorexia nervosa (Solmi et al., Reference Solmi, Veronese, Favaro, Santonastaso, Manzato, Sergi and Correll2015b; Dalton et al., Reference Dalton, Bartholdy, Robinson, Solmi, Ibrahim, Breen, Schmidt and Himmerich2018). Previous studies have also shown that exposure to prenatal infections is associated with impaired neurodevelopmental traits [e.g. set-shifting (Khandaker et al., Reference Khandaker, Zimbron, Lewis and Jones2013), IQ (Benros et al., Reference Benros, Sørensen, Nielsen, Nordentoft, Mortensen and Petersen2015; Khandaker et al., Reference Khandaker, Dalman, Kappelmann, Stochl, Dal, Kosidou, Jones and Karlsson2018)], which are also observed in individuals with eating disorders, particularly anorexia nervosa, in general population and clinical samples (Gillberg et al., Reference Gillberg, Rastam, Wentz, Gillberg, Carina Gillberg, Råstam, Wentz and Gillberg2007; Lang et al., Reference Lang, Lloyd, Khondoker, Simic, Treasure and Tchanturia2015).

To the best of our knowledge, only two investigations have tested the hypothesis that maternal infections during gestation are associated with increased risk of eating disorder in the offspring (Favaro et al., Reference Favaro, Tenconi, Ceschin, Zanetti, Bosello and Santonastaso2011). One found that children whose gestational period coincided with peak epidemics of chickenpox and rubella had an increased risk of developing anorexia nervosa (Favaro et al., Reference Favaro, Tenconi, Ceschin, Zanetti, Bosello and Santonastaso2011). However, due to its ecological design, this study could not ascertain whether mothers had actually contracted any of these infections, therefore causal inferences cannot be drawn. Another recent study, using Danish register data, did not find an association between maternal infections during the prenatal period and greater risk of behavioural syndromes, i.e. including eating disorders, in the offspring after adjustment for multiple comparisons (Lydholm et al., Reference Lydholm, Köhler-Forsberg, Nordentoft, Yolken, Mortensen, Petersen and Benros2018). However, the authors did not report eating disorder-specific estimates; hence, it is unclear if a specific association might exist.

To overcome some of the limitations of previous research, we investigated whether exposure to prenatal infections was associated with an increased risk of disordered eating behaviours (i.e. binge-eating, purging, dieting, fasting) and cognitions (i.e. weight and shape concerns) in adolescence using longitudinal data from a UK birth cohort sample. Disordered eating behaviours such as binge-eating, purging, fasting and dieting, and weight and shape concerns represent core trans-diagnostic features of eating disorder psychopathology (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Although not everyone engaging in disordered eating transitions to a diagnosis, research suggests that those who do are at an increased risk of developing an eating disorder (Stice, Reference Stice2016). As eating disorders are relatively uncommon (Smink et al., Reference Smink, van Hoeken and Hoek2013), using behavioural and cognitive phenotypes, relying on the hypothesis that eating disorders occur on a continuum of severity (Dancyger and Garfinkel, Reference Dancyger and Garfinkel1995; Tylka and Subich, Reference Tylka and Subich1999), can aide aetiological research in general population samples (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Vaillancourt and Hanna2009). However, it is not yet known whether individuals with more severe eating disorder presentations represent aetiologically distinct entities from low-threshold behaviours more common in the general population. Therefore, to capture these putative differences, in this study, we tested our hypothesis using both broad and narrow definitions of disordered eating.

Method

Sample

We used data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC), a general population cohort study of women and their offspring. A total of 14 451 women with an expected delivery date between 1 April 1991 and 31 December 1992 residing in the former region of Avon were recruited. The core ALSPAC sample consists of 14 062 live births, of whom 13 988 were alive at one year. More details on the ALSPAC cohort can be found in previous publications (Boyd et al., Reference Boyd, Golding, Macleod, Lawlor, Fraser, Henderson, Molloy, Ness, Ring, Davey Smith, Macdonald-Wallis, Tilling, Boyd, Golding, Davey Smith, Henderson, Macleod, Molloy, Ness, Ring, Nelson and Lawlor2013; Fraser et al., Reference Fraser, Macdonald-Wallis, Tilling, Boyd, Golding, Davey Smith, Henderson, Macleod, Molloy, Ness, Ring, Nelson and Lawlor2013) and on the study website (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac). This also contains details of all the data that are available through a fully searchable data dictionary available at: http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/our-data/.

In this study, we included children in the core ALSPAC sample who had complete data on our primary exposure. In case of twins, we excluded the second born (twin ‘B’). Birth order could not be a confounder of the association between our exposure and outcome, because both twins would be exposed to infection and there is no evidence that twin birth order is associated with eating disorder risk (Holland et al., Reference Holland, Hall, Murray, Russell and Crisp1984). Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Law and Ethics committee and the Local Research Ethics committees.

Exposures

At 18 weeks of gestation, participating mothers were asked using questionnaire whether they had had influenza, thrush, rubella, genital herpes, urinary tract infections (UTIs) or any other infection in the first trimester of pregnancy or from the fourth month of pregnancy until the interview. Possible answers were: ‘no; in the first 3 months; from the fourth month until now; or at both time points’. At 32 weeks of gestation and at 8 weeks postpartum, women were also asked whether they had experienced the same conditions in the previous 3 months (i.e. from approximately the fourth month of gestation until the seventh month) and during the last 2 months of pregnancy, respectively. Since we could not identify which type of infection(s) would be included in ‘any other infections’, we defined our primary exposure as maternal self-reported any infection (influenza, thrush, rubella, genital herpes, UTI) anytime during pregnancy. In sensitivity analyses, we also re-ran all models with an alternative definition of the exposure using this additional information.

Outcomes: disordered eating behaviours, weight and shape concerns

Our primary outcomes were presence of disordered eating behaviours (any, monthly and weekly), and weight and shape concerns at age 14 years. As sensitivity analyses, we explored disordered eating behaviours at age 16 years, when disordered eating was more common, although data were affected by greater proportions of missing data because of attrition.

Disordered eating behaviours

At age 14 and 16 years, children answered postal questionnaires including a battery of questions adapted from the Youth Risk Behaviour Survey questionnaire (Brener et al., Reference Brener, Collins, Kann, Warren and Williams1995) investigating the presence of disordered eating behaviours in the previous 12 months.

We used data from both time points to test the robustness and consistency of the observed associations (if any). Moreover, the prevalence of disordered eating (and particularly of purging behaviours) increases from early to mid-adolescence in this sample. Therefore, although there is greater outcome missingness at age 16 years, the overall prevalence of the outcome is greater, increasing the statistical power of our analyses. Adolescents were asked whether they had fasted (i.e. not eaten in 24 h to lose weight), purged (i.e. thrown up or used laxative for weight loss) or binge eaten (i.e. eaten a large amount of food in a short period of time with a sense of loss of control) and with which frequency (never, less than monthly, monthly and weekly/daily). They were also asked if they had dieted and, if so, how frequently and for how long. As dieting is a very common behaviour, particularly among girls (Neumark-Sztainer et al., Reference Neumark-Sztainer, Wall, Haines, Story and Eisenberg2007), we only defined as having dieted adolescents who reported being on a diet ‘several times’, ‘often’ or ‘always’’ on a diet, and if they reported dieting for at least 1 month and up to 12 months continuously. This allowed us to restrict this definition to the more severe end of the dieting spectrum and align its definition with that of other behaviours we considered. These questions have been previously validated [questions on binge-eating were validated by Field et al. (Reference Field, Taylor, Celio and Colditz2004)] and used in a number of previous studies (Micali et al., Reference Micali, Solmi, Horton, Crosby, Eddy, Calzo, Sonneville, Swanson and Field2015a, Reference Micali, Horton, Crosby, Swanson, Sonneville, Solmi, Calzo, Eddy and Field2017; Solmi et al., Reference Solmi, Sonneville, Easter, Horton, Crosby, Rodriguez, Jarvelin, Field and Micali2015a, Reference Solmi, Melamed, Lewis and Kirkbride2018; Sonneville et al., Reference Sonneville, Calzo, Horton, Field, Crosby, Solmi and Micali2015).

From these four behavioural variables, we created two outcome variables. First, an overall indicator denoting whether the adolescents experienced any dieting, binge-eating, purging or fasting on at least a monthly basis. Second, a variable indicating whether the adolescent had any monthly disordered eating behaviours and/or dieting; or any weekly disordered eating behaviours (bingeing, purging, fasting) and/or also dieted. We created this second variable to test the presence of threshold effects in relation to more severe disordered eating presentations, as weekly disordered eating episodes are the threshold defined by DSM-5 diagnoses. (More details are provided in online Supplementary Material.)

Weight and shape concerns

Weight and shape concerns were measured only at age 14 years with three questions from the McKnight Risk Factor Survey (Shisslak et al., Reference Shisslak, Renger, Sharpe, Crago, McKnight, Gray, Bryson, Estes, Parnaby, Killen and Taylor1999) asking ‘in the past year’ (1) ‘how happy have you been with the way your body looks?’; (2) ‘how much has your weight made a difference to how you feel about yourself?’; and (3) ‘how much have you worried about gaining a little weight (as little as 1 kilo)?’. Using a four-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (‘very happy’ for question 1, ‘not at all’ for questions 2 and 3), to 3 (‘very unhappy’ for question 1, ‘a lot’ for questions 2 and 3), we combined individual items to create a composite scale ranging from zero to nine, with higher scores indicating greater weight and shape concerns. This scale has good internal consistency given the low number of items (Chronbach's α = 0.66) and has previously been used in this sample (Micali et al., Reference Micali, De Stavola, Ploubidis, Simonoff, Treasure and Field2015b).

Causal assumptions

In Fig. 1, we show our causal assumptions depicted using Direct Acyclic Graphs (DAGs) (Greenland et al., Reference Greenland, Pearl and Robins1999) and the web programme DAGitty (Textor et al., Reference Textor, Hardt and Knüppel2011). Based on our assumptions, in order to estimate the total effect of the exposure (prenatal infections) on each of the outcomes (eating disorder behaviours and cognitions), it would be sufficient to adjust our models for: maternal depression, maternal pre-pregnancy body mass index (BMI), family socio-economic status (captured by: maternal age, paternal profession and maternal highest education), smoking in the first trimester of pregnancy and lifetime diabetes. We provide extensive details on the rationale for inclusion of each of these variables in online Supplementary Material.

Fig. 1. Direct Acyclic Graphs describing the causal assumptions for our analytical models.

Confounders

Maternal depression, maternal pre-pregnancy BMI and lifetime history of diabetes were measured at 12 weeks of gestation. Women were asked whether they had ever suffered from severe depression. Possible answers were: no; yes, recently; yes, in the past, which we recoded as a binary variable (never/yes, at some point). Maternal pre-pregnancy BMI was calculated as kg/m2 from maternally-reported measures of height and pre-pregnancy weight and used as a continuous variable. History of diabetes was also self-reported (possible answers: no/yes).

At 18 weeks of gestation, women were asked how many cigarettes they had smoked per day in the first 3 months of pregnancy. Possible answers were none to over 30, which we recoded as a binary variable (none/one or more). At 32 weeks of gestation, women reported their highest academic education level (compulsory/non-compulsory) and the profession of their partner (manual/non-manual). Although these questions were asked after the exposure, it is unlikely that for a large proportion of the sample these would have changed since the beginning of the pregnancy, hence were used as potential confounders. Maternal age at delivery was derived from the date of delivery and the mother's date of birth collected at study enrolment.

Statistical analyses

We described the sample characteristics using means and standard deviations and frequencies with proportions.

To investigate the association between exposure to prenatal infections (denoted X) and each of our outcomes (Y), we adopted a counterfactual framework approach to define the causal effect of X on Y, denoted hereafter average causal effect (ACE), as the difference of mean potential outcomes (POs) (Hernan and Robins, Reference Hernan and Robins2019). Let Yx represent the outcome that would occur had the exposure level be set to take the value X = x, and E(Yx) the PO averaged across the population of interest. For a binary exposure taking a value 0 or 1,

$${\rm ACE} = E(Y^1)-E(Y^0).$$

For binary outcomes, such as most of those studied here, the ACE is the comparison of two risks and therefore is a risk difference (RD) measure; when the outcome of interest is a count, it measures the difference in the mean number of events (MD). This definition has a causal connotation as it compares outcomes in worlds where all individuals in the populations are exposed v. when no one is exposed. Estimation was implemented in Stata 15 (StataCorp, 2017) with the teffects ra (i.e. treatment effects with regression adjustment) command, under the assumption of no unmeasured confounding (or equivalent, conditional exchangeability) and the technical assumptions of no interference and counterfactual consistency (Stata Press, 2017). This procedure fits separate regression models for the outcome on the previously identified confounders separately for each exposure level and averaging the outcomes predicted by each model over all the data to estimate E(Y 1) and E(Y 0). For binary outcomes, we used a logistic regression model to predict the POs; for count outcomes, we used Poisson regression. Results include the mean PO in the unexposed (to reflect the reference level) and, for binary outcomes, also the estimated causal risk ratios (reported as percentage risk increases). For results based on Poisson regression, the inference was based on robust standard errors to account for event over-dispersion. To investigate the impact of measured confounding, we also report the estimated crude associations between each exposure and each outcome.

We dealt with the effect of missing data on our estimates by using multiple imputation with chained equations, on the assumption that data would be missing at random, conditionally on the variables included in the models. We imputed 50 datasets using the Stata mi impute chained command using linear, logistic, multinomial logistic regression models according to the nature of the variable imputed. In our imputation model, we included all variables used in our analysis model and a list of auxiliary variables which we hypothesised would be (or there is evidence in the literature that are) associated with exposure, outcome and missingness. Here, we present the results based on participants with complete exposure and outcome and missing, and hence imputed, confounder data. In online Supplementary Material, we present the findings of complete case analyses. All analyses were conducted in Stata 15 (StataCorp, 2017).

Results

Sample

Of all children in the core sample alive at one year restricted to singletons and one twin (N = 13 793), 10 202 children (73.9%) had available exposure data across the three trimesters of pregnancy. Of these, 4785 (46.9%) had available outcome data at 14 years of age and 4111 (40.3%) at age 16 years.

Table 1 shows the demographic characteristics of children with complete exposure data and outcome data at 14 years. The majority of mothers had only completed compulsory education (n = 2571, 53.9%), had a partner working in a non-manual profession (n = 2893, 63.8%), did not smoke in the first trimester of pregnancy (n = 4026, 84.4%) and did not have a history of severe depression (n = 4426, 93.2%). Mean age of mothers in the sample was 29.3 years [standard deviation (s.d.) = 4.4] and mean pre-pregnancy BMI was 22.8 (s.d. = 3.7).

Table 1. Sample characteristics and their distributions among exposed children (complete exposure and outcome at age 14 years, n = 4785)

a Column percentages showing distribution of the variable in the sample.

b Row percentages, showing proportions with the outcome in each level of the confounder.

s.d., standard deviation.

A total of 2034 women (42.5%) reported the occurrence of at least one of our target infections (i.e. not including ‘other’ infections) during the whole prenatal period (see Table 1). As expected, infections did not vary by child's sex; however, they were more common in mothers with lower education and who had a partner working in a manual occupation. A greater proportion of mothers with lifetime depression and who smoked reported having an infection, but fewer with a history of diabetes did so. On average, mothers who experienced an infection were slightly younger, but had similar BMI compared with those who did not. When including ‘other’ infections, the proportion of mothers reporting any infection increased to 51.3% (n = 2453, see online Supplementary Table S1). Although mothers with depression and who smoked still reported more infections using this definition, now the latter were more common among mothers who were more educated.

Missing data

Among children with complete exposure data in the first trimester, outcome data were missing for 53.1% of children (n = 5417) at age 14 years, and for 59.7% (n = 6091) at age 16 years. Children who were male, with mothers who had lower education, were younger, smoked in pregnancy, had a history of depression, as well as those whose father worked in a manual profession were more likely to have missing outcome data at both time points. Exposure to prenatal infections was also associated with missing outcome data (see online Supplementary Table S2).

Prevalence of disordered eating behaviours in adolescence

Of those with available exposure data, at age 14 years and age 16 years, 380 (7.9%) and 647 (12.8%) adolescents reported disordered eating behaviours, respectively. At age 14 years, 136 (2.8%) of the sample and at 16 years 241 adolescents (4.8%) reported that these behaviours occurred weekly. More detailed information on the prevalence of individual disordered eating behaviours is provided in online Supplementary Table S3.

Exposure to prenatal infections and outcomes at 14 years of age

As shown in Table 2, children have a 7.4% estimated baseline risk in the absence of exposure [i.e. E(Y 0)] of reporting any disordered eating [95% confidence intervals (CI) 6.4–8.3]; a 5.0% risk of reporting monthly behaviours (95% CI 4.2–5.9); and a 2.5% risk of reporting weekly behaviours (95% CI 1.9–3.2).

Table 2. Average causal effects of exposure to infections in pregnancy and study outcomes

Samples based on participants with complete exposure and outcome available and thus vary across models.

Confounders accounted for in ACE estimation: maternal: age, education, lifetime history of diabetes and depression, pre-pregnancy BMI, smoking in the first trimester; and paternal profession.

a For disordered eating, these figures represent risk differences (we report these as proportions in the results section). For weight and shape concerns, these are mean differences.

b This potential outcome mean (POM) figure refers to the baseline risk of the outcome among the unexposed and, as such, should be interpreted as a proportion (i.e. multiplied by 100), which is how we present these figures in the Results section of manuscript. For weight and shape concerns, this represents a mean score instead.

There was some evidence that prenatal infection was associated with frequent (weekly) disordered eating behaviours in both unadjusted (difference in observed prevalence 1.2%, 95% CI 0.2–2.2, p < 0.01) and confounder-adjusted analyses (RD 0.9%, 95% CI −0.1 to 1.9, p = 0.08). The latter is equivalent to a 36% increase in risk. Less frequent disordered eating behaviour (monthly) or any disordered eating was not associated with prenatal infection (Table 2).

Average weight and shape concerns score in the unexposed group was 2.3 (95% CI 2.2–2.3). The observed mean difference (MD) in scores in the exposed group was (MD 0.18, 95% CI 0.07–0.29, p < 0.01). This estimate decreased in the confounder-adjusted model (MD 0.16, 95% CI 0.05–0.26, p ⩽ 0.01) representing a 6.8% increase in total weight and shape concerns score in the exposed group.

Exposure to prenatal infections and outcomes at 16 years of age

Similar to what we observed at age 14 years, prenatal infection was associated with frequent (weekly) disordered eating behaviour at age 16 years, though evidence of an association was stronger at this age.

Table 2 shows that unexposed children have a 14.5% estimated baseline risk of reporting any disordered eating (95% CI 13.1–19.8); a 9.9% risk of reporting monthly behaviours (95% CI 8.7–11.1); and a 5.5% risk of reporting weekly behaviours (95% CI 4.6–6.5).

The observed difference in prevalence of any eating disorder behaviours was of 3.8% (unadjusted analysis; 95% CI 1.5–6.1). The causal RD for any disordered eating estimated accounting for the pre-defined confounders was smaller (RD 3.0%, 95% CI 0.7–5.3, p < 0.01). Prenatal infection was strongly associated with frequent (weekly) disordered eating behaviours in both unadjusted (difference in observed prevalence 2.9%, 95% CI 1.2– 4.5, p < 0.01) and confounder-adjusted analyses (RD 2.3%, 95% CI 0.6–3.9, p < 0.01). The latter is equivalent to a 41.8% increase in risk. We did not find an association between prenatal infection and less frequent disordered eating behaviour (monthly) (Table 2).

Sensitivity and complete case analyses

Results of our analyses based on complete cases (see online Supplementary Table S4) were comparable to those from imputed models in terms of magnitude of the association, although here evidence of an association was stronger. When we included ‘other’ infections as part of our exposure (see online Supplementary Table S5), we also observed qualitatively similar associations, although there was weaker evidence of an association between prenatal infections and weekly disordered eating at 16 years. These findings were also comparable to those based on complete cases (see online Supplementary Table S6).

Discussion

In this study, we found that children who were exposed prenatally to infections had increased risk of reporting weekly disordered eating behaviours. Evidence for this association was stronger at age 16 years, possibly due to greater statistical power at this time point since the risk ratios (i.e. the proportion of increase in risk) were comparable at age 14 years (41.8% at age 16 years v. 36.0% at age 14 years, Table 2). Exposed children also had greater weight and shape concerns – a trans-diagnostic cognitive marker of eating disorders – at age 14 years.

Our findings of an increased risk of more frequent disordered eating behaviours in children exposed to prenatal infections are broadly in line with those of previous studies (Favaro et al., Reference Favaro, Tenconi, Ceschin, Zanetti, Bosello and Santonastaso2011; Lydholm et al., Reference Lydholm, Köhler-Forsberg, Nordentoft, Yolken, Mortensen, Petersen and Benros2018).

Strengths and limitations

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the association between exposure to prenatal infections and self-reported disordered eating in the general population. In this study, we relied on data from the ALSPAC dataset, a large birth cohort covering a wealth of biological and environmental information on children and their families. In building our analytical models – rooted in a counterfactual framework approach – we relied on pre-specified causal assumptions, outlined using DAGs, which allowed to establish the minimum sets of confounders necessary to calculate a causal effect (if our assumptions are correct and there is no residual confounding). Our exposure was prospectively collected at assessments that took place throughout pregnancy, thus minimising recall bias.

Data on disordered eating behaviours were self-reported by adolescents, which represents an advantage over parental report, as there is evidence that parents might miss key behaviours such as binge eating and purging (Swanson et al., Reference Swanson, Aloisio, Horton, Sonneville, Crosby, Eddy, Field and Micali2014). This is particularly important as adolescents who binge and purge are also often undetected in clinical settings, making general population studies key when investigating these behaviours. Our disordered eating definitions have not been validated against clinical diagnoses and thus their diagnostic validity could have been improved from combination with clinical interviews.

To study more ‘severe’ presentations, we created frequency cut-offs modelled after DSM-5 criteria for eating disorders. Previous studies have also found that these behaviours –whose prevalence is higher than that of diagnoses in the general population – are associated with substantial comorbidity (Micali et al., Reference Micali, Solmi, Horton, Crosby, Eddy, Calzo, Sonneville, Swanson and Field2015a; Solmi et al., Reference Solmi, Sonneville, Easter, Horton, Crosby, Rodriguez, Jarvelin, Field and Micali2015a; Micali et al., Reference Micali, Horton, Crosby, Swanson, Sonneville, Solmi, Calzo, Eddy and Field2017; Solmi et al., Reference Solmi, Melamed, Lewis and Kirkbride2018) making the study of their aetiology an important aim in itself. Furthermore, the use of trans-diagnostic definitions of behaviours and cognitions in general population samples has the advantage of capturing presentations that might index increased susceptibility to develop threshold diagnoses and reduce limitations (e.g. low power and selection bias) arising from the use of clinical diagnoses.

A number of limitations should also be acknowledged. First, infections were self-reported by the mother and this could result in some measurement error. In order to minimise this error and to increase statistical power, we pooled all infections together across the three trimesters. Because our outcome is relatively uncommon, we also lacked statistical power to investigate associations by type of infection, particularly as some of these were rather rare due to widespread immunisation (e.g. rubella), or by timing of infections. We were also unable to investigate the presence of dose–response associations (e.g. number of infections in pregnancy) as for some women some of the questionnaires might have referred to the same time period, and hence, we could not exclude double counting.

Literature suggests an association between Type I diabetes and eating disorders (Raevuori et al., Reference Raevuori, Haukka, Vaarala, Suvisaari, Gissler, Grainger, Linna and Suokas2014; Zerwas et al., Reference Zerwas, Larsen, Petersen, Thornton, Quaranta, Koch, Pisetsky, Mortensen and Bulik2017; Hedman et al., Reference Hedman, Breithaupt, Hübel, Thornton, Tillander, Norring, Birgegård, Larsson, Ludvigsson, Sävendahl, Almqvist and Bulik2018). Although we adjusted our analyses for maternal lifetime history of diabetes, we did not have information on type of diabetes, and hence, this could have resulted in some measurement error.

ALSPAC is affected by high levels of attrition. We used multiple imputation to impute missing confounder data; however, the amount of missingness (>50%) in the outcome was too large for confident imputation in the absence of established literature from longitudinal studies of antecedents of eating disorders. Indeed, imputing outcomes under these circumstances is discouraged (White et al., Reference White, Royston and Wood2011). We could not control our analyses for a number of hypothesised confounders, such as genetic predisposition, family environment and parental psychopathology relevant to exposure and outcome. Future investigations should aim to triangulate these findings using study designs which can better account for these largely unobservable factors.

Interpretation of findings

Several hypotheses have been advanced to explain the observed excess of mental health problems in individuals exposed to infections prenatally. One possibility is that exposure to maternal immune activation could directly affect foetal neurodevelopment. Animal studies have reported that, in mice, exposure to prenatal infection results in reduced sociability (Malkova et al., Reference Malkova, Yu, Hsiao, Moore and Patterson2012; Weber-Stadlbauer et al., Reference Weber-Stadlbauer, Richetto, Labouesse, Bohacek, Mansuy and Meyer2017) and increased repetitive behaviours (Malkova et al., Reference Malkova, Yu, Hsiao, Moore and Patterson2012). Deficits in social communication, elevated autistic traits and social anxiety are common in individuals with eating disorders (Christensen et al., Reference Christensen, Bentz, Clemmensen, Strandberg-Larsen and Olsen2019). Another hypothesis is that exposure to prenatal infection could predispose the offspring to autoimmune disorders. A number of studies have now shown that individuals with autoimmune disorders have a greater risk of eating disorders (Raevuori et al., Reference Raevuori, Haukka, Vaarala, Suvisaari, Gissler, Grainger, Linna and Suokas2014; Zerwas et al., Reference Zerwas, Larsen, Petersen, Thornton, Quaranta, Koch, Pisetsky, Mortensen and Bulik2017; Hedman et al., Reference Hedman, Breithaupt, Hübel, Thornton, Tillander, Norring, Birgegård, Larsson, Ludvigsson, Sävendahl, Almqvist and Bulik2018). Finally, exposure to prenatal infections might be associated with alterations of the Hypothalamic Pituitary Adrenal (HPA) axis – involved in cortisol release and stress response – which might also be involved in the onset and maintenance of eating disorders (Lo Sauro et al., Reference Lo Sauro, Ravaldi, Cabras, Faravelli and Ricca2008). Future general population-based and experimental studies should attempt to test these potential mechanisms.

Our findings could also be explained by non-causal mechanisms, for instance residual confounding. Although we did not observe large attenuations in size or strength of the association once including indicators of socio-economic status (suggesting that the latter might not be an important confounder), we cannot exclude that these associations might be explained by residual genetic confounding or familial characteristics which we were not able to capture in this dataset. Future studies should attempt to control for these factors by using study designs that control for these unobserved factors and by triangulating different approaches.

In conclusion, we present novel findings of an association between exposure to prenatal infections and disordered eating presentations in adolescence. Our findings add to the existing evidence base linking prenatal exposure to infections to a range of psychopathological outcomes in the offspring, by showing that this effect might be relevant in the aetiology of eating disorders as well. Future studies should aim to investigate putative mechanisms and replicate these results in other settings and datasets possibly triangulating findings from different designs in order to strengthen causal inference.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719000795

Author ORCIDs

Francesca Solmi, 0000-0003-0219-9503

Acknowledgements

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses.

Financial support

Dr Francesca Solmi is supported by a Sir Henry Wellcome Fellowship funded by the Wellcome Trust (grant no. 209196/Z/17/Z). This research is also supported by the UCLH NIHR Biomedical Research Centre. Dr Khandaker acknowledges the grant support from the Wellcome Trust (201486/Z/16/Z), UK Medical Research Council (MC_PC_17213) and MQ: Transforming Mental Health (MQDS17/40). The UK Medical Research Council and Wellcome (grant ref: 102215/2/13/2) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. This publication is the work of the authors and all authors will serve as guarantors of the contents of this paper. A comprehensive list of grants funding is available on the ALSPAC website (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/external/documents/grant-acknowledgements.pdf). Outcome measures employed in this study was funded by the following grants: NIH (grant no. MH087786-01), NIHR (grant no. CS/01/2008/014). Prof Bulik acknowledges funding from the Swedish Research Council (VR Dnr: 538-2013-8864).

Conflict of interest

CMB reports: Shire (Scientific Advisory Board member) and Pearson (author, royalty recipient). FS, BLDS, GMK, CD and GL have no conflicts of interest to declare.

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5™), 5th edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Atladóttir, , Thorsen, P, Østergaard, L, Schendel, DE, Lemcke, S, Abdallah, M and Parner, ET (2010) Maternal infection requiring hospitalization during pregnancy and autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 40, 14231430.10.1007/s10803-010-1006-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benros, ME, Sørensen, HJ, Nielsen, PR, Nordentoft, M, Mortensen, PB and Petersen, L (2015) The association between infections and general cognitive ability in young men – a nationwide study. PLoS ONE 10, e0124005.10.1371/journal.pone.0124005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, A, Golding, J, Macleod, J, Lawlor, DA, Fraser, A, Henderson, J, Molloy, L, Ness, A, Ring, S, Davey Smith, G, Macdonald-Wallis, C, Tilling, K, Boyd, A, Golding, J, Davey Smith, G, Henderson, J, Macleod, J, Molloy, L, Ness, A, Ring, S, Nelson, SM and Lawlor, DA (2013) Cohort profile: the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. International Journal of Epidemiology 42, 97110.10.1093/ije/dys064CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brener, ND, Collins, JL, Kann, L, Warren, CW and Williams, BI (1995) Reliability of the Youth Risk Behavior Survey Questionnaire. American Journal of Epidemiology 141, 575580.10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a117473CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, AS and Derkits, EJ (2010) Prenatal infection and schizophrenia: a review of epidemiologic and translational studies. The American Journal of Psychiatry 167, 261280.10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09030361CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, AS, Begg, MD, Gravenstein, S, Schaefer, CA, Wyatt, RJ, Bresnahan, M, Babulas, VP and Susser, ES (2004) Serologic evidence of prenatal influenza in the etiology of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 61, 774780.10.1001/archpsyc.61.8.774CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buka, SL, Tsuang, MT, Torrey, EF, Klebanoff, Ma, Bernstein, D and Yolken, RH (2001) Maternal infections and subsequent psychosis among offspring. Archives of General Psychiatry 58, 10321037.10.1001/archpsyc.58.11.1032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canetta, S, Sourander, A, Surcel, HM, Hinkka-Yli-Salomäki, S, Leiviskä, J, Kellendonk, C, McKeague, IW and Brown, AS (2014) Elevated maternal C-reactive protein and increased risk of schizophrenia in a national birth cohort. American Journal of Psychiatry 17, 960968.10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13121579CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, SS, Bentz, M, Clemmensen, L, Strandberg-Larsen, K and Olsen, EM (2019) Disordered eating behaviours and autistic traits – Are there any associations in nonclinical populations? A systematic review. European Eating Disorders Review 27, 823.10.1002/erv.2627CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalton, B, Bartholdy, S, Robinson, L, Solmi, M, Ibrahim, MAA, Breen, G, Schmidt, U and Himmerich, H (2018) A meta-analysis of cytokine concentrations in eating disorders. Journal of Psychiatric Research 103, 252264.10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.06.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dancyger, IF and Garfinkel, PE (1995) The relationship of partial syndrome eating disorders to anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Psychological Medicine 25, 1019.10.1017/S003329170003751XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Favaro, A, Tenconi, E, Ceschin, L, Zanetti, T, Bosello, R and Santonastaso, P (2011) In utero exposure to virus infections and the risk of developing anorexia nervosa. Psychological Medicine 41, 21932199.10.1017/S0033291710002655CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, AE, Taylor, CB, Celio, A and Colditz, GA (2004) Comparison of self-report to interview assessment of bulimic behaviors among preadolescent and adolescent girls and boys. The International Journal of Eating Disorders 35, 8692.10.1002/eat.10220CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flinkkilä, E, Keski-Rahkonen, A, Marttunen, M and Raevuori, A (2016) Prenatal inflammation, infections and mental disorders. Psychopathology 49, 317333.10.1159/000448054CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraser, A, Macdonald-Wallis, C, Tilling, K, Boyd, A, Golding, J, Davey Smith, G, Henderson, J, Macleod, J, Molloy, L, Ness, A, Ring, S, Nelson, SM and Lawlor, DA (2013) Cohort profile: the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. International Journal of Epidemiology 42, 97110.10.1093/ije/dys066CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gesi, C, Carmassi, C, Luciano, M, Bossini, L, Ricca, V, Fagiolini, A, Maj, M and Dell'Osso, L (2017) Autistic traits in patients with anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa or binge eating disorder: a pilot study. European Psychiatry 41, S100.10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.01.310CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillberg, IC, Rastam, M, Wentz, E, Gillberg, C, Carina Gillberg, I, Råstam, M, Wentz, E and Gillberg, C (2007) Cognitive and executive functions in anorexia nervosa ten years after onset of eating disorder. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 29, 170178.10.1080/13803390600584632CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldsmith, DR, Rapaport, MH and Miller, BJ (2016) A meta-analysis of blood cytokine network alterations in psychiatric patients: comparisons between schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and depression. Molecular Psychiatry 21, 16961709.10.1038/mp.2016.3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenland, S, Pearl, J and Robins, JM (1999) Causal diagrams for epidemiologic research. Epidemiology 10, 3748.10.1097/00001648-199901000-00008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedman, A, Breithaupt, L, Hübel, C, Thornton, LM, Tillander, A, Norring, C, Birgegård, A, Larsson, H, Ludvigsson, JF, Sävendahl, L, Almqvist, C and Bulik, CM (2018) Bidirectional relationship between eating disorders and autoimmune diseases. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12958. [Epub ahead of print]Google ScholarPubMed
Hernan, MA and Robins, JM (2019) Causal Inference. Boca Raton: Chapman & Hall/CRC, forthcoming.Google Scholar
Holland, AJ, Hall, A, Murray, R, Russell, GF and Crisp, AH (1984) Anorexia nervosa: a study of 34 twin pairs and one set of triplets. British Journal of Psychiatry 145, 414419.10.1192/bjp.145.4.414CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hornig, M, Bresnahan, MA, Che, X, Schultz, AF, Ukaigwe, JE, Eddy, ML, Hirtz, D, Gunnes, N, Lie, KK, Magnus, P, Mjaaland, S, Reichborn-Kjennerud, T, Schjølberg, S, Øyen, AS, Levin, B, Susser, ES, Stoltenberg, C and Lipkin, WI (2018) Prenatal fever and autism risk. Molecular Psychiatry 23, 759766.10.1038/mp.2017.119CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hudson, JI, Hiripi, E, Pope, HG and Kessler, RC (2007) The prevalence and correlates of eating disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Biological Psychiatry 61, 348358.10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khandaker, GM, Zimbron, J, Lewis, G and Jones, PB (2013) Prenatal maternal infection, neurodevelopment and adult schizophrenia: a systematic review of population-based studies. Psychological Medicine 43, 239257.10.1017/S0033291712000736CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khandaker, GM, Dalman, C, Kappelmann, N, Stochl, J, Dal, H, Kosidou, K, Jones, PB and Karlsson, H (2018) Association of childhood infection with IQ and adult nonaffective psychosis in Swedish men a population-based longitudinal cohort and co-relative study. JAMA Psychiatry 75, 356362.10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.4491CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koyanagi, A, Stickley, A and Haro, JM (2016) Psychotic-like experiences and disordered eating in the English general population. Psychiatry Research 241, 2634.10.1016/j.psychres.2016.04.045CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, K, Lloyd, S, Khondoker, M, Simic, M, Treasure, J and Tchanturia, K (2015) Do children and adolescents with anorexia nervosa display an inefficient cognitive processing style? PLoS ONE 10, e0131724.10.1371/journal.pone.0131724CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, BK, Magnusson, C, Gardner, RM, Blomström, Å, Newschaffer, CJ, Burstyn, I, Karlsson, H and Dalman, C (2015) Maternal hospitalization with infection during pregnancy and risk of autism spectrum disorders. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 44, 100105.10.1016/j.bbi.2014.09.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lo Sauro, C, Ravaldi, C, Cabras, PL, Faravelli, C and Ricca, V (2008) Stress, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and eating disorders. Neuropsychobiology 57, 95115.10.1159/000138912CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lydholm, CN, Köhler-Forsberg, O, Nordentoft, M, Yolken, RH, Mortensen, PB, Petersen, L and Benros, ME (2018) Parental infections before, during and after pregnancy as risk factors for mental disorders in childhood and adolescence – a nationwide Danish study. Biological Psychiatry 85, 317325.10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.09.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malkova, NV, Yu, CZ, Hsiao, EY, Moore, MJ and Patterson, PH (2012) Maternal immune activation yields offspring displaying mouse versions of the three core symptoms of autism. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 26, 607616.10.1016/j.bbi.2012.01.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Micali, N, Solmi, F, Horton, NJ, Crosby, RD, Eddy, KT, Calzo, JP, Sonneville, KR, Swanson, SA and Field, AE (2015 a) Adolescent eating disorders predict psychiatric, high-risk behaviors and weight outcomes in young adulthood. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 54, 652659.e1.10.1016/j.jaac.2015.05.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Micali, N, De Stavola, B, Ploubidis, G, Simonoff, E, Treasure, J and Field, AE (2015b) Adolescent eating disorder behaviours and cognitions: gender-specific effects of child, maternal and family risk factors. The British Journal of Psychiatry 207, 320327.10.1192/bjp.bp.114.152371CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Micali, N, Horton, NJ, Crosby, RD, Swanson, SA, Sonneville, KR, Solmi, F, Calzo, JP, Eddy, KT and Field, AE (2017) Eating disorder behaviours amongst adolescents: investigating classification, persistence and prospective associations with adverse outcomes using latent class models. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 26, 231240.10.1007/s00787-016-0877-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, JL, Vaillancourt, T and Hanna, SE (2009) The measurement of ‘eating-disorder-thoughts’ and ‘eating-disorder-behaviors’: implications for assessment and detection of eating disorders in epidemiological studies. Eating Behaviors 10, 8996.10.1016/j.eatbeh.2009.02.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mortensen, PB, Nørgaard-Pedersen, B, Waltoft, BL, Sørensen, TL, Hougaard, D, Torrey, EF and Yolken, RH (2007) Toxoplasma gondii as a risk factor for early-onset schizophrenia: analysis of filter paper blood samples obtained at birth. Biological Psychiatry 61, 688693.10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neumark-Sztainer, D, Wall, M, Haines, J, Story, M and Eisenberg, ME (2007) Why does dieting predict weight gain in adolescents? Findings from Project EAT-II: a 5-year longitudinal study. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 107, 448455.10.1016/j.jada.2006.12.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parboosing, R, Bao, Y, Shen, L, Schaefer, CA, Brown, AS (2013) Gestational influenza and bipolar disorder in adult offspring. JAMA Psychiatry 70, 677.10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.896CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raevuori, A, Haukka, J, Vaarala, O, Suvisaari, JM, Gissler, M, Grainger, M, Linna, MS, Suokas, JT (2014) The increased risk for autoimmune diseases in patients with eating disorders. PLoS ONE 9, e104845.10.1371/journal.pone.0104845CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shisslak, CM, Renger, R, Sharpe, T, Crago, M, McKnight, KM, Gray, N, Bryson, S, Estes, LS, Parnaby, OG, Killen, J and Taylor, CB (1999) Development and evaluation of the McKnight Risk Factor Survey for assessing potential risk and protective factors for disordered eating in preadolescent and adolescent girls. The International Journal of Eating Disorders 25, 195214.10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199903)25:2<195::AID-EAT9>3.0.CO;2-B3.0.CO;2-B>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simanek, AM and Meier, HCS (2015) Association between prenatal exposure to maternal infection and offspring mood disorders: a review of the literature. Current Problems in Pediatric and Adolescent Health Care 45, 325364.10.1016/j.cppeds.2015.06.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smink, FRE, van Hoeken, D and Hoek, HW (2013) Epidemiology, course, and outcome of eating disorders. Current Opinion in Psychiatry 26, 543548.10.1097/YCO.0b013e328365a24fCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, SK, Trevena, L, Simpson, JM, Barratt, A, Nutbeam, D and McCaffery, KJ (2010) A decision aid to support informed choices about bowel cancer screening among adults with low education: randomised controlled trial. British Medical Journal 341, c5370.10.1136/bmj.c5370CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solmi, F, Sonneville, KR, Easter, A, Horton, N, Crosby, R, Rodriguez, AR, Jarvelin, M-R, Field, AE and Micali, N (2015 a) Purging behaviours at age 16: prevalence in three community-based international cohorts. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 56, 8796.10.1111/jcpp.12283CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solmi, M, Veronese, N, Favaro, A, Santonastaso, P, Manzato, E, Sergi, G and Correll, CU (2015 b) Inflammatory cytokines and anorexia nervosa: a meta-analysis of cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. Psychoneuroendocrinology 51, 237252.10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.09.031CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solmi, F, Melamed, D, Lewis, G and Kirkbride, JB (2018) Longitudinal associations between psychotic experiences and disordered eating behaviours in adolescence: a UK population-based study. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health 2, 591599.10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30180-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sonneville, KR, Calzo, JP, Horton, NJ, Field, AE, Crosby, RD, Solmi, F and Micali, N (2015) Childhood hyperactivity/inattention and eating disturbances predict binge eating in adolescence. Psychological Medicine 45, 25112520.10.1017/S0033291715000148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
StataCorp (2017) Stata Statistical Software: Release 15. College Station, TX: StataCorp LLC.Google Scholar
Stata Press (2017) Stata Treatment-Effects Reference Manual: Potential Outcomes/Counterfactual Outcomes. Release 15. College Station, TX: Stata Press.Google Scholar
Stice, E (2016) Interactive and Mediational Etiologic Models of Eating Disorder Onset: Evidence from Prospective Studies. SSRN.10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-093317CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, SA, Aloisio, KM, Horton, NJ, Sonneville, KR, Crosby, RD, Eddy, KT, Field, AE and Micali, N (2014) Assessing eating disorder symptoms in adolescence: is there a role for multiple informants? The International Journal of Eating Disorders 47, 475482.10.1002/eat.22250CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Textor, J, Hardt, J and Knüppel, S (2011) DAGitty. Epidemiology 22, 745.10.1097/EDE.0b013e318225c2beCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tylka, TL and Subich, LM (1999) Exploring the construct validity of the eating disorder continuum. Journal of Counseling Psychology 46, 268276.10.1037/0022-0167.46.2.268CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Upthegrove, R, Manzanares-Teson, N and Barnes, NM (2014) Cytokine function in medication-naive first episode psychosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research 155, 101108.10.1016/j.schres.2014.03.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weber-Stadlbauer, U, Richetto, J, Labouesse, MA, Bohacek, J, Mansuy, IM and Meyer, U (2017) Transgenerational transmission and modification of pathological traits induced by prenatal immune activation. Molecular Psychiatry 22, 102112.10.1038/mp.2016.41CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, IR, Royston, P and Wood, AM (2011) Multiple imputation using chained equations: issues and guidance for practice. Statistics in Medicine 30, 377399.10.1002/sim.4067CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xiao, J, Buka, SL, Cannon, TD, Suzuki, Y, Viscidi, RP, Torrey, EF and Yolken, RH (2009) Serological pattern consistent with infection with type I Toxoplasma gondii in mothers and risk of psychosis among adult offspring. Microbes and Infection/Institut Pasteur 11, 10111018.10.1016/j.micinf.2009.07.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zammit, S, Odd, D, Horwood, J, Thompson, A, Thomas, K, Menezes, P, Gunnell, D, Hollis, C, Wolke, D, Lewis, G and Harrison, G (2009) Investigating whether adverse prenatal and perinatal events are associated with non-clinical psychotic symptoms at age 12 years in the ALSPAC birth cohort. Psychological Medicine 39, 1457.10.1017/S0033291708005126CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zerwas, S, Larsen, JT, Petersen, L, Thornton, LM, Quaranta, M, Koch, SV, Pisetsky, D, Mortensen, PB and Bulik, CM (2017) Eating disorders, autoimmune, and autoinflammatory disease. Pediatrics 140, e20162089.10.1542/peds.2016-2089CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Direct Acyclic Graphs describing the causal assumptions for our analytical models.

Figure 1

Table 1. Sample characteristics and their distributions among exposed children (complete exposure and outcome at age 14 years, n = 4785)

Figure 2

Table 2. Average causal effects of exposure to infections in pregnancy and study outcomes

Supplementary material: File

Solmi et al. supplementary material

Solmi et al. supplementary material 1

Download Solmi et al. supplementary material(File)
File 45.6 KB
Supplementary material: File

Solmi et al. supplementary material

Solmi et al. supplementary material 2

Download Solmi et al. supplementary material(File)
File 46.2 KB