Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T06:26:30.024Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Addressing perceptual insensitivity to facial affect in violent offenders: first evidence for the efficacy of a novel implicit training approach

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2013

M. Schönenberg*
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen, Germany
S. Christian
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen, Germany
A.-K. Gaußer
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen, Germany
S. V. Mayer
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen, Germany
M. Hautzinger
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen, Germany
A. Jusyte
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen, Germany
*
*Address for correspondence: Michael Schönenberg, Ph.D., University of Tübingen, Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Schleichstraße 4, D-72076 Tübingen, Germany. (Email: [email protected])

Abstract

Background

Although impaired recognition of affective facial expressions has been conclusively linked to antisocial behavior, little is known about the modifiability of this deficit. This study investigated whether and under which circumstances the proposed perceptual insensitivity can be addressed with a brief implicit training approach.

Method

Facial affect recognition was assessed with an animated morph task, in which the participants (44 male incarcerated violent offenders and 43 matched controls) identified the onset of emotional expressions in animated morph clips that gradually changed from neutral to one of the six basic emotions. Half of the offenders were then implicitly trained to direct attention to salient face regions (attention training, AT) using a modified dot-probe task. The other half underwent the same protocol but the intensity level of the presented expressions was additionally manipulated over the course of training sessions (sensitivity to emotional expressions training, SEE training). Subsequently, participants were reassessed with the animated morph task.

Results

Facial affect recognition was significantly impaired in violent offenders as compared with controls. Further, our results indicate that only the SEE training group exhibited a pronounced improvement in emotion recognition.

Conclusions

We demonstrated for the first time that perceptual insensitivity to facial affect can be addressed by an implicit training that directs attention to salient regions of a face and gradually decreases the intensity of the emotional expression. Future studies should focus on the potential of this intervention to effectively increase empathy and inhibit violent behavior in antisocial individuals.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adolphs, R (2010). What does the amygdala contribute to social cognition? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1191, 4261.Google Scholar
Adolphs, R, Gosselin, F, Buchanan, TW, Tranel, D, Schyns, P, Damasio, AR (2005). A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage. Nature 433, 6872.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bar-Haim, Y (2010). Research review: attention bias modification (ABM): a novel treatment for anxiety disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 51, 859870.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R, Mitchell, D, Peschardt, K, Colledge, E, Leonard, R, Shine, J, Murray, L, Perrett, D (2004). Reduced sensitivity to others’ fearful expressions in psychopathic individuals. Personality and Individual Differences 37, 11111122.Google Scholar
Blair, RJR (2001). Neurocognitive models of aggression, the antisocial personality disorders, and psychopathy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 71, 727731.Google Scholar
Buss, AH, Perry, M (1992). The Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 63, 452459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dadds, MR, Cauchi, AJ, Wimalaweera, S, Hawes, DJ, Brennan, J (2012). Outcomes, moderators, and mediators of empathic-emotion recognition training for complex conduct problems in childhood. Psychiatry Research 199, 201207.Google Scholar
Dadds, M, El Masry, Y, Wimalaweera, S, Guastella, A (2008). Reduced eye gaze explains ‘fear blindness’ in childhood psychopathic traits. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 47, 455463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dadds, M, Perry, Y, Hawes, D, Merz, S, Riddell, A, Haines, D, Solak, E, Abeygunawardane, A (2006). Look at the eyes: fear recognition in child psychopathy. British Journal of Psychiatry 189, 180181.Google Scholar
Dawel, A, O'Kearney, R, McKone, E, Palermo, R (2012). Not just fear and sadness: meta-analytic evidence of pervasive emotion recognition deficits for facial and vocal expressions in psychopathy. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36, 22882304.Google Scholar
Dolan, M, Fullam, R (2006). Face affect recognition deficits in personality-disordered offenders: association with psychopathy. Psychological Medicine 36, 15631569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenbarth, H, Alpers, GW (2008). Psychopathic Personality Inventory–Revised, German Version (PPI-R). Hogrefe: Göttingen.Google Scholar
Eisenbarth, H, Alpers, GW, Segrè, D, Calogero, A, Angrilli, A (2008). Categorization and evaluation of emotional faces in psychopathic women. Psychiatry Research 159, 189195.Google Scholar
Fairchild, G, Stobbe, Y, Van Goozen, SHM, Calder, AJ, Goodyer, IM (2010). Facial expression recognition, fear conditioning, and startle modulation in female subjects with conduct disorder. Biological Psychiatry 68, 272279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fairchild, G, Van Goozen, SHM, Calder, AJ, Stollery, SJ, Goodyer, IM (2009). Deficits in facial expression recognition in male adolecents with early-onset or adolescence-onset conduct disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 50, 627636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, RD (2003). The Psychopathy Checklist – Revised, 2nd edn. Multi-Health Systems: Toronto.Google Scholar
Harms, MB, Martin, A, Wallace, GL (2010). Facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychology Review 20, 290322.Google Scholar
Hastings, ME, Tangney, JP, Stuewig, J (2008). Psychopathy and identification of facial expressions of emotion. Personality and Individual Differences 44, 14741483.Google Scholar
Herzberg, PY (2003). Faktorstruktur Gütekriterien und Konstruktvalidität der deutschen Übersetzung des Aggressionsfragebogens von Buss und Perry [Psychometric evaluation and validity of the German translation of the Aggression Questionnaire by Buss and Perry]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie 24, 311323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, A, Laurens, K, Herba, C, Barker, G, Viding, E (2009). Amygdala hypoactivity to fearful faces in boys with conduct problems and callous–unemotional traits. American Journal of Psychiatry 166, 95102.Google Scholar
Joormann, J, Gotlib, IH (2006). Is this happiness I see? Biases in the identification of emotional facial expressions in depression and social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 115, 705713.Google Scholar
Kohler, CG, Walker, JB, Martin, EA, Healey, KM, Moberg, PJ (2010). Facial emotion perception in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Schizophrenia Bulletin 36, 10091019.Google Scholar
Kosson, DS, Suchy, Y, Mayer, AR, Libby, J (2002). Facial affect recognition in criminal psychopaths. Emotion 2, 398411.Google Scholar
Langner, O, Dotsch, R, Bijlstra, G, Wigboldus, DHJ, Hawk, ST, Van Knippenberg, A (2010). Presentation and validation of the Radboud Faces Database. Cognition and Emotion 24, 13771388.Google Scholar
Lilienfeld, SO, Widows, MR (2005). Psychopathic Personality Inventory–Revised (PPI-R): Professional Manual. Psychological Assessment Resources: Lutz, FL.Google Scholar
MacLeod, C, Mathews, A, Tata, P (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 95, 1520.Google Scholar
Marsh, AA, Blair, R (2008). Deficits in facial affect recognition among antisocial populations: a meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32, 454465.Google Scholar
Marsh, PJ, Green, MJ, Russell, TA, McGuire, J, Harris, A, Coltheart, M (2010). Remediation of facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia: functional predictors, generalizability, and durability. American Journal of Psychiatric Rehabilitation 13, 143170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, PJ, Luckett, G, Russell, T, Coltheart, M, Green, MJ (2012). Effects of facial emotion recognition remediation on visual scanning of novel face stimuli. Schizophrenia Research 141, 234240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montagne, B, van Honk, J, Kessels, RP, Frigerio, E, Burt, M, van Zandvoort, MJ, Perrett, DI, de Haan, EH (2005). Reduced efficiency in recognising fear in subjects scoring high on psychopathic personality characteristics. Personality and Individual Differences 38, 511.Google Scholar
Penton-Voak, IS, Thomas, J, Gagel, SH, McMurran, M, McDonald, S, Munafò, MR (2013). Increasing recognition of happiness in ambiguous facial expressions reduces anger and aggressive behavior. Psychological Science 24, 688697.Google Scholar
Pham, TH, Philippot, P (2010). Decoding of facial expression of emotion in criminal psychopaths. Journal of Personality Disorders 24, 445459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raine, A, Lee, L, Yang, Y, Colletti, P (2010). Neurodevelopmental marker for limbic maldevelopment in antisocial personality disorder and psychopathy. British Journal of Psychiatry 197, 186192.Google Scholar
Russell, TA, Chu, E, Phillips, ML (2006). A pilot study to investigate the effectiveness of emotion recognition remediation in schizophrenia using the micro-expression training tool. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 45, 579583.Google Scholar
Sato, W, Uono, S, Matsuura, N, Toichi, M (2009). Misrecognition of facial expressions in delinquents. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health 3, 2734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schönenberg, M, Louis, K, Mayer, S, Jusyte, A (2013). Impaired identification of threat-related social information in male delinquents with antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders. Published online 15 04 2013 . doi:10.1521/pedi_2013_27_100.Google Scholar
Sebastian, CL, McCrory, EJ, Cecil, CA, Lockwood, PL, De Brito, SA, Fontaine, NM, Viding, E (2012). Neural responses to affective and cognitive theory of mind in children with conduct problems and varying levels of callous–unemotional traits. Archives of General Psychiatry 69, 814822.Google Scholar
Staugaard, SR (2010). Threatening faces and social anxiety: a literature review. Clinical Psychology Review 30, 669690.Google Scholar
Young, AW, Rowland, D, Calder, AJ, Etcoff, NL (1997). Facial expression megamix: tests of dimensional and category accounts of emotion recognition. Cognition 63, 271313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Schönenberg et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Movie

Download Schönenberg et al. supplementary material(Video)
Video 25.6 MB
Supplementary material: File

Schönenberg supplementary material

Supplementary table

Download Schönenberg supplementary material(File)
File 49.7 KB