Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T04:28:47.820Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Visual flicker in depression: response criteria, confidence ratings and response times

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

Joseph E. Herskovic*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychophysiology, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Department of Psychology, Queen's College of the City University of New York
Mitchell L. Kietzman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychophysiology, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Department of Psychology, Queen's College of the City University of New York
Samuel Sutton
Affiliation:
Department of Psychophysiology, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Department of Psychology, Queen's College of the City University of New York
*
1Address for correspondence: Dr Joseph E. Herskovic, Psychopharmacology Unit, Research Trailer No. 350, VA Medical Center, Brentwood (691/B151D), West Los Angeles, CA 90073, USA.

Synopsis

Differences in response criterion and sensory sensitivity to visual flicker among major depressive patients, dysthymic patients, and normal controls were investigated. Also, signal detection confidence ratings and response times were compared. The results indicated that major depressive patients responded more conservatively (i.e. were less willing to respond ‘flicker’) than either of the other groups. The groups did not differ significantly on a criterion free measure of flicker sensitivity. The major conclusions are: (1) previously reported visual flicker differences between depressed patients and normal controls were probably due to the more conservative response criterion of the patients and not to flicker sensitivity differences between groups; and (2) confidence ratings and response times yield similar conclusions with respect to visual flicker sensitivity and response criterion. Therefore, interpretations concerning a sensory or perceptual deficit in depression must take into account the differences in response criterion between depressed patients and normal controls.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1986

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association (1980). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd edn). APA: Washington, D.C.Google Scholar
Black, S., Franklin, L. M., deSilva, F. P. R. & Wijewickrama, H. S. D. S. (1975). The flicker-fusion threshold in schizophrenia and depression. New Zealand Medical Journal 81, 244246.Google ScholarPubMed
Bruder, G. E., Sutton, S., Babkoff, H., Gurland, B. J., Yozawitz, A. & Fleiss, J. L. (1975). Auditory signal detectability and facilitation of simple reaction time in psychiatric patients and non-patients. Psychological Medicine 5, 260272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, D. G. (1977). Affect and vigilance performance in depressive illness. Journal of Psychiatric Research 13, 185191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, R. A. (1963). Detection of a noise signal of varying duration. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 35, 17321737.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, W. C. (1966). The psyche in psychophysics: a sensory-decision analysis of the effect of instructions on flicker sensitivity and response bias. Psychological Bulletin 65, 358366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, W. C., Rutschmann, J., Link, R. & Brown, J. C. (1963). Comparison of flicker-fusion thresholds obtained by the methods of forced-choice and limits on psychiatric patients. Perceptual and Motor Skills 16, 1930.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, W. C., Brown, J. C. & Rutschmann, J. (1967). Flicker sensitivity and response bias in psychiatric patients and normal subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 72, 3542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, G. C., Buchsbaum, M. S. & Bunney, W. E. (1979). Analgesia to painful stimuli in affective illness. American Journal of Psychiatry 136, 11481151.Google ScholarPubMed
Dunbar, G. C. & Lishman, W. A. (1984). Depression, recognitionmemory and hedonic tone. A signal detection analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry 144, 376382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emmerich, D. S., Gray, J. L., Watson, C. S. & Tanis, D. C. (1972). Response latency, confidence, and ROCas in auditory signal detection. Perception and Psychophysics 11, 6572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Endicott, J. & Spitzer, R. L (1978). A diagnostic interview – the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 35, 837844.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmberg, G. (1981). Critical flicker fusion (CFF) test for sedative effect of antidepressants. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 63 (Suppl. 290), 289301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, F. N. (1975). Depression: some proposals for future research. Diseases of the Nervous System 36, 228232.Google ScholarPubMed
Karp, E. & Pollack, M. (1963). Comparative studies of chlorpromazine and imipramine. III. Critical flicker fusion thresholds in psychiatric patients. Psychopharmacologia 4, 452458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katz, L. (1970). A comparison of type II operating characteristics derived from confidence ratings and from latencies. Perception and Psychophysics 8, 6568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerkof, G. A., van der Schaaaf, T. W. & Korving, H. J. (1980). Auditory signal detection: effects of long-term practice and time on task. Perception and Psychophysics 28, 7981.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kietzman, M. L., Spring, B. & Zubin, J. (1980). Perception, cognition, and attention. In Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry, Vol. 3 (ed. Kaplan, H. I., Freedman, A. M. and Sadock, B. J.), pp. 334370. Williams & Wilkins: Baltimore.Google Scholar
Levitt, H. & Treisman, M. (1969). Control charts for sequential testing. Psychometrika 34, 509518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNicol, D. (1972). A Primer of Signal Detection Theory. George Allen & Unwin: London.Google Scholar
Miller, E. & Lewis, P. (1977). Recognition memory in elderly patients with depression and dementia: a signal detection analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 86, 8486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, W. R. (1975). Psychological deficit in depression. Psychological Bulletin 82, 238260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moss, S. M., Myers, J. L. & Filmore, T. (1970). Short-term recognition memory of tones. Perception and Psychophysics 7, 369373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norman, D. A. & Wickelgren, W. A. (1969). Strength theory of decision rules and latency in retrieval from short-term memory. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 6, 192208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nunn, C. M. H. (1980). A model of the functional psychoses. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 62, 7584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perris, C. (1966). A study of bipolar (manic-depressive) and unipolar recurrent depressive psychoses. VII. Studies in perception: (b) flicker and fusion threshold. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 42 (Suppl), 102117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teuting, P. (1981). Special Report on Depression Research. DHHS Publication No. ADM 81–1085. US Government Printing Office: Washington, D.C.Google Scholar
Weintraub, D. J. & Fidell, L. S. (1979). A signal-detection approach to subception: concomitant verbal and finger-latency responses in metacontrast. Perception and Psychophysics 26, 143152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeally, A. K. & Aitken, R. C. B. (1969). Measurement of mood. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine 62, 993996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar