Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T17:30:03.788Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Association between common early-childhood infection and subsequent depressive symptoms and psychotic experiences in adolescence: a population-based longitudinal birth cohort study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 November 2020

Anna B. Chaplin*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Peter B. Jones
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, UK
Golam M. Khandaker
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Anna B. Chaplin, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Childhood infections are associated with adult psychosis and depression, but studies of psychotic experiences (PEs) and depressive symptoms in childhood, adolescence, and early-adulthood are scarce. Previous studies have typically examined severe infections, but studies of common infections are also scarce.

Methods

Using data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) birth cohort, we examined associations of the number of infections in childhood from age 1.5 to 7.5 years with depressive symptom scores at age 10, 13, 14, 17, 18, and 19 years, and with PEs at 12 and 18 years. We performed additional analysis using infection burden (‘low’ = 0–4 infections, ‘medium’ = 5–6, ‘high’ = 7–9, or ‘very high’ = 10–22 infections) as the exposure.

Results

The risk set comprised 11 786 individuals with childhood infection data. Number of childhood infections was associated with depressive symptoms from age 10 (adjusted beta = 0.14; standard error (s.e.) = 0.04; p = <0.01) to 17 years (adjusted beta = 0.17; s.e. = 0.08; p = 0.04), and with PEs at age 12 (suspected/definite PEs: adjusted odds ratio (OR) = 1.18; 95% confidence interval (CI) = 1.09–1.27). These effect sizes were larger when the exposure was defined as very high infection burden (depressive symptoms age 17: adjusted beta = 0.79; s.e. = 0.29; p = 0.01; suspected/definite PEs at age 12: adjusted OR = 1.60; 95% CI = 1.25–2.05). Childhood infections were not associated with depressive/psychotic outcomes at age 18 or 19.

Conclusions

Common early-childhood infections are associated with depressive symptoms up to mid-adolescence and with PEs subsequently in childhood, but not with these outcomes in early-adulthood. These findings require replication including larger samples with outcomes in adulthood.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Early-life infections are associated with an increased risk of serious mental health disorders in adulthood. There is an extensive literature linking prenatal maternal and childhood infections with schizophrenia and related psychotic disorders, which are associated with both central nervous system (CNS) and non-CNS infections (Benros et al., Reference Benros, Nielsen, Nordentoft, Eaton, Dalton and Mortensen2011; Brown & Derkits, Reference Brown and Derkits2010; Dalman et al., Reference Dalman, Allebeck, Gunnell, Harrison, Kristensson, Lewis and Karlsson2008; Khandaker et al., Reference Khandaker, Dalman, Kappelmann, Stochl, Dal, Kosidou and Karlsson2018; Khandaker, Zimbron, Dalman, Lewis, & Jones, Reference Khandaker, Zimbron, Dalman, Lewis and Jones2012; Khandaker, Zimbron, Lewis, & Jones, Reference Khandaker, Zimbron, Lewis and Jones2013). Early-life exposure to infection may result in activation of an acute inflammatory response potentially affecting neurodevelopment (Boulanger & Shatz, Reference Boulanger and Shatz2004). Acute inflammation causes reductions in neuroplasticity, an important process for brain functioning, which may lead to neuropsychiatric outcomes later on (De Picker, Morrens, Chance, & Boche, Reference De Picker, Morrens, Chance and Boche2017). Compared with psychosis, longitudinal studies of early-life infection and depression are relatively rare, though inflammation is increasingly thought to play a role in the pathogenesis of depression (Dantzer, O'Connor, Freund, Johnson, & Kelley, Reference Dantzer, O'Connor, Freund, Johnson and Kelley2008; Khandaker, Dantzer, & Jones, Reference Khandaker, Dantzer and Jones2017; Miller, Maletic, & Raison, Reference Miller, Maletic and Raison2009). Most of the existing longitudinal studies of early-life infection have focused on psychosis and depression in adults (Bechter et al., Reference Bechter, Reiber, Herzog, Fuchs, Tumani and Maxeiner2010; Benros et al., Reference Benros, Nielsen, Nordentoft, Eaton, Dalton and Mortensen2011; Eisenberger et al., Reference Eisenberger, Berkman, Inagaki, Rameson, Mashal and Irwin2010; Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Brydon, Walker, Gray, Steptoe and Critchley2009; Laske et al., Reference Laske, Zank, Klein, Stransky, Batra, Buchkremer and Schott2008; Meyer, Schwarz, & Müller, Reference Meyer, Schwarz and Müller2011). Longitudinal studies of depressive and psychotic experiences (PEs) during childhood/adolescence are scarce (Goodwin, Reference Goodwin2011). Childhood/adolescent depressive symptoms are associated with adult depression (Copeland, Shanahan, Costello, & Angold, Reference Copeland, Shanahan, Costello and Angold2009; Dunn & Goodyer, Reference Dunn and Goodyer2006; Fombonne, Wostear, Cooper, Harrington, & Rutter, Reference Fombonne, Wostear, Cooper, Harrington and Rutter2001; Lewinsohn, Rohde, Seeley, Klein, & Gotlib, Reference Lewinsohn, Rohde, Seeley, Klein and Gotlib2000). PEs during childhood/early-adolescence may be part of typical development and are transient for most individuals. However, population-based longitudinal studies suggest association between early-life PEs with the risk of psychotic disorders subsequently in adulthood (Poulton et al., Reference Poulton, Caspi, Moffitt, Cannon, Murray and Harrington2000; Zammit et al., Reference Zammit, Kounali, Cannon, David, Gunnell, Heron and Lewis2013), and with risk factors for schizophrenia including impaired neurodevelopment (Cannon et al., Reference Cannon, Caspi, Moffitt, Harrington, Taylor, Murray and Poulton2002; Kelleher & Cannon, Reference Kelleher and Cannon2011). PEs are also associated with adolescent psychiatric multi-morbidity (Kelleher et al., Reference Kelleher, Keeley, Corcoran, Lynch, Fitzpatrick, Devlin and Cannon2012), and with other psychiatric disorders in adulthood (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Caspi, Poulton, Meier, Houts, Harrington and Moffitt2013). Therefore, studying adolescent PEs and depressive symptoms may offer insights into the development of adult psychotic and mood disorders.

Some previous longitudinal studies have considered the issue of timing, i.e. whether early-life infections are associated with psychotic outcomes closer to the time of exposure or subsequently after several years (Benros et al., Reference Benros, Nielsen, Nordentoft, Eaton, Dalton and Mortensen2011; Nielsen, Benros, & Mortensen, Reference Nielsen, Benros and Mortensen2014), but such studies of depression are scarce. Moreover, previous studies have typically examined effects of severe infections (Benros et al., Reference Benros, Nielsen, Nordentoft, Eaton, Dalton and Mortensen2011, Reference Benros, Waltoft, Nordentoft, Østergaard, Eaton, Krogh and Mortensen2013), but studies of common childhood infections and subsequent depressive/psychotic outcomes, or studies of the number of childhood infection and subsequent depression are scarce. Prospective cohort studies with repeated measures of depressive symptoms and PEs over a long period are required to address these issues, but such studies are relatively rare.

Infections are common during childhood, but some children are disproportionately prone to high infection burden possibly due to genetic and environmental factors. While childhood infection severity, duration, and related hospitalization have been linked with depression and psychosis in adulthood (Benros et al., Reference Benros, Nielsen, Nordentoft, Eaton, Dalton and Mortensen2011, Reference Benros, Waltoft, Nordentoft, Østergaard, Eaton, Krogh and Mortensen2013; Nielsen et al., Reference Nielsen, Benros and Mortensen2014), it is unclear whether the degree of childhood infection burden is associated with psychotic/depressive outcomes in childhood/adolescence. A high number of childhood infections may be a risk factor for psychiatric disorders, as a dose-response association between increasing number of childhood infection and adult mood or psychotic disorders has been reported (Benros et al., Reference Benros, Nielsen, Nordentoft, Eaton, Dalton and Mortensen2011, Reference Benros, Waltoft, Nordentoft, Østergaard, Eaton, Krogh and Mortensen2013). Early-life infections could also be a marker for shared genetic and environmental risk factors for infection and major psychiatric disorders (Nielsen, Laursen, & Mortensen, Reference Nielsen, Laursen and Mortensen2013). It is possible that inflammatory immune response during a critical developmental window is detrimental for brain development/function (Cordeiro, Tsimis, & Burd, Reference Cordeiro, Tsimis and Burd2015). A high burden of common childhood infections may also reflect underlying familial factors predisposing to infection, such as socioeconomic status, living conditions, and genetic factors (Nielsen et al., Reference Nielsen, Laursen and Mortensen2013). Therefore, studies investigating the effects of infection burden are required.

Using data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) (Boyd et al., Reference Boyd, Golding, Macleod, Lawlor, Fraser, Henderson and Davey Smith2013), a general population-representative prospective British birth cohort, we have investigated the longitudinal associations of childhood infections from age 1.5 to 7.5 years old with depressive symptoms measured six times from age 10 to 19 years and with PEs at age 12 and 18 years. We examined not only the number of childhood infections as exposure, but also the effect of infection burden, grouped as low, medium, high or very high. We hypothesized that a higher overall number of infections and very high infection burden would be associated with higher risks for depressive symptoms and PEs subsequently up to early-adulthood.

Methods

Description of cohort and sample

ALSPAC is a general population-based birth cohort in the former Avon County in the South West region of England. Initially, 14 541 pregnant women resident in the study catchment areas and with expected delivery dates between 1 April 1991 and 31 December 1992 were recruited into the cohort. Detailed information about the ALSPAC cohort can be found on the study website (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac), and the sample characteristics and methodology have been previously described (Boyd et al., Reference Boyd, Golding, Macleod, Lawlor, Fraser, Henderson and Davey Smith2013; Fraser et al., Reference Fraser, Macdonald-Wallis, Tilling, Boyd, Golding, Davey Smith and Lawlor2013). For information on all available ALSPAC data, a fully searchable data dictionary is also available (http://www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/our-data).

The risk set for this study comprised 11 786 individuals with data on infections during childhood. Out of the risk set, the number of individuals with depressive symptoms data decreased from 6685 at age 10 to 3101 at age 19 (online Supplementary Fig. 1). From the risk set, PEs data were available for 6176 individuals at age 12 and for 4253 individuals at age 18 (online Supplementary Fig. 2). These samples formed the basis for the analyses presented.

Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees. Written informed consent was provided by all participants. No financial compensation was given.

Exposure measures

Childhood infections from age 1.5 to age 7.5 years

Approximately once per year when the child was aged between 1.5 and 7.5 years old, caregivers completed seven postal questionnaires about common childhood infections experienced by their child. These included German measles, measles, chickenpox, mumps, meningitis, cold sores, whooping cough, urinary infections, eye infections, ear infections, chest infections, tonsillitis/laryngitis, scarlet fever, influenza, cold and ‘other’.

Overall, 11 786 children had fully/partly completed infection questionnaires from the seven time-points. In the questionnaire about childhood infections, caregivers could tick the ‘Yes’ or ‘No’ boxes for infections that their child has had. Some people simply ticked the ‘Yes’ box to indicate that their child has had certain infections, but did not tick the ‘No’ box for infections their child did not have. In those instances, we coded the un-ticked boxes as ‘No’ that year rather than dropping the subject from analysis for missing data. However, if a parent had all boxes unticked, they were coded as missing, as we did not have any indication whether their child had any infection that year.

Childhood infection count was used as a standardized continuous variable. Infection was also used as a categorical variable representing the degree of infection burden: low (50th percentile and below; 0–4 infections), medium (51–75th percentile; 5–6 infections), high (76–90th percentile; 7–9 infections), and very high burden (above 90th percentile; 10–22 infections). These categories were based on a prior study and were chosen to capture the positive skew of childhood infection distribution (Mackinnon, Zammit, Lewis, Jones, & Khandaker, Reference Mackinnon, Zammit, Lewis, Jones and Khandaker2018).

Outcome measures

Depressive symptoms at age 10, 13, 14, 17, 18, and 19 years

Depressive symptoms were self-reported by the child/young person using the Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ) (Sharp, Goodyer, & Croudace, Reference Sharp, Goodyer and Croudace2006). Depressive symptoms were measured at age 10 (mean age in years = 10.6; standard deviation (s.d.) = 0.3), 13 (mean = 12.8; s.d. = 0.2), 14 (mean = 13.8; s.d. = 0.2), 17 (mean = 16.7; s.d. = 0.2), 18 (mean = 17.8; s.d. = 0.4), and 19 years (mean = 18.6; s.d. = 0.5). The SMFQ is a widely used, age-appropriate, and validated tool comprising 13 items that cover core symptoms of depression and anxiety experienced in the past 2 weeks. Each item is scored zero (not true), one (sometimes true) or two (true) giving a total score of 0–26. Depressive symptoms were used as a continuous variable.

PEs at age 12 and 18 years

PEs were identified using the face-to-face, semi-structured Psychosis-Like Symptom Interview (PLIKSi) (Horwood et al., Reference Horwood, Salvi, Thomas, Duffy, Gunnell, Hollis and Harrison2008; Zammit et al., Reference Zammit, Kounali, Cannon, David, Gunnell, Heron and Lewis2013) conducted by trained psychology graduates in assessment clinics. PEs were coded according to the definitions and rating rules for the Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry, Version 2.0. (World Health Organisation, 1994). PLIKSi at age 12 has ‘fair’ inter-rater (kappa = 0.75) and test-retest (kappa = 0.48) reliability (Horwood et al., Reference Horwood, Salvi, Thomas, Duffy, Gunnell, Hollis and Harrison2008). PLIKSi at age 18 has ‘good’ inter-rater (kappa = 0.83) and test-retest (kappa = 0.76) reliability (Zammit et al., Reference Zammit, Kounali, Cannon, David, Gunnell, Heron and Lewis2013).

PLIKSi covers the three main domains of positive PEs: hallucinations (visual and auditory), delusions (spied on, persecuted, thoughts read, reference, control, grandiosity, and other), and thought interference (insertion, withdrawal, and broadcasting). After cross-questioning, interviewers rated PEs as ‘not present’, ‘suspected’, or ‘definitely present’. Uncertain responses were always ‘rated down’, and symptoms were rated as definite only if a clear example could be provided. For suspected or definite PEs, interviewers also recorded the frequency, affect, effects on social/educational/occupational function, help-seeking, and attributions including fever, hypnopompic/hypnogogic state, or illicit drugs.

PEs measured at age 12 refer to experiences from the previous 6 months while PEs measured at age 18 refer to experiences since age 12. In line with previous papers originally describing PEs in the ALSPAC birth cohort, we used three increasingly strict definitions for this outcome, i.e. any PEs (suspected or definite PEs); definite PEs; and definite PEs without attribution. The number of participants meeting each of the outcome definitions are different (online Supplementary Fig. 2) and the risk of each outcome was analysed separately. The comparison group for each outcome included all the individuals who did not meet that specific definition for the outcome (Khandaker, Zammit, Lewis, & Jones, Reference Khandaker, Zammit, Lewis and Jones2014).

Confounders

Based on previous studies, we included sex, ethnicity, birth weight, maternal social status, and parental history of severe depression or schizophrenia as these are associated with exposure and/or outcome and so could be confounders (Abel et al., Reference Abel, Wicks, Susser, Dalman, Pedersen, Mortensen and Webb2010; Benros et al., Reference Benros, Waltoft, Nordentoft, Østergaard, Eaton, Krogh and Mortensen2013; Khandaker et al., Reference Khandaker, Zimbron, Dalman, Lewis and Jones2012; Khandaker, Stochl, Zammit, Lewis, & Jones, Reference Khandaker, Stochl, Zammit, Lewis and Jones2014). Sex (binary) and birth weight (grams, continuous) were assessed at birth. Ethnicity was recorded as White, Black African, Black Caribbean, Black Other, Bangladeshi, Chinese, Indian, Pakistani, and Other. We re-coded this variable as White and Other due to low counts for non-White ethnic groups. Maternal social status was originally documented using Office of National Statistics categories (Office for National Statistics, 2016) and re-coded as non-manual (I, II and IIIa) and manual (IIIb, IV and V). The armed forces were excluded as the sample size for this category was too small (n = 4). Mothers and their partners completed separate questionnaires at 12 weeks gestation in which they self-reported having severe depression or schizophrenia, which were used as binary variables. See Supplementary Material for how these binary variables were defined.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were carried out using R version 3.6.1. Regression analyses were performed before and after adjusting for potential confounders. All main analyses were based on the dataset after imputation of missing values for confounders.

Imputation of missing confounder variables

Regression analyses testing associations between exposure and outcomes were conducted after imputation of missing data for confounders (ethnicity, maternal social status, birth weight, parental schizophrenia, and parental severe depression) to increase the sample size. Sex had no missing data. The percentage of missing values across the relevant confounder variables varied between 1.5% and 35.0% (online Supplementary Table 1). We used the TestMCARNormality function to test whether data were missing completely at random (MCAR) (Jamshidian, Jalal, & Jansen, Reference Jamshidian, Jalal and Jansen2014). The hypothesis of MCAR was rejected at the 0.05 significance level. Using the missing_compare function, each variable returned significance (p < 0.05) with at least one other variable, suggesting that the data met the missing at random (MAR) assumption.

We used multiple imputations using fully conditional Markov chain Monte Carlo method for the above confounder variables plus auxiliary variables that were indicators of missingness. Exposure and outcome variables were also included. The selected auxiliary variables included: housing/living conditions, parental education/employment status, financial difficulties, life events, and maternal characteristics (age, body mass index, marital status, anxiety/post-natal depression, and smoking status).

We used the R package mice version 3.0 to create and analyse the multiply imputed datasets (van Buuren & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, Reference van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn2011). Since missing data were present in 19.9% of subjects, we used 20 imputations as recommended (White, Royston, & Wood, Reference White, Royston and Wood2011). The parameters of interest were estimated in each dataset separately and combined using Rubin's rules.

Associations of childhood infection with depressive symptoms and PEs

Linear regression was used to examine the association between the number of childhood infections/infection burden and depressive symptoms. Low infection burden (0–4 infections) was used as the reference group where infection burden was used as the exposure. Regression models were adjusted for sex, birth weight, maternal social status, ethnicity, and parental history of severe depression. Beta estimates and standard error (s.e.) from regression models are presented.

Logistic regression was used to calculate odds ratios (OR) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) for PEs at age 12 or age 18 associated with the number of childhood infections/infection burden. The low burden of infection (0–4 infections) was used as the reference group for the latter. Regression models were adjusted for sex, birth weight, maternal social status, ethnicity, and parental history of schizophrenia.

For all analyses, we used the maximum available sample for each outcome measure to increase statistical power. Holm-Bonferroni p value correction was performed to correct for multiple testing (online Supplementary Table 2) (Holm, Reference Holm1979).

Sensitivity analysis exploring impact of missing data

To explore the potential impact of missing data we repeated our main analyses based on the complete case-set, defined as participants with no missing data for exposure, outcome or confounder measures. We then carried out a number of comparisons between different samples. (1) We compared the risk set (i.e. participants with data on childhood infection) with the complete case-set for depressive symptoms (i.e. data on childhood infection, all confounders, and depressive symptoms at all follow-ups are available). We did the same comparison for PEs. (2) We compared the analytic sample for depressive symptoms at age 19 years (i.e. data on both childhood infection and depressive symptoms at age 19 available) with the missing sample (i.e. childhood infection data present but depressive symptoms data at age 19 missing). We performed the same comparison for PEs at age 18 separately.

Results

Baseline characteristics of the sample

The risk set (N = 11 786) was predominantly of white ethnicity (97.7%) (Table 1). The average number of childhood infections in the total sample was 4.6 (s.d. = 3.2), and the median was 4 (inter-quartile range = 2–6). The number of children with very high infection burden (⩾90th percentile; 10–22 infections) was 947 (8.0%).

Table 1. Characteristics of participants in the risk set (using maximum available sample and after imputation of confounders)

There was a general increase in depressive symptoms between age 10 and 19 years; mean SMFQ score at age 10 was 4.0 (s.d. = 3.5), and was 6.8 (s.d. = 5.9) at age 19 (Table 1). The percentage of participants with definite PEs was similar at age 12 (5.7%) and at age 18 (4.8%).

Association between number of childhood infections and depressive symptoms

Number of childhood infections from age 1.5 to age 7.5 years was associated with subsequent depressive symptoms at age 10 (N = 6685; beta = 0.14, s.e. = 0.04; p = <0.01), 13 (N = 6087; beta = 0.24; s.e. = 0.05; p = <0.001), and 14 years (N = 5504; beta = 0.23, s.e. = 0.06; p = <0.001) (Table 2). Evidence for these associations remained after adjusting for potential confounders. Childhood infections were not associated with depressive symptoms at age 17, 18, or 19 years.

Table 2. Beta estimate (s.e.) for the association between 1 s.d. increase in childhood infections and depressive symptoms from age 10 to 19 years

a Adjusted for sex, birth weight, maternal social status, ethnicity, and parental history of severe depression.

After correction for multiple testing, evidence remained for an association between number of childhood infections and depressive symptoms at age 10 (p = 0.02), 13 (p = <0.001) and 14 years (p = <0.01) (online Supplementary Table 2).

Association between number of childhood infections and PEs

Number of childhood infections was associated with subsequent PEs at age 12 years (N = 6176), including suspected/definite PEs (OR = 1.17, 95% CI = 1.09–1.26), definite PEs (OR = 1.16, 95% CI = 1.04–1.29), and definite PEs without attribution (OR = 1.17, 95% CI = 1.04–1.31) (Table 3). Evidence for these associations remained after adjusting for potential confounders. The number of childhood infections was not associated with PEs at age 18 years.

Table 3. Odds ratio (95% CI) for the association between 1 s.d. increase in childhood infections and psychotic experiences (PE) at age 12 and age 18 years

a Adjusted for sex, birth weight, maternal social status, ethnicity, and parental history of schizophrenia.

After correction for multiple testing, evidence remained for an association between number of childhood infections and suspected/definite PEs at age 12 (p = <0.001), but not with definite PEs (p = 0.12) or definite PEs without attribution at age 12 years (p = 0.12) (online Supplementary Table 2).

Association between childhood infection burden and depressive symptoms

Compared with low infection burden, very high infection burden was associated with depressive symptoms at age 10 (beta = 0.38; s.e. = 0.16; p = 0.01), 13 (beta = 0.76; s.e. = 0.17; p = <0.001), 14 (beta = 0.66; s.e. = 0.21; p = <0.01), and 17 years (beta = 0.68; s.e. = 0.30; p = 0.02) (Table 4). Evidence for these associations remained after adjusting for potential confounders. Very high infection burden was not associated with depressive symptoms at age 18 (beta = 0.33; s.e. = 0.29; p = 0.26) or 19 years (beta = 0.41; s.e. = 0.38; p = 0.27).

Table 4. Beta estimate (s.e.) for the association between childhood infection burden and depressive symptoms from age 10 to 19 years

a Adjusted for sex, birth weight, maternal social status, ethnicity, and parental history of severe depression.

After correction for multiple testing, evidence remained for the association between very high infection burden and depressive symptoms at age 13 (p = <0.001) and 14 years (p = 0.02) (online Supplementary Table 2). Please see Table 4 for risk associated with other categories of infection burden.

Association between childhood infection burden and PEs

Compared with low infection burden, very high infection burden was associated with suspected/definite PEs at age 12 years (OR = 1.59, 95% CI = 1.24–2.04). Evidence for this association remained after adjusting for potential confounders. Very high infection burden was not associated with PEs at age 18 (Table 5).

Table 5. Odds ratio (95% CI) for the association between childhood infection burden and psychotic experiences (PEs) at age 12 and age 18 years

a Adjusted for sex, birth weight, maternal social status, ethnicity and parental history of schizophrenia.

After correction for multiple testing, evidence remained for an association between very high infection burden and suspected/definite PEs at age 12 years (p = <0.01) (online Supplementary Table 2). Please see Table 5 for risk associated with other categories of infection burden.

Results based on complete case-set analyses and comparisons between analytic and missing samples

Results from the complete case-set for depressive symptoms (N = 1133) and that for PEs (N = 2495) were very similar to the main results reported above (online Supplementary Tables 3–6). See online Supplementary Tables 7 and 8 for comparison of the risk set (N = 11 786) with the complete case-set for depressive symptoms or PEs, respectively.

Comparison between the analytic sample for depressive symptoms (N = 3101) and the missing sample (N = 8685) showed that those included in analyses were more likely to be female (p = <0.001), white (p = 0.05), have a higher number of childhood infections (p = <0.001), and have lower depressive symptoms at age 10 and 18 years (p = <0.001) (online Supplementary Table 9). The analytic sample for depressive symptoms was less likely to have a mother of manual occupational status (p < 0.001) or have a parent with a history of severe depression (p < 0.001).

The analytic sample for PEs (N = 4253), compared with the missing sample (N = 7533), was more likely to be female (p = <0.001), not have a mother of manual occupational status (p = <0.001), and have a higher number of childhood infections (p = <0.001) (online Supplementary Table 10).

Discussion

Our findings suggest that common early-childhood infections, particularly a very high infection burden, are associated with (i) the risk of depressive symptoms subsequently up to the age of 17, and (ii) the risk of suspected/definite PEs subsequently at age 12. The associations of infection with depressive symptoms at age 13 and 14 years and with suspected/definite PEs at age 12 were robust and persisted after correction for multiple testing. Childhood infections were not associated with risk of depressive symptoms or PEs at age 18/19 years.

Strong links exist between child/adolescent and adult depression (Copeland et al., Reference Copeland, Shanahan, Costello and Angold2009; Dunn & Goodyer, Reference Dunn and Goodyer2006; Fombonne et al., Reference Fombonne, Wostear, Cooper, Harrington and Rutter2001; Lewinsohn et al., Reference Lewinsohn, Rohde, Seeley, Klein and Gotlib2000). Adolescents with recurrent depressive episodes are at particularly high risk of subsequent recurrent depression as adults (Lewinsohn et al., Reference Lewinsohn, Rohde, Seeley, Klein and Gotlib2000). Longitudinal studies suggest that recurrence of depression may be similar in clinic and community samples (Dunn & Goodyer, Reference Dunn and Goodyer2006). PEs in childhood or adolescence may also provide a valid method for studying the development of adult psychotic disorders (Kelleher & Cannon, Reference Kelleher and Cannon2011; Murray & Jones, Reference Murray and Jones2012). PEs in childhood may be transient but population-based studies suggest that PEs in the general population and those observed in psychotic disorders may exist on a continuum (van Os, Linscott, Myin-Germeys, Delespaul, & Krabbendam, Reference van Os, Linscott, Myin-Germeys, Delespaul and Krabbendam2009). Prospective birth cohort studies have reported that PEs in childhood are associated with an increased risk of psychotic disorders in adulthood (Zammit et al., Reference Zammit, Kounali, Cannon, David, Gunnell, Heron and Lewis2013). Common underlying mechanisms for PEs in healthy individuals and in schizophrenia have also been reported (Howes et al., Reference Howes, Shotbolt, Bloomfield, Daalman, Demjaha, Diederen and Sommer2013). Early-life infections could increase psychosis risk by affecting neurodevelopment, consistent with the neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia (Murray & Lewis, Reference Murray and Lewis1987; Weinberger, Reference Weinberger1987). In many cases, PEs in our sample are likely to be part of normal development while for other participants these symptoms may be more pathological (De Loore et al., Reference De Loore, Gunther, Drukker, Feron, Sabbe, Deboutte and Myin-Germeys2011; Rubio, Sanjuán, Flórez-Salamanca, & Cuesta, Reference Rubio, Sanjuán, Flórez-Salamanca and Cuesta2012). Childhood PEs are also familial and heritable and associated with multiple risk factors for schizophrenia (Khandaker, Zammit, et al., Reference Khandaker, Zammit, Lewis and Jones2014; Polanczyk et al., Reference Polanczyk, Moffitt, Arseneault, Cannon, Ambler, Keefe and Caspi2010; Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Harrison, Zammit, Lewis, Horwood, Heron and Gunnell2009; Zammit et al., Reference Zammit, Odd, Horwood, Thompson, Thomas, Menezes and Harrison2009). PEs and depressive symptoms in children and adolescents appear to be markers of mental distress and represent useful indicators for subsequent mental health problems.

In our sample, associations observed between childhood infections and PEs/depressive symptoms in childhood and adolescence did not persist for these outcomes in early-adulthood at age 18/19 years. There could be a number of explanations for this, including that childhood infections do not have a lasting effect on the risk for adult mental health disorders. However, findings from other long-term follow-up studies argue against this. For instance, a meta-analysis of childhood infections reported that children exposed to viral infections of the CNS were at higher risk of adult psychosis (Khandaker et al., Reference Khandaker, Zimbron, Dalman, Lewis and Jones2012). Longitudinal studies of childhood infection and subsequent non-affective psychosis found similar results (Dalman et al., Reference Dalman, Allebeck, Gunnell, Harrison, Kristensson, Lewis and Karlsson2008; Khandaker et al., Reference Khandaker, Dalman, Kappelmann, Stochl, Dal, Kosidou and Karlsson2018). Furthermore, population-based longitudinal studies suggest that childhood infections and autoimmune disease increase risk for adult schizophrenia and mood disorders in a dose-response fashion (Benros et al., Reference Benros, Nielsen, Nordentoft, Eaton, Dalton and Mortensen2011, Reference Benros, Waltoft, Nordentoft, Østergaard, Eaton, Krogh and Mortensen2013). Benros and colleagues reported that a history of hospitalization for infection increases the risk of mood disorders and schizophrenia by 62% and 60%, respectively (Benros et al., Reference Benros, Nielsen, Nordentoft, Eaton, Dalton and Mortensen2011, Reference Benros, Waltoft, Nordentoft, Østergaard, Eaton, Krogh and Mortensen2013). One explanation for the null findings could be attrition. At age 19 years, 86% of the sample with data on childhood infections were missing from follow-up for depressive symptoms and at age 18 years, 68% of the sample were missing from follow-up for PEs. Another explanation could be the choice of exposure and outcome used. For instance, previous studies used serious infections requiring hospitalization as exposure (Benros et al., Reference Benros, Nielsen, Nordentoft, Eaton, Dalton and Mortensen2011, Reference Benros, Waltoft, Nordentoft, Østergaard, Eaton, Krogh and Mortensen2013), and diagnosis of schizophrenia or depression as an outcome. We have used common childhood infections as exposure and depressive symptoms/PEs as outcomes. Nevertheless, we are still able to show that relatively common childhood infections are associated with risk for depressive symptoms and PEs in adolescence. The sample size for outcomes in early-adulthood was relatively small and there was attrition over time, a common issue for prospective studies. In future, studies with larger sample sizes for cases are required.

It is possible that childhood infections contribute indirectly via inflammatory mechanisms to the risk of mental health disorders. Inflammatory responses to infection, such as elevated cytokine levels and fever, may represent the mechanism through which risk for mental disorders is increased (Flinkkilä, Keski-Rahkonen, Marttunen, & Raevuori, Reference Flinkkilä, Keski-Rahkonen, Marttunen and Raevuori2016). ‘Sickness behaviour’ is common in infection and is present in some cases of depression. Sickness behaviour is triggered by proinflammatory cytokines in response to infectious agents and includes symptoms such as fatigue, anhedonia, concentration difficulties, social withdrawal, and appetite changes (Dantzer, Reference Dantzer2009; Dantzer et al., Reference Dantzer, O'Connor, Freund, Johnson and Kelley2008). Along with sickness behaviour, proinflammatory cytokines may induce depression in physically ill individuals with no history of mental health disorders. Depression may be therefore a maladaptive form of cytokine-induced sickness in some individuals (Dantzer, Reference Dantzer2009; Dantzer et al., Reference Dantzer, O'Connor, Freund, Johnson and Kelley2008). In addition, infections in early-life have been associated with adult schizophrenia via inflammation (Al-Haddad et al., Reference Al-Haddad, Jacobsson, Chabra, Modzelewska, Olson, Bernier and Sengpiel2019; Brown et al., 2000, Reference Brown, Cohen, Harkavy-Friedman, Babulas, Malaspina, Gorman and Susser2001, Reference Brown, Schaefer, Quesenberry, Liu, Babulas and Susser2005; Brown, Begg, et al., Reference Brown, Begg, Gravenstein, Schaefer, Wyatt, Bresnahan and Susser2004; Brown, Deicken, et al., Reference Brown, Deicken, Vinogradov, Kremen, Poole, Penner and Schaefer2009; Brown, Hooton, et al., Reference Brown, Hooton, Schaefer, Zhang, Petkova, Babulas and Susser2004; Brown, Vinogradov, et al., Reference Brown, Vinogradov, Kremen, Poole, Deicken, Penner and Schaefer2009; Buka, Cannon, Torrey, & Yolken, Reference Buka, Cannon, Torrey and Yolken2008; Buka, Tsuang, Torrey, Klebanoff, Bernstein, et al., Reference Buka, Tsuang, Torrey, Klebanoff, Bernstein and Yolken2001; Buka, Tsuang, Torrey, Klebanoff, Wagner, et al., Reference Buka, Tsuang, Torrey, Klebanoff, Wagner and Yolken2001; Khandaker et al., Reference Khandaker, Dalman, Kappelmann, Stochl, Dal, Kosidou and Karlsson2018; Reference Khandaker, Zimbron, Lewis and Jones2013; Mortensen et al., Reference Mortensen, Nørgaard-Pedersen, Waltoft, Sørensen, Hougaard, Torrey and Yolken2007, Reference Mortensen, Pedersen, Hougaard, Nørgaard-Petersen, Mors, Børglum and Yolken2010; Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Fineberg, Maxwell, Alloy, Zimmermann, Krigbaum and Ellman2017; Zammit et al., Reference Zammit, Allebeck, David, Dalman, Hemmingsson, Lundberg and Lewis2004). Infection-related inflammation may influence neurodevelopment resulting in heightened risk for schizophrenia (Benros, Mortensen, & Eaton, Reference Benros, Mortensen and Eaton2012). Inflammation represents a compelling link between infection and mental health disorders and requires further investigation.

The interaction of multiple genetic and environmental factors is likely to contribute to risk for psychosis or depression. Genetic susceptibility plays an important role in the risk of mental health disorders, but there is evidence to suggest that a significant proportion of cases may be preventable through modification of the environment (Benros, Eaton, & Mortensen, Reference Benros, Eaton and Mortensen2014; Uher, Reference Uher2014). We attempted to explore the effects of environmental factors by controlling for maternal social status and birth weight. Evidence for the association between infection and PEs/depressive symptoms attenuated but did not disappear, suggesting that these environmental factors partly explain these associations. Some genetic and environmental risk factors for psychosis and depression may be shared (Uher, Reference Uher2014). Childhood maltreatment, social disadvantage, and minority status are independently associated with both psychosis and depression (Uher, Reference Uher2014). By recognizing infection and other environmental factors that are associated with depression and psychosis, we may be able to identify at-risk individuals early and prevent the onset of symptoms.

The strengths of this study include longitudinal design and repeated measures of both depressive symptoms and PEs. A limitation is the method for collection of childhood infection data. The questionnaire containing questions on infections were completed by primary caregiver throughout childhood with a short period of recall (past 12 months), thus minimizing, though not completely eliminating, the risk of erroneous recall. The questions on infections also require parents to have a good understanding of infectious diseases and may be vulnerable to misreporting. For example, 8.2% of parents reported that their child had no infections during childhood; a somewhat unlikely scenario. Another limitation is attrition; the number of completed mental health assessments decreases over time. This may be indicative of selective attrition of mental health cases since mental health problems have been associated with non-response and attrition (Dupuis, Strippoli, Gholam-Rezaee, Preisig, & Vandeleur, Reference Dupuis, Strippoli, Gholam-Rezaee, Preisig and Vandeleur2019). Attrition and subsequent smaller sample size could result in underestimation of the true effect of infection on mental health outcomes. Another possible explanation for the lack of association with PEs/depressive symptoms in early-adulthood could be that the relative contribution of infection to mental health becomes negligible over time due to neuroplasticity and brain development. Finally, statistical power was an issue in some of our PE analyses, resulting in wide CI.

Childhood infections are inevitable, but the adverse outcomes of such physical health problems may go beyond physical health later in life. Here we present evidence that childhood infections, particularly a very high infection burden, can negatively impact mental health well into adolescence. Future work is needed (1) to replicate the observed associations between common childhood infections and mental health outcomes during adolescence; (2) to examine whether common childhood infections have an effect on depressive symptoms and PEs in adulthood; and (3) to elucidate potential mechanisms for these associations.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720004080.

Acknowledgements

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, and nurses. The UK Medical Research Council and Wellcome (Grant ref: 102215/2/13/2) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. The publication is the work of the authors who will serve as guarantors for the contents of this paper. A comprehensive list of grants funding is available on the ALSPAC website (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/external/documents/grant-acknowledgements.pdf). This report of research supported in part by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Applied Research Collaboration (ARC) East of England expresses the views of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR, the Department of Health and Social Care or other funding bodies. ABC is supported by the National Institute of Health Research CLAHRC RCF (MRR73-1795-00000), NIHR ARC East of England, the MQ: Transforming Mental Health (Data Science Award; grant code: MQDS17/40) and Wolfson College. GMK acknowledges funding support from the Wellcome Trust (Intermediate Clinical Fellowship; grant code: 201486/Z/16/Z), MQ as above, and the Medical Research Council (MICA: Mental Health Data Pathfinder; grant code: MC_PC_17213). PBJ acknowledges funding from MQ and the MRC, as above, NIHR PGfAR 0616-20003, and from the NIHR ARC East of England.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Abel, K. M., Wicks, S., Susser, E. S., Dalman, C., Pedersen, M. G., Mortensen, P. B., & Webb, R. T. (2010). Birth weight, schizophrenia, and adult mental disorder: Is risk confined to the smallest babies? Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(9), 923930. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Al-Haddad, B. J. S., Jacobsson, B., Chabra, S., Modzelewska, D., Olson, E. M., Bernier, R., … Sengpiel, V. (2019). Long-term risk of neuropsychiatric disease after exposure to infection in Utero. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(6), 594602. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechter, K., Reiber, H., Herzog, S., Fuchs, D., Tumani, H., & Maxeiner, H. G. (2010). Cerebrospinal fluid analysis in affective and schizophrenic spectrum disorders: Identification of subgroups with immune responses and blood–CSF barrier dysfunction. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 44(5), 321330. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.08.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benros, M. E., Eaton, W. W., & Mortensen, P. B. (2014). The epidemiologic evidence linking autoimmune diseases and psychosis. Biological Psychiatry, 75(4), 300306. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benros, M. E., Mortensen, P. B., & Eaton, W. W. (2012). Autoimmune diseases and infections as risk factors for schizophrenia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1262(1), 5666. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06638.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benros, M. E., Nielsen, P. R., Nordentoft, M., Eaton, W. W., Dalton, S. O., & Mortensen, P. B. (2011). Autoimmune diseases and severe infections as risk factors for schizophrenia: A 30-year population-based register study. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(12), 13031310. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11030516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benros, M. E., Waltoft, B. L., Nordentoft, M., Østergaard, S. D., Eaton, W. W., Krogh, J., & Mortensen, P. B. (2013). Autoimmune diseases and severe infections as risk factors for mood disorders: A nationwide study. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(8), 812820. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.1111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boulanger, L. M., & Shatz, C. J. (2004). Immune signalling in neural development, synaptic plasticity and disease. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(7), 521531. doi: 10.1038/nrn1428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyd, A., Golding, J., Macleod, J., Lawlor, D. A., Fraser, A., Henderson, J., … Davey Smith, G. (2013). Cohort profile: The ‘children of the 90s’—the index offspring of the Avon longitudinal study of parents and children. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42(1), 111127. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys064.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, A. S., Begg, M. D., Gravenstein, S., Schaefer, C. A., Wyatt, R. J., Bresnahan, M., … Susser, E. S. (2004). Serologic evidence of prenatal influenza in the etiology of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(8), 774780. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.8.774.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, A. S., Cohen, P., Harkavy-Friedman, J., Babulas, V., Malaspina, D., Gorman, J. M., & Susser, E. S. (2001). A.E. Bennett research award. Prenatal rubella, premorbid abnormalities, and adult schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 49(6), 473486. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01068-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, A. S., Deicken, R. F., Vinogradov, S., Kremen, W. S., Poole, J. H., Penner, J. D., … Schaefer, C. A. (2009). Prenatal infection and cavum septum pellucidum in adult schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 108(1–3), 285287. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.11.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, A. S., & Derkits, E. J. (2010). Prenatal infection and schizophrenia: A review of epidemiologic and translational studies. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(3), 261280. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09030361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, A. S., Hooton, J., Schaefer, C. A., Zhang, H., Petkova, E., Babulas, V., … Susser, E. S. (2004). Elevated maternal interleukin-8 levels and risk of schizophrenia in adult offspring. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(5), 889895. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.5.889.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, A. S., Schaefer, C. A., Quesenberry, C. P., Liu, L., Babulas, V. P., & Susser, E. S. (2005). Maternal exposure to toxoplasmosis and risk of schizophrenia in adult offspring. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(4), 767773. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.4.767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, A. S., Schaefer, C. A., Wyatt, R. J., Goetz, R., Begg, M. D., Gorman, J. M., & Susser, E. S. (2000). Maternal exposure to respiratory infections and adult schizophrenia spectrum disorders: A prospective birth cohort study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26(2), 287295. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, A. S., Vinogradov, S., Kremen, W. S., Poole, J. H., Deicken, R. F., Penner, J. D., … Schaefer, C. A. (2009). Prenatal exposure to maternal infection and executive dysfunction in adult schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(6), 683690. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08010089.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buka, S. L., Cannon, T. D., Torrey, E. F., & Yolken, R. H., & Collaborative Study Group on the Perinatal Origins of Severe Psychiatric Disorders. (2008). Maternal exposure to herpes simplex virus and risk of psychosis among adult offspring. Biological Psychiatry, 63(8), 809815. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.09.022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buka, S. L., Tsuang, M. T., Torrey, E. F., Klebanoff, M. A., Bernstein, D., & Yolken, R. H. (2001). Maternal infections and subsequent psychosis among offspring. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58(11), 10321037. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.11.1032.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buka, S. L., Tsuang, M. T., Torrey, E. F., Klebanoff, M. A., Wagner, R. L., & Yolken, R. H. (2001). Maternal cytokine levels during pregnancy and adult psychosis. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 15(4), 411420. doi: 10.1006/brbi.2001.0644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cannon, M., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Harrington, H., Taylor, A., Murray, R. M., & Poulton, R. (2002). Evidence for early-childhood, pan-developmental impairment specific to schizophreniform disorder: Results from a longitudinal birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(5), 449456. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.5.449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Copeland, W. E., Shanahan, L., Costello, E. J., & Angold, A. (2009). Childhood and adolescent psychiatric disorders as predictors of young adult disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(7), 764772. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cordeiro, C. N., Tsimis, M., & Burd, I. (2015). Infections and brain development. Obstetrical & Gynecological Survey, 70(10), 644655. doi: 10.1097/OGX.0000000000000236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalman, C., Allebeck, P., Gunnell, D., Harrison, G., Kristensson, K., Lewis, G., … Karlsson, H. (2008). Infections in the CNS during childhood and the risk of subsequent psychotic illness: A cohort study of more than one million Swedish subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(1), 5965. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07050740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dantzer, R. (2009). Cytokine, sickness behavior, and depression. Immunology and Allergy Clinics of North America, 29(2), 247264. doi: 10.1016/j.iac.2009.02.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dantzer, R., O'Connor, J. C., Freund, G. G., Johnson, R. W., & Kelley, K. W. (2008). From inflammation to sickness and depression: When the immune system subjugates the brain. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 9(1), 4656. doi: 10.1038/nrn2297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Loore, E., Gunther, N., Drukker, M., Feron, F., Sabbe, B., Deboutte, D., … Myin-Germeys, I. (2011). Persistence and outcome of auditory hallucinations in adolescence: A longitudinal general population study of 1800 individuals. Schizophrenia Research, 127(1), 252256. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.01.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Picker, L. J., Morrens, M., Chance, S. A., & Boche, D. (2017). Microglia and brain plasticity in acute psychosis and schizophrenia illness course: A meta-review. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8, 238. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, V., & Goodyer, I. M. (2006). Longitudinal investigation into childhood-and adolescence-onset depression: Psychiatric outcome in early adulthood. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 188(3), 216222. doi: 10.1192/bjp.188.3.216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dupuis, M., Strippoli, M.-P. F., Gholam-Rezaee, M., Preisig, M., & Vandeleur, C. L. (2019). Mental disorders, attrition at follow-up, and questionnaire non-completion in epidemiologic research. Illustrations from the CoLaus|PsyCoLaus study. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 28(4), e1805. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1805.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, N. I., Berkman, E. T., Inagaki, T. K., Rameson, L. T., Mashal, N. M., & Irwin, M. R. (2010). Inflammation-induced anhedonia: Endotoxin reduces ventral striatum responses to reward. Biological Psychiatry, 68(8), 748754. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.06.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, H. L., Caspi, A., Poulton, R., Meier, M. H., Houts, R., Harrington, H., … Moffitt, T. E. (2013). Specificity of childhood psychotic symptoms for predicting schizophrenia by 38 years of age: A birth cohort study. Psychological Medicine, 43(10), 20772086. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712003091.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flinkkilä, E., Keski-Rahkonen, A., Marttunen, M., & Raevuori, A. (2016). Prenatal inflammation, infections and mental disorders. Psychopathology, 49(5), 317333. doi: 10.1159/000448054.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fombonne, E., Wostear, G., Cooper, V., Harrington, R., & Rutter, M. (2001). The Maudsley long-term follow-up of child and adolescent depression: I. Psychiatric outcomes in adulthood. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 179(3), 210217. doi: 10.1192/bjp.179.3.210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraser, A., Macdonald-Wallis, C., Tilling, K., Boyd, A., Golding, J., Davey Smith, G., … Lawlor, D. A. (2013). Cohort profile: The Avon longitudinal study of parents and children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42(1), 97110. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys066.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, R. D. (2011). Association between infection early in life and mental disorders among youth in the community: A cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health, 11, 878. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, N. A., Brydon, L., Walker, C., Gray, M. A., Steptoe, A., & Critchley, H. D. (2009). Inflammation causes mood changes through alterations in subgenual cingulate activity and mesolimbic connectivity. Biological Psychiatry, 66(5), 407414. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6(2), 6570. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/4615733.Google Scholar
Horwood, J., Salvi, G., Thomas, K., Duffy, L., Gunnell, D., Hollis, C., … Harrison, G. (2008). IQ And non-clinical psychotic symptoms in 12-year-olds: Results from the ALSPAC birth cohort. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 193(3), 185191. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.051904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howes, O. D., Shotbolt, P., Bloomfield, M., Daalman, K., Demjaha, A., Diederen, K. M. J., … Sommer, I. E. (2013). Dopaminergic function in the psychosis Spectrum: An [18F]-DOPA imaging study in healthy individuals with auditory hallucinations. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39(4), 807814. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jamshidian, M., Jalal, S., & Jansen, C. (2014). Missmech: An R package for testing homoscedasticity, multivariate normality, and missing completely at random (MCAR). Journal of Statistical Software, 56(1), 131. doi: 10.18637/jss.v056.i06.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelleher, I., & Cannon, M. (2011). Psychotic-like experiences in the general population: Characterizing a high-risk group for psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 41(1), 16. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelleher, I., Keeley, H., Corcoran, P., Lynch, F., Fitzpatrick, C., Devlin, N., … Cannon, M. (2012). Clinicopathological significance of psychotic experiences in non-psychotic young people: Evidence from four population-based studies. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 201(1), 2632. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.101543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khandaker, G. M., Dalman, C., Kappelmann, N., Stochl, J., Dal, H., Kosidou, K., … Karlsson, H. (2018). Association of childhood infection with IQ and adult nonaffective psychosis in Swedish Men: A population-based longitudinal cohort and co-relative study. JAMA Psychiatry, 75(4), 356. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.4491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khandaker, G. M., Dantzer, R., & Jones, P. B. (2017). Immunopsychiatry: Important facts. Psychological Medicine, 47(13), 22292237. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717000745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khandaker, G. M., Stochl, J., Zammit, S., Lewis, G., & Jones, P. B. (2014). Childhood Epstein-Barr virus infection and subsequent risk of psychotic experiences in adolescence: A population-based prospective serological study. Schizophrenia Research, 158(0), 1924. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.05.019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khandaker, G. M., Zammit, S., Lewis, G., & Jones, P. B. (2014). A population-based study of atopic disorders and inflammatory markers in childhood before psychotic experiences in adolescence. Schizophrenia Research, 152(1), 139145. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.09.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khandaker, G. M., Zimbron, J., Dalman, C., Lewis, G., & Jones, P. B. (2012). Childhood infection and adult schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of population-based studies. Schizophrenia Research, 139(1–3), 161168. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.05.023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khandaker, G. M., Zimbron, J., Lewis, G., & Jones, P. B. (2013). Prenatal maternal infection, neurodevelopment and adult schizophrenia: A systematic review of population-based studies. Psychological Medicine, 43(2), 239257. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712000736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laske, C., Zank, M., Klein, R., Stransky, E., Batra, A., Buchkremer, G., & Schott, K. (2008). Autoantibody reactivity in serum of patients with major depression, schizophrenia and healthy controls. Psychiatry Research, 158(1), 8386. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.04.023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewinsohn, P. M., Rohde, P., Seeley, J. R., Klein, D. N., & Gotlib, I. H. (2000). Natural course of adolescent major depressive disorder in a community sample: Predictors of recurrence in young adults. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(10), 15841591. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackinnon, N., Zammit, S., Lewis, G., Jones, P. B., & Khandaker, G. M. (2018). Association between childhood infection, serum inflammatory markers and intelligence: Findings from a population-based prospective birth cohort study. Epidemiology and Infection, 146(2), 256264. doi: 10.1017/S0950268817002710.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, U., Schwarz, M. J., & Müller, N. (2011). Inflammatory processes in schizophrenia: A promising neuroimmunological target for the treatment of negative/cognitive symptoms and beyond. Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 132(1), 96110. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2011.06.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, A. H., Maletic, V., & Raison, C. L. (2009). Inflammation and its discontents: The role of cytokines in the pathophysiology of major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 65(9), 732741. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.11.029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mortensen, P. B., Nørgaard-Pedersen, B., Waltoft, B. L., Sørensen, T. L., Hougaard, D., Torrey, E. F., & Yolken, R. H. (2007). Toxoplasma gondii as a risk factor for early-onset schizophrenia: Analysis of filter paper blood samples obtained at birth. Biological Psychiatry, 61(5), 688693. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mortensen, P. B., Pedersen, C. B., Hougaard, D. M., Nørgaard-Petersen, B., Mors, O., Børglum, A. D., & Yolken, R. H. (2010). A Danish National Birth Cohort study of maternal HSV-2 antibodies as a risk factor for schizophrenia in their offspring. Schizophrenia Research, 122(1–3), 257263. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.06.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, S. K., Fineberg, A. M., Maxwell, S. D., Alloy, L. B., Zimmermann, L., Krigbaum, N. Y., … Ellman, L. M. (2017). Maternal infection and stress during pregnancy and depressive symptoms in adolescent offspring. Psychiatry Research, 257, 102110. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.07.025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, G. K., & Jones, P. B. (2012). Psychotic symptoms in young people without psychotic illness: Mechanisms and meaning. British Journal of Psychiatry, 201(1), 46. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.107789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, R. M., & Lewis, S. W. (1987). Is schizophrenia a neurodevelopmental disorder? British Medical Journal (Clinical Research Ed.), 295(6600), 681682. doi: 10.1136/bmj.295.6600.681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, P. R., Benros, M. E., & Mortensen, P. B. (2014). Hospital contacts with infection and risk of schizophrenia: A population-based cohort study with linkage of Danish national registers. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40(6), 15261532. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, P. R., Laursen, T. M., & Mortensen, P. B. (2013). Association between parental hospital-treated infection and the risk of schizophrenia in adolescence and early adulthood. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39(1), 230237. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Office for National Statistics (2016). The national statistics socio-economic classification (NS-SEC). Office for National Statistics. Retrieved from https://www.ons.gov.uk/methodology/classificationsandstandards/otherclassifications/thenationalstatisticssocioeconomicclassificationnssecrebasedonsoc2010.Google Scholar
Polanczyk, G., Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Cannon, M., Ambler, A., Keefe, R. S. E., … Caspi, A. (2010). Etiological and clinical features of childhood psychotic symptoms: Results from a birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(4), 328338. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poulton, R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Cannon, M., Murray, R., & Harrington, H. (2000). Children's self-reported psychotic symptoms and adult schizophreniform disorder: A 15-year longitudinal study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(11), 10531058. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.11.1053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubio, J. M., Sanjuán, J., Flórez-Salamanca, L., & Cuesta, M. J. (2012). Examining the course of hallucinatory experiences in children and adolescents: A systematic review. Schizophrenia Research, 138(2), 248254. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.03.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharp, C., Goodyer, I. M., & Croudace, T. J. (2006). The short mood and feelings questionnaire (SMFQ): A unidimensional item response theory and categorical data factor analysis of self-report ratings from a community sample of 7-through 11-year-Old children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34(3), 365377. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9027-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, K., Harrison, G., Zammit, S., Lewis, G., Horwood, J., Heron, J., … Gunnell, D. (2009). Association of measures of fetal and childhood growth with non-clinical psychotic symptoms in 12-year-olds: The ALSPAC cohort. British Journal of Psychiatry, 194(6), 521526. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.051730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uher, R. (2014). Gene–environment interactions in severe mental illness. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5, 48. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Buuren, S., & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. (2011). mice: Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 45(3), 167. Retrieved from https://ideas.repec.org/a/jss/jstsof/v045i03.html.Google Scholar
van Os, J., Linscott, R., Myin-Germeys, I., Delespaul, P., & Krabbendam, L. (2009). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: Evidence for a psychosis proneness-persistence-impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychological Medicine, 39(2), 179195. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinberger, D. R. (1987). Implications of normal brain development for the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44(7), 660669. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800190080012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, I. R., Royston, P., & Wood, A. M. (2011). Multiple imputation using chained equations: Issues and guidance for practice. Statistics in Medicine, 30(4), 377399. doi: 10.1002/sim.4067.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organisation (1994). SCAN: Schedules for clinical assessment in neuropsychiatry, version 2.0. Geneva, Switzerland: Psychiatric Publishers International/American Psychiatric Press Inc. Retrieved from https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/40356.Google Scholar
Zammit, S., Allebeck, P., David, A. S., Dalman, C., Hemmingsson, T., Lundberg, I., & Lewis, G. (2004). A longitudinal study of premorbid IQ score and risk of developing schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, severe depression, and other nonaffective psychoses. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(4), 354360. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.4.354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zammit, S., Kounali, D., Cannon, M., David, A. S., Gunnell, D., Heron, J., … Lewis, G. (2013). Psychotic experiences and psychotic disorders at age 18 in relation to psychotic experiences at age 12 in a Longitudinal Population-based Cohort Study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(7), 742750. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12060768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zammit, S., Odd, D., Horwood, J., Thompson, A., Thomas, K., Menezes, P., … Harrison, G. (2009). Investigating whether adverse prenatal and perinatal events are associated with non-clinical psychotic symptoms at age 12 years in the ALSPAC birth cohort. Psychological Medicine, 39(9), 14571467. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708005126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Characteristics of participants in the risk set (using maximum available sample and after imputation of confounders)

Figure 1

Table 2. Beta estimate (s.e.) for the association between 1 s.d. increase in childhood infections and depressive symptoms from age 10 to 19 years

Figure 2

Table 3. Odds ratio (95% CI) for the association between 1 s.d. increase in childhood infections and psychotic experiences (PE) at age 12 and age 18 years

Figure 3

Table 4. Beta estimate (s.e.) for the association between childhood infection burden and depressive symptoms from age 10 to 19 years

Figure 4

Table 5. Odds ratio (95% CI) for the association between childhood infection burden and psychotic experiences (PEs) at age 12 and age 18 years

Supplementary material: File

Chaplin et al. supplementary material

Chaplin et al. supplementary material

Download Chaplin et al. supplementary material(File)
File 285 KB